13 results on '"Quadt, Lisa"'
Search Results
2. 'I’m trying to reach out, I’m trying to find my people' – A mixed methods investigation of loneliness and loneliness distress in autistic adults
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Relationship between PTSD, Camouflaging, and Chronic Pain/Fatigue in Autism
- Author
-
Aycan, Pinar and Quadt, Lisa
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Clinical Psychology ,mental disorders ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
There is a link between neurodevelopmental, physical and mental health conditions. Autism, which is a neurodevelopmental condition, will be the focus of this study. To be more specific, the relationship between trauma, camouflaging behavior, and chronic pain/fatigue conditions in Autism will be investigated. First, the literature shows that diagnosis of Autism in females is more difficult compared to males (Bargiela et al., 2016). Camouflaging behavior is one of the potential reasons for why less females get diagnosed with Autism. The detection of Autism in females becomes very difficult since they are more likely to engage in camouflaging behavior and to become experts in camouflaging (Baldwin and Costley, 2015). Therefore, it has been suggested that camouflaging behavior to appear more neurotypical should be included in the definition of the female autism phenotype (Holliday-Willey, 2014). Second, autistic people are more likely to be diagnosed with Post-traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and complex PTSD. Autistic individuals, especially females, are more vulnerable to experiencing traumatic life events such as negative social events, and therefore have an increased risk for PTSD (Haruvi et al., 2020). In fact, autistic women are at risk for victimization by their partners including sexual abuse (Bigelow, 2011). Autistic people state that they use camouflaging strategies to achieve acceptance in personal and professional relationships, and to avoid possible traumatic events and harm (Hull et al., 2019). Therefore, these findings might suggest a relationship between PTSD and camouflaging. Lastly, chronic pain has been shown to be overrepresented in autistic people (Csecs et al., 2022), and those with PTSD (insert a reference which shows this). Chronic fatigue is more common in people diagnosed with PTSD (reference), and may be overrepresented in autistic individuals, although research is lacking. Chronic pain can be defined as a difficult physical condition where pain persists for more than three months (Blyth et al.,2001), while chronic fatigue can be defined as an extreme tiredness that lasts for at least six months (Yancey & Thomas, 2012). A large proportion of women who were diagnosed as autistic stated that they have chronic pain (Asztély et al., 2019). Likewise, PTSD is associated with chronic pain and fatigue (Roy-Byrne et al., 2004). Additionally, camouflaging might result in physical problems such as increased exhaustion and pain (Bradley et al., 2021). Based on these findings in the literature, the purpose of this study is to get a better understanding of the relationship between (PTSD), camouflaging behavior, and chronic pain/fatigue in Autism. To achieve this, a secondary data analysis using the relevant data from an already existing data set will be conducted.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Exploring the relationship between autistic traits and rejection sensitivity in complex chronic conditions
- Author
-
Culyer, Ella and Quadt, Lisa
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
The present study will be analysing data collected in a previous study. This data includes measures from a patient group with complex chronic conditions (CCCs), - Chronic pain (e.g., fibromyalgia) - Chronic fatigue (e.g., myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome (ME/CFS)) - Complex personality presentations (i.e., borderline/emotionally unstable personality disorder (BPD/EUPD), complex post-traumatic stress disorder (C-PTSD)) The present study will investigate a potential moderation relationship between autistic traits and increased rejection sensitivity (RS) in people with CCCs, using questionnaire measures of autistic traits (Ritvo Asperger and Autism Diagnostic Scale (RAADS; Ritvo et al., 2011); Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q; Hull et al., 2019), and rejection sensitivity (Rejection Sensitivity Adult Questionnaire (A-RSQ; Berenson et al., 2009).
