42 results on '"Alexander Thiele"'
Search Results
2. Dopamine influences attentional rate modulation in Macaque posterior parietal cortex
- Author
-
Jochem van Kempen, Christian Brandt, Claudia Distler, Mark A. Bellgrove, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Abstract Cognitive neuroscience has made great strides in understanding the neural substrates of attention, but our understanding of its neuropharmacology remains incomplete. Although dopamine has historically been studied in relation to frontal functioning, emerging evidence suggests important dopaminergic influences in parietal cortex. We recorded single- and multi-unit activity whilst iontophoretically administering dopaminergic agonists and antagonists while rhesus macaques performed a spatial attention task. Out of 88 units, 50 revealed activity modulation by drug administration. Dopamine inhibited firing rates according to an inverted-U shaped dose–response curve and increased gain variability. D1 receptor antagonists diminished firing rates according to a monotonic function and interacted with attention modulating gain variability. Finally, both drugs decreased the pupil light reflex. These data show that dopamine shapes neuronal responses and modulates aspects of attentional processing in parietal cortex.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Tissue-type plasminogen activator induces conditioned receptive field plasticity in the mouse auditory cortex
- Author
-
Caitlin Smart, Anna Mitchell, Fiona McCutcheon, Robert L. Medcalf, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Cell biology ,Sensory neuroscience ,Science - Abstract
Summary: Tissue-type plasminogen activator (tPA) is a serine protease that is expressed in various compartments in the brain. It is involved in neuronal plasticity, learning and memory, and addiction. We evaluated whether tPA, exogenously applied, could influence neuroplasticity within the mouse auditory cortex. We used a frequency-pairing paradigm to determine whether neuronal best frequencies shift following the pairing protocol. tPA administration significantly affected the best frequency after pairing, whereby this depended on the pairing frequency relative to the best frequency. When the pairing frequency was above the best frequency, tPA caused a best frequency shift away from the conditioned frequency. tPA significantly widened auditory tuning curves. Our data indicate that regional changes in proteolytic activity within the auditory cortex modulate the fine-tuning of auditory neurons, supporting the function of tPA as a modulator of neuronal plasticity.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Effects of muscarinic and nicotinic receptors on contextual modulation in macaque area V1
- Author
-
Jose L. Herrero and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Abstract Context affects the salience and visibility of image elements in visual scenes. Collinear flankers can enhance or decrease the perceptual and neuronal sensitivity to flanked stimuli. These effects are mediated through lateral interactions between neurons in the primary visual cortex (area V1), in conjunction with feedback from higher visual areas. The strength of lateral interactions is affected by cholinergic neuromodulation. Blockade of muscarinic receptors should increase the strength of lateral intracortical interactions, while nicotinic blockade should reduce thalamocortical feed-forward drive. Here we test this proposal through local iontophoretic application of the muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine and the nicotinic receptor antagonist mecamylamine, while recording single cells in parafoveal representations in awake fixating macaque V1. Collinear flankers generally reduced neuronal contrast sensitivity. Muscarinic and nicotinic receptor blockade equally reduced neuronal contrast sensitivity. Contrary to our hypothesis, flanker interactions were not systematically affected by either receptor blockade.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Stimulus dependence of directed information exchange between cortical layers in macaque V1
- Author
-
Marc Alwin Gieselmann and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
oscillations ,local field potential ,visual cortex ,granger causality ,Medicine ,Science ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Perception and cognition require the integration of feedforward sensory information with feedback signals. Using different sized stimuli, we isolate spectral signatures of feedforward and feedback signals, and their effect on communication between layers in primary visual cortex of male macaque monkeys. Small stimuli elicited gamma frequency oscillations predominantly in the superficial layers. These Granger-causally originated in upper layer 4 and lower supragranular layers. Unexpectedly, large stimuli generated strong narrow band gamma oscillatory activity across cortical layers. They Granger-causally arose in layer 5, were conveyed through layer six to superficial layers, and violated existing models of feedback spectral signatures. Equally surprising, with large stimuli, alpha band oscillatory activity arose predominantly in granular and supragranular layers and communicated in a feedforward direction. Thus, oscillations in specific frequency bands are dynamically modulated to serve feedback and feedforward communication and are not restricted to specific cortical layers in V1.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Neuronal figure-ground responses in primate primary auditory cortex
- Author
-
Felix Schneider, Fabien Balezeau, Claudia Distler, Yukiko Kikuchi, Jochem van Kempen, Alwin Gieselmann, Christopher I. Petkov, Alexander Thiele, and Timothy D. Griffiths
- Subjects
auditory figure-ground segregation ,scene analysis ,stochastic figure-ground stimulus ,auditory cortex ,non-human primate ,A1 ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Summary: Figure-ground segregation, the brain’s ability to group related features into stable perceptual entities, is crucial for auditory perception in noisy environments. The neuronal mechanisms for this process are poorly understood in the auditory system. Here, we report figure-ground modulation of multi-unit activity (MUA) in the primary and non-primary auditory cortex of rhesus macaques. Across both regions, MUA increases upon presentation of auditory figures, which consist of coherent chord sequences. We show increased activity even in the absence of any perceptual decision, suggesting that neural mechanisms for perceptual grouping are, to some extent, independent of behavioral demands. Furthermore, we demonstrate differences in figure encoding between more anterior and more posterior regions; perceptual saliency is represented in anterior cortical fields only. Our results suggest an encoding of auditory figures from the earliest cortical stages by a rate code.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. MRI monitoring of macaque monkeys in neuroscience: Case studies, resource and normative data comparisons
- Author
-
Fabien Balezeau, Jennifer Nacef, Yukiko Kikuchi, Felix Schneider, Francesca Rocchi, Ross S. Muers, Rocio Fernandez-Palacios O'Connor, Christoph Blau, Benjamin Wilson, Richard C. Saunders, Matthew Howard, III, Alexander Thiele, Timothy D. Griffiths, Christopher I. Petkov, and Kathy Murphy
- Subjects
Magnetic resonance imaging ,Primate ,Welfare ,Neurology ,Monitoring ,Diagnosis ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Information from Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) is useful for diagnosis and treatment management of human neurological patients. MRI monitoring might also prove useful for non-human animals involved in neuroscience research provided that MRI is available and feasible and that there are no MRI contra-indications precluding scanning. However, MRI monitoring is not established in macaques and a resource is urgently needed that could grow with scientific community contributions. Here we show the utility and potential benefits of MRI-based monitoring in a few diverse cases with macaque monkeys. We also establish a PRIMatE MRI Monitoring (PRIME-MRM) resource within the PRIMatE Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) and quantitatively compare the cases to normative information drawn from MRI data from typical macaques in PRIME-DE. In the cases, the monkeys presented with no or mild/moderate clinical signs, were well otherwise and MRI scanning did not present a significant increase in welfare impact. Therefore, they were identified as suitable candidates for clinical investigation, MRI-based monitoring and treatment. For each case, we show MRI quantification of internal controls in relation to treatment steps and comparisons with normative data in typical monkeys drawn from PRIME-DE. We found that MRI assists in precise and early diagnosis of cerebral events and can be useful for visualising, treating and quantifying treatment response. The scientific community could now grow the PRIME-MRM resource with other cases and larger samples to further assess and increase the evidence base on the benefits of MRI monitoring of primates, complementing the animals’ clinical monitoring and treatment regime.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Perceptual learning of fine contrast discrimination changes neuronal tuning and population coding in macaque V4
- Author
-
Mehdi Sanayei, Xing Chen, Daniel Chicharro, Claudia Distler, Stefano Panzeri, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Science - Abstract
Perceptual learning, the improvement in perceptual abilities with training, is thought to involve changes in neuronal 'tuning'. Here, the authors show that perceptual learning works by making neurons increasingly sensitive to task-relevant differences in stimuli, and by improving population coding mechanisms.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. 26th Annual Computational Neuroscience Meeting (CNS*2017): Part 2
- Author
-
Leonid L. Rubchinsky, Sungwoo Ahn, Wouter Klijn, Ben Cumming, Stuart Yates, Vasileios Karakasis, Alexander Peyser, Marmaduke Woodman, Sandra Diaz-Pier, James Deraeve, Eliana Vassena, William Alexander, David Beeman, Pawel Kudela, Dana Boatman-Reich, William S. Anderson, Niceto R. Luque, Francisco Naveros, Richard R. Carrillo, Eduardo Ros, Angelo Arleo, Jacob Huth, Koki Ichinose, Jihoon Park, Yuji Kawai, Junichi Suzuki, Hiroki Mori, Minoru Asada, Sorinel A. Oprisan, Austin I. Dave, Tahereh Babaie, Peter Robinson, Alejandro Tabas, Martin Andermann, André Rupp, Emili Balaguer-Ballester, Henrik Lindén, Rasmus K. Christensen, Mari Nakamura, Tania R. Barkat, Zach Tosi, John Beggs, Davide Lonardoni, Fabio Boi, Stefano Di Marco, Alessandro Maccione, Luca Berdondini, Joanna Jędrzejewska-Szmek, Daniel B. Dorman, Kim T. Blackwell, Christoph Bauermeister, Hanna Keren, Jochen Braun, João V. Dornas, Eirini Mavritsaki, Silvio Aldrovandi, Emma Bridger, Sukbin Lim, Nicolas Brunel, Anatoly Buchin, Clifford Charles Kerr, Anton Chizhov, Gilles Huberfeld, Richard Miles, Boris Gutkin, Martin J. Spencer, Hamish Meffin, David B. Grayden, Anthony N. Burkitt, Catherine E. Davey, Liangyu Tao, Vineet Tiruvadi, Rehman Ali, Helen Mayberg, Robert Butera, Cengiz Gunay, Damon Lamb, Ronald L. Calabrese, Anca Doloc-Mihu, Víctor J. López-Madrona, Fernanda S. Matias, Ernesto Pereda, Claudio R. Mirasso, Santiago Canals, Alice Geminiani, Alessandra Pedrocchi, Egidio D’Angelo, Claudia Casellato, Ankur Chauhan, Karthik Soman, V. Srinivasa Chakravarthy, Vignayanandam R. Muddapu, Chao-Chun Chuang, Nan-yow Chen, Mehdi Bayati, Jan Melchior, Laurenz Wiskott, Amir Hossein Azizi, Kamran Diba, Sen Cheng, Elena Y. Smirnova, Elena G. Yakimova, Anton V. Chizhov, Nan-Yow Chen, Chi-Tin Shih, Dorian Florescu, Daniel Coca, Julie Courtiol, Viktor K. Jirsa, Roberto J. M. Covolan, Bartosz Teleńczuk, Richard Kempter, Gabriel Curio, Alain Destexhe, Jessica Parker, Alexander N. Klishko, Boris I. Prilutsky, Gennady Cymbalyuk, Felix Franke, Andreas Hierlemann, Rava Azeredo da Silveira, Stefano Casali, Stefano Masoli, Martina Rizza, Martina Francesca Rizza, Yinming Sun, Willy Wong, Faranak Farzan, Daniel M. Blumberger, Zafiris J. Daskalakis, Svitlana Popovych, Shivakumar Viswanathan, Nils Rosjat, Christian Grefkes, Silvia Daun, Damiano Gentiletti, Piotr Suffczynski, Vadym Gnatkovski, Marco De Curtis, Hyeonsu Lee, Se-Bum Paik, Woochul Choi, Jaeson Jang, Youngjin Park, Jun Ho Song, Min Song, Vicente Pallarés, Matthieu Gilson, Simone Kühn, Andrea Insabato, Gustavo Deco, Katharina Glomb, Adrián Ponce-Alvarez, Petra Ritter, Adria Tauste Campo, Alexander Thiele, Farah Deeba, P. A. Robinson, Sacha J. van Albada, Andrew Rowley, Michael Hopkins, Maximilian Schmidt, Alan B. Stokes, David R. Lester, Steve Furber, Markus Diesmann, Alessandro Barri, Martin T. Wiechert, David A. DiGregorio, Alexander G. Dimitrov, Catalina Vich, Rune W. Berg, Antoni Guillamon, Susanne Ditlevsen, Romain D. Cazé, Benoît Girard, Stéphane Doncieux, Nicolas Doyon, Frank Boahen, Patrick Desrosiers, Edward Laurence, Louis J. Dubé, Russo Eleonora, Daniel Durstewitz, Dominik Schmidt, Tuomo Mäki-Marttunen, Florian Krull, Francesco Bettella, Christoph Metzner, Anna Devor, Srdjan Djurovic, Anders M. Dale, Ole A. Andreassen, Gaute T. Einevoll, Solveig Næss, Torbjørn V. Ness, Geir Halnes, Eric Halgren, Klas H. Pettersen, Marte J. Sætra, Espen Hagen, Alina Schiffer, Axel Grzymisch, Malte Persike, Udo Ernst, Daniel Harnack, Udo A. Ernst, Nergis Tomen, Stefano Zucca, Valentina Pasquale, Giuseppe Pica, Manuel Molano-Mazón, Michela Chiappalone, Stefano Panzeri, Tommaso Fellin, Kelvin S. Oie, David L. Boothe, Joshua C. Crone, Alfred B. Yu, Melvin A. Felton, Isma Zulfiqar, Michelle Moerel, Peter De Weerd, Elia Formisano, Kelvin Oie, Piotr Franaszczuk, Roland Diggelmann, Michele Fiscella, Domenico Guarino, Jan Antolík, Andrew P. Davison, Yves Frègnac, Benjamin Xavier Etienne, Flavio Frohlich, Jérémie Lefebvre, Encarni Marcos, Maurizio Mattia, Aldo Genovesio, Leonid A. Fedorov, Tjeerd M.H. Dijkstra, Louisa Sting, Howard Hock, Martin A. Giese, Laure Buhry, Clément Langlet, Francesco Giovannini, Christophe Verbist, Stefano Salvadé, Michele Giugliano, James A. Henderson, Hendrik Wernecke, Bulcsú Sándor, Claudius Gros, Nicole Voges, Paulina Dabrovska, Alexa Riehle, Thomas Brochier, Sonja Grün, Yifan Gu, Pulin Gong, Grégory Dumont, Nikita A. Novikov, Boris S. Gutkin, Parul Tewatia, Olivia Eriksson, Andrei Kramer, Joao Santos, Alexandra Jauhiainen, Jeanette H. Kotaleski, Jovana J. Belić, Arvind Kumar, Jeanette Hellgren Kotaleski, Masanori Shimono, Naomichi Hatano, Subutai Ahmad, Yuwei Cui, Jeff Hawkins, Johanna Senk, Karolína Korvasová, Tom Tetzlaff, Moritz Helias, Tobias Kühn, Michael Denker, PierGianLuca Mana, David Dahmen, Jannis Schuecker, Sven Goedeke, Christian Keup, Katja Heuer, Rembrandt Bakker, Paul Tiesinga, Roberto Toro, Wei Qin, Alex Hadjinicolaou, Michael R. Ibbotson, Tatiana Kameneva, William W. Lytton, Lealem Mulugeta, Andrew Drach, Jerry G. Myers, Marc Horner, Rajanikanth Vadigepalli, Tina Morrison, Marlei Walton, Martin Steele, C. Anthony Hunt, Nicoladie Tam, Rodrigo Amaducci, Carlos Muñiz, Manuel Reyes-Sánchez, Francisco B. Rodríguez, Pablo Varona, Joseph T. Cronin, Matthias H. Hennig, Elisabetta Iavarone, Jane Yi, Ying Shi, Bas-Jan Zandt, Werner Van Geit, Christian Rössert, Henry Markram, Sean Hill, Christian O’Reilly, Rodrigo Perin, Huanxiang Lu, Alexander Bryson, Michal Hadrava, Jaroslav Hlinka, Ryosuke Hosaka, Mark Olenik, Conor Houghton, Nicolangelo Iannella, Thomas Launey, Rebecca Kotsakidis, Jaymar Soriano, Takatomi Kubo, Takao Inoue, Hiroyuki Kida, Toshitaka Yamakawa, Michiyasu Suzuki, Kazushi Ikeda, Samira Abbasi, Amber E. Hudson, Detlef H. Heck, Dieter Jaeger, Joel Lee, Skirmantas Janušonis, Maria Luisa Saggio, Andreas Spiegler, William C. Stacey, Christophe Bernard, Davide Lillo, Spase Petkoski, Mark Drakesmith, Derek K. Jones, Ali Sadegh Zadeh, Chandra Kambhampati, Jan Karbowski, Zeynep Gokcen Kaya, Yair Lakretz, Alessandro Treves, Lily W. Li, Joseph Lizier, Cliff C. Kerr, Timothée Masquelier, Saeed Reza Kheradpisheh, Hojeong Kim, Chang Sub Kim, Julia A. Marakshina, Alexander V. Vartanov, Anastasia A. Neklyudova, Stanislav A. Kozlovskiy, Andrey A. Kiselnikov, Kanako Taniguchi, Katsunori Kitano, Oliver Schmitt, Felix Lessmann, Sebastian Schwanke, Peter Eipert, Jennifer Meinhardt, Julia Beier, Kanar Kadir, Adrian Karnitzki, Linda Sellner, Ann-Christin Klünker, Lena Kuch, Frauke Ruß, Jörg Jenssen, Andreas Wree, Paula Sanz-Leon, Stuart A. Knock, Shih-Cheng Chien, Burkhard Maess, Thomas R. Knösche, Charles C. Cohen, Marko A. Popovic, Jan Klooster, Maarten H.P. Kole, Erik A. Roberts, Nancy J. Kopell, Daniel Kepple, Hamza Giaffar, Dima Rinberg, Alex Koulakov, Caroline Garcia Forlim, Leonie Klock, Johanna Bächle, Laura Stoll, Patrick Giemsa, Marie Fuchs, Nikola Schoofs, Christiane Montag, Jürgen Gallinat, Ray X. Lee, Greg J. Stephens, Bernd Kuhn, Luiz Tauffer, Philippe Isope, Katsuma Inoue, Yoshiyuki Ohmura, Shogo Yonekura, Yasuo Kuniyoshi, Hyun Jae Jang, Jeehyun Kwag, Marc de Kamps, Yi Ming Lai, Filipa dos Santos, K. P. Lam, Peter Andras, Julia Imperatore, Jessica Helms, Tamas Tompa, Antonieta Lavin, Felicity H. Inkpen, Michael C. Ashby, Nathan F. Lepora, Aaron R. Shifman, John E. Lewis, Zhong Zhang, Yeqian Feng, Christian Tetzlaff, Tomas Kulvicius, Yinyun Li, Rodrigo F. O. Pena, Davide Bernardi, Antonio C. Roque, Benjamin Lindner, Sebastian Vellmer, Ausra Saudargiene, Tiina Maninen, Riikka Havela, Marja-Leena Linne, Arthur Powanwe, Andre Longtin, Jesús A. Garrido, Joe W. Graham, Salvador Dura-Bernal, Sergio L. Angulo, Samuel A. Neymotin, and Srdjan D. Antic
- Subjects
Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 ,Neurophysiology and neuropsychology ,QP351-495 - Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Behavioural and neural signatures of perceptual decision-making are modulated by pupil-linked arousal
- Author
-
Jochem van Kempen, Gerard M Loughnane, Daniel P Newman, Simon P Kelly, Alexander Thiele, Redmond G O'Connell, and Mark A Bellgrove
- Subjects
decision-making ,pupil diameter ,EEG ,human ,arousal ,Medicine ,Science ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
The timing and accuracy of perceptual decision-making is exquisitely sensitive to fluctuations in arousal. Although extensive research has highlighted the role of various neural processing stages in forming decisions, our understanding of how arousal impacts these processes remains limited. Here we isolated electrophysiological signatures of decision-making alongside signals reflecting target selection, attentional engagement and motor output and examined their modulation as a function of tonic and phasic arousal, indexed by baseline and task-evoked pupil diameter, respectively. Reaction times were shorter on trials with lower tonic, and higher phasic arousal. Additionally, these two pupil measures were predictive of a unique set of EEG signatures that together represent multiple information processing steps of decision-making. Finally, behavioural variability associated with fluctuations in tonic and phasic arousal, indicative of neuromodulators acting on multiple timescales, was mediated by its effects on the EEG markers of attentional engagement, sensory processing and the variability in decision processing.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Muscarinic and Nicotinic Contribution to Contrast Sensitivity of Macaque Area V1 Neurons
- Author
-
Jose L. Herrero, Marc A. Gieselmann, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
acetylcholine ,visual cortex organization ,contrast sensitivity ,normalization ,primary visual cortex (V1) ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Acetylcholine is a neuromodulator that shapes information processing in different cortical and subcortical areas. Cell type and location specific cholinergic receptor distributions suggest that acetylcholine in macaque striate cortex should boost feed-forward driven activity, while also reducing population excitability by increasing inhibitory tone. Studies using cholinergic agonists in anesthetized primate V1 have yielded conflicting evidence for such a proposal. Here we investigated how muscarinic or nicotinic receptor blockade affect neuronal excitability and contrast response functions in awake macaque area V1. Muscarinic or nicotinic receptor blockade caused reduced activity for all contrasts tested, without affecting the contrast where neurons reach their half maximal response (c50). The activity reduction upon muscarinic and nicotinic blockade resulted in reduced neuronal contrast sensitivity, as assessed through neurometric functions. In the majority of cells receptor blockade was best described by a response gain model (a multiplicative scaling of responses), indicating that ACh is involved in signal enhancement, not saliency filtering in macaque V1.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Auditory motion-specific mechanisms in the primate brain.
- Author
-
Colline Poirier, Simon Baumann, Pradeep Dheerendra, Olivier Joly, David Hunter, Fabien Balezeau, Li Sun, Adrian Rees, Christopher I Petkov, Alexander Thiele, and Timothy D Griffiths
- Subjects
Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
This work examined the mechanisms underlying auditory motion processing in the auditory cortex of awake monkeys using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We tested to what extent auditory motion analysis can be explained by the linear combination of static spatial mechanisms, spectrotemporal processes, and their interaction. We found that the posterior auditory cortex, including A1 and the surrounding caudal belt and parabelt, is involved in auditory motion analysis. Static spatial and spectrotemporal processes were able to fully explain motion-induced activation in most parts of the auditory cortex, including A1, but not in circumscribed regions of the posterior belt and parabelt cortex. We show that in these regions motion-specific processes contribute to the activation, providing the first demonstration that auditory motion is not simply deduced from changes in static spatial location. These results demonstrate that parallel mechanisms for motion and static spatial analysis coexist within the auditory dorsal stream.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. The use of preferred social stimuli as rewards for rhesus macaques in behavioural neuroscience.
- Author
-
Helen Gray, Bradley Pearce, Alexander Thiele, and Candy Rowe
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Macaques are often motivated to perform in neuroscientific experiments by implementing fluid restriction protocols. Daily access to water is controlled and the monkeys are rewarded with droplets of fluid for performing correct trials in the laboratory. Although these protocols are widely used and highly effective, it is important from a 3Rs perspective to investigate refinements that may help to lessen the severity of the fluid restriction applied. We assessed the use of social stimuli (images of conspecifics) as rewards for four rhesus macaques performing simple cognitive tasks. We found that individual preferences for images of male faces, female perinea and control stimuli could be identified in each monkey. However, using preferred images did not translate into effective motivators on a trial-by-trial basis: animals preferred fluid rewards, even when fluid restriction was relaxed. There was no difference in the monkeys' performance of a task when using greyscale versus colour images. Based on our findings, we cannot recommend the use of social stimuli, in this form, as a refinement to current fluid restriction protocols. We discuss the potential alternatives and possibilities for future research.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. The topography of frequency and time representation in primate auditory cortices
- Author
-
Simon Baumann, Olivier Joly, Adrian Rees, Christopher I Petkov, Li Sun, Alexander Thiele, and Timothy D Griffiths
- Subjects
auditory cortex ,amplitude modulation ,tonotopy ,topography ,fMRI ,macaque ,Medicine ,Science ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Natural sounds can be characterised by their spectral content and temporal modulation, but how the brain is organized to analyse these two critical sound dimensions remains uncertain. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we demonstrate a topographical representation of amplitude modulation rate in the auditory cortex of awake macaques. The representation of this temporal dimension is organized in approximately concentric bands of equal rates across the superior temporal plane in both hemispheres, progressing from high rates in the posterior core to low rates in the anterior core and lateral belt cortex. In A1 the resulting gradient of modulation rate runs approximately perpendicular to the axis of the tonotopic gradient, suggesting an orthogonal organisation of spectral and temporal sound dimensions. In auditory belt areas this relationship is more complex. The data suggest a continuous representation of modulation rate across several physiological areas, in contradistinction to a separate representation of frequency within each area.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Stimulus roving and flankers affect perceptual learning of contrast discrimination in Macaca mulatta.
- Author
-
Xing Chen, Mehdi Sanayei, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
'Stimulus roving' refers to a paradigm in which the properties of the stimuli to be discriminated vary from trial to trial, rather than being kept constant throughout a block of trials. Rhesus monkeys have previously been shown to improve their contrast discrimination performance on a non-roving task, in which they had to report the contrast of a test stimulus relative to that of a fixed-contrast sample stimulus. Human psychophysics studies indicate that roving stimuli yield little or no perceptual learning. Here, we investigate how stimulus roving influences perceptual learning in macaque monkeys and how the addition of flankers alters performance under roving conditions. Animals were initially trained on a contrast discrimination task under non-roving conditions until their performance levels stabilized. The introduction of roving contrast conditions resulted in a pronounced drop in performance, which suggested that subjects initially failed to heed the sample contrast and performed the task using an internal memory reference. With training, significant improvements occurred, demonstrating that learning is possible under roving conditions. To investigate the notion of flanker-induced perceptual learning, flanker stimuli (30% fixed-contrast iso-oriented collinear gratings) were presented jointly with central (roving) stimuli. Presentation of flanker stimuli yielded substantial performance improvements in one subject, but deteriorations in the other. Finally, after the removal of flankers, performance levels returned to their pre-flanker state in both subjects, indicating that the flanker-induced changes were contingent upon the continued presentation of flankers.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. The representation of time windows in primate auditory cortex
- Author
-
Pradeep Dheerendra, Simon Baumann, Olivier Joly, Fabien Balezeau, Christopher I Petkov, Alexander Thiele, and Timothy D Griffiths
- Subjects
Auditory Cortex ,Brain Mapping ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Auditory Perception ,Animals ,Humans ,Macaca mulatta - Abstract
Whether human and nonhuman primates process the temporal dimension of sound similarly remains an open question. We examined the brain basis for the processing of acoustic time windows in rhesus macaques using stimuli simulating the spectrotemporal complexity of vocalizations. We conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging in awake macaques to identify the functional anatomy of response patterns to different time windows. We then contrasted it against the responses to identical stimuli used previously in humans. Despite a similar overall pattern, ranging from the processing of shorter time windows in core areas to longer time windows in lateral belt and parabelt areas, monkeys exhibited lower sensitivity to longer time windows than humans. This difference in neuronal sensitivity might be explained by a specialization of the human brain for processing longer time windows in speech.
- Published
- 2022
17. Contribution of Ionotropic Glutamatergic Receptors to Excitability and Attentional Signals in Macaque Frontal Eye Field
- Author
-
Miguel Dasilva, Christian Brandt, Marc Alwin Gieselmann, Alexander Thiele, and Claudia Distler
- Subjects
Male ,N-Methylaspartate ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,primate ,Macaque ,neuropharmacology ,Receptors, N-Methyl-D-Aspartate ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Glutamatergic ,Dopamine ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Excitatory Amino Acid Agonists ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Animals ,Receptors, AMPA ,AcademicSubjects/MED00385 ,Receptor ,alpha-Amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic Acid ,Neuropharmacology ,biology ,frontal cortex ,Attentional control ,Macaca mulatta ,attention ,Frontal Lobe ,Original Article ,AcademicSubjects/MED00310 ,Neuroscience ,Acetylcholine ,Photic Stimulation ,Ionotropic effect ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Top-down attention, controlled by frontal cortical areas, is a key component of cognitive operations. How different neurotransmitters and neuromodulators flexibly change the cellular and network interactions with attention demands remains poorly understood. While acetylcholine and dopamine are critically involved, glutamatergic receptors have been proposed to play important roles. To understand their contribution to attentional signals, we investigated how ionotropic glutamatergic receptors in the frontal eye field (FEF) of male macaques contribute to neuronal excitability and attentional control signals in different cell types. Broad-spiking and narrow-spiking cells both required N-methyl-D-aspartic acid and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptor activation for normal excitability, thereby affecting ongoing or stimulus-driven activity. However, attentional control signals were not dependent on either glutamatergic receptor type in broad- or narrow-spiking cells. A further subdivision of cell types into different functional types using cluster-analysis based on spike waveforms and spiking characteristics did not change the conclusions. This can be explained by a model where local blockade of specific ionotropic receptors is compensated by cell embedding in large-scale networks. It sets the glutamatergic system apart from the cholinergic system in FEF and demonstrates that a reduction in excitability is not sufficient to induce a reduction in attentional control signals.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Directed information exchange between cortical layers in macaque V1 and V4 and its modulation by selective attention
- Author
-
Alexander Thiele, Demetrio Ferro, Michael Boyd, Stefano