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Neurdivergence in Complex Chronic Conditions
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Clinical Psychology ,Mental and Social Health ,Medicine and Health Sciences ,Psychology ,Diseases ,Psychiatric and Mental Health ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
Neurodivergence describes a set of neurodevelopmental conditions, such as Autism Spectrum Disorder (henceforth "Autism", Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, and Tourette's Syndrome. Neurodivergent people may think and perceive the world differently, interact with other people differently, and experience emotions in a unique way. We also know that being neurodivergent is linked with dramatically increased risks for mental and physical health conditions, where it is reported that up to 84% of neurodivergent adults suffer from poor health. Previous research from our group suggests that some patient populations, namely those with specific complex chronic conditions (CCCs) have especially high rates of "hidden", that is, undiagnosed neurodiversity. These complex chronic conditions include both physical and mental health conditions, specifically chronic pain and fatigue, and complex personality presentations (i.e., Borderline/Emotionally Intense/Emotionally Unstable Personality Disorder, Complex Posttraumatic stress disorder [C-PTSD]). This project aims to find out how many people with these complex chronic conditions meet criteria for autism or ADHD, and have associated traits. It is important to detect neurodivergence, and to adjust treatment and care options accordingly. Identifying the prevalence of neurodivergent traits in patients with CCCs is the first step in assessing commonalities in these populations, and ultimately finding targeted treatment opportunities. To do so, we recruited individuals with CCCs (diagnosis of chronic pain/fatigue and/or complex personality presentation), and individuals without CCCs. Participants were asked to fill out a survey that includes diagnostic screening questionnaires for autism and ADHD, and questionnaires assessing neurodivergent traits, such as masking/camouflaging behaviour, sensory sensitivities, rejection sensitivity, and dissociative experiences.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Development and Validation of a Modified Version of the UCLA Loneliness Scale: UCLA Loneliness, Distress, and Contentment Scale (UCLA-LDC)
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa and Bartlett, James
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Clinical Psychology ,Social Statistics ,Health Psychology ,Psychology ,Quantitative Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
Loneliness is shown to have negative consequences for physical and mental health across cultures, lifespan, and clinical populations (for a recent review, see Quadt et al., 2020). The World Health Organization has declared loneliness a priority for researchers and care providers, given its potential adverse consequences, including depression (Cacioppo et al., 2010), anxiety (Anderson & Harvey, 1988), psychotic disorders (Lim et al., 2018), cardiovascular disease (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015), chronic health conditions (Barlow et al., 2015), and immunological/inflammatory changes (Moieni et al., 2015). The surge of the Covid-19 pandemic and associated lockdown measures world-wide have added to the importance of understanding the potential consequences of feelings of loneliness. The term ‘loneliness’ can be quite elusive, being defined differently in everyday language, and also in academic research. It is often conceptualized as the actual or perceived absence of meaningful social interactions (Cacioppo et al., 2015; Weiss, 1973), although it is acknowledged that there is a qualitative difference between social isolation (number of social contacts) and feelings of loneliness (perceived lack of meaningful social contacts). Most standard self-report tools in loneliness research on adults ask how often a person feels lonely, or how many social contacts they have. Research has shown that it is the subjective experience of being distressed by loneliness that induces adverse health effects (Cacioppo et al., 2010; Matthews & Tye, 2019). It is therefore important for loneliness measures to explicitly pick up this dimension of loneliness and reliably distinguish feelings of distress associated with loneliness from chosen solitude that may not induce feelings of distress. The most commonly used loneliness measures to date touch on negative affect associated with loneliness through items such as “I have an unmet need for a close romantic relationship” (SELSA; DiTommaso & Spinner, 1993), “I miss having people around” (De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale; Gierveld & Tilburg, 2006), or “How often do you feel that your relationships with others are not meaningful?” (UCLA-LS; Russell, 1996). However, while it is assumed that this perceived lack of meaningful relationships will lead to distress, this may not always be the case. Similarly, populations believed to not be motivated for meaningful social interactions, such as autistic individuals (Chevallier et al., 2012), most likely actually feel distress at being lonely (Causton-Theoharis et al., 2009). Standard questionnaires about loneliness do not pick up these subtleties explicitly. We believe these to be of importance for detecting and distinguishing feeling states associated with loneliness. Therefore, the aim of this study is to modify the UCLA-LS, and add the question “How do you feel about this?” after each original item, with a unipolar 5 point Likert scale ranging from “Very bad” to “Very good”. These additional questions are designed to capture feelings of distress and contentment associated with loneliness (UCLA Loneliness, Distress and Contentment Scale; UCLA-LDC). In a large-scale survey, the goal is to assess the psychometric properties of a modified loneliness questionnaire, statistically test whether the added questions indeed assess the latent constructs of distress and contentment, and test whether they capture aspects of loneliness that multi-dimensional loneliness questionnaires, like the De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale, do not.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Interoception and emotion: shared mechanisms and clinical implications
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa, Critchley, Hugo D, Garfinkel, Sarah N, Tsakiris, Manos, and De Preester, Helena
- Published
- 2018
8. The neurobiology of interoception in health and disease
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa, Critchley, Hugo D, and Garfinkel, Sarah N
- Subjects
RC0321 ,RC0438 - Abstract
Interoception is the sensing of internal bodily sensations. Interoception is an umbrella term that encompasses (1) the afferent (body‐to‐brain) signaling through distinct neural and humoral (including immune and endocrine) channels; (2) the neural encoding, representation, and integration of this information concerning internal bodily state; (3) the influence of such information on other perceptions, cognitions, and behaviors; (4) and the psychological expression of these representations as consciously accessible physical sensations and feelings. Interoceptive mechanisms ensure physiological health through the cerebral coordination of homeostatic reflexes and allostatic responses that include motivational behaviors and associated affective and emotional feelings. Furthermore, the conscious, unitary sense of self in time and space may be grounded in the primacy and lifelong continuity of interoception. Body‐to‐brain interactions influence physical and mental well‐being. Consequently, we show that systematic investigation of how individual differences, and within‐individual changes, in interoceptive processing can contribute to the mechanistic understanding of physical and psychological disorders. We present a neurobiological overview of interoception and describe how interoceptive impairments at different levels relate to specific physical and mental health conditions, including sickness behaviors and fatigue, depression, eating disorders, autism, and anxiety. We frame these findings in an interoceptive predictive processing framework and highlight potential new avenues for treatments.
- Published
- 2018
9. Action-oriented predictive processing and social cognition
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa
- Subjects
100 Philosophy ,100 Philosophie - Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Levels of social embodiment - towards a unifying perspective on social cognition
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa Anna
- Subjects
100 Philosophy ,100 Philosophie - Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Action-Oriented Predictive Processing and Social Cognition
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa A.
- Abstract
The research field on social cognition currently finds itself confronted with two conflicting theoretical camps, cognitivism and enactivism. In their most extreme formulations, the former claims that mindreading skills exhaust our social cognitive capacities, while the latter stresses the sufficiency of interaction and embodiment. My aim is to find a middle position that provides the basis for discussing social cognition as interactive and embodied, while remaining in non-radical territory. This can be achieved by situating social cognition within the framework of action-oriented predictive processing (Clark 2013). Specifically, I propose three conceptual tools, namely (1) embodied social inference (EmSI), (2) action-oriented predictions (a-o predictions) (Clark 2016), and (3) interactive inference (InI). The first concept of EmSI refers to the more general term of “embodied inference” (Friston 2012), which means that organism’s morphology incorporates the demands of its environment. This idea can be applied to the social realm, in the sense that the kind of body an individual has constrains the kind of social interaction they can engage in. While humans, for example, can exploit their speech apparatus for communication, ants instead rely on their pheromone system. The body of an individual thus also constrains social cognitive skills and can be said to play a crucial role for interactions. This becomes obvious when considering the second concept of “action-oriented predictions”. The basic idea is that the job of a predictive model is to distribute the cognitive workload and recruit embodied action whenever possible. Here too the body plays an indispensable role in that it realizes prediction error minimization by engaging with the external world via active inference. Related to this idea is the last concept of ”interactive inference”. I claim that interaction plays the same role for social cognition as action does for general cognition – namely gathering information about the social environment and thus actively sculpting not only one’s external, but also internal environment. InI can be described as the minimization of prediction error while navigating the social environment. It serves to actively sample proof for predictions or to disambiguate competing models about the other. In what I call replicative interactive inference (RInI), the bodily state (e.g., posture, movements) of another person is mimicked in order to supplement exteroceptive information about them with interoceptive and proprioceptive information. Mimicry, synchronization and automatic imitation are instances of RInI that function to make predictions about the other more precise by increasing the number of signal sources that yield relevant information. Secondly, complementary interactive inference (CInI) refers to changing one’s internal or external environment in response to the other person. It serves to either regulate the other’s current state (e.g., mothers lowering their body temperature to cool down their infant’s feverish body; Nyqvist et al. 2010), or to evoke further behavioral response that then serves as additional exteroceptive input (e.g., using gestures to express one’s uncertainty). These conceptual tools can serve to alleviate the tension between enactivist and cognitivist theories. The present proposal thereby enables a dialogue about social cognition as an interactive and embodied process., Philosophy and Predictive Processing
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Multiplicity Needs Coherence – Towards a Unifying Framework for Social Understanding
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa
- Abstract
In this commentary, I focus on Albert Newen’s multiplicity view (MV) and aim to provide an alternative framework in which it can be embedded. Newen claims that social understanding draws on at least four different epistemic mechanisms, thus rejecting the idea that there is a default mechanism for social cognition. I claim that MV runs the risk of combining elements that have been described in metaphysically incompatible theories. I will argue that multiplicity needs coherence, which can be achieved by applying the theoretical framework of first-, second-, and third-order embodiment (1-3E; Metzinger 2014) to the study of social cognition. The modified version of this theory, 1-3sE (first-, second-, and third-order social embodiment), can serve as a unifying framework for a pluralistic account of social understanding., Open MIND
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Multiplicity needs coherence : towards a unifying framework for social understanding ; a commentary on Albert Newen
- Author
-
Quadt, Lisa
- Subjects
100 Philosophy ,100 Philosophie - Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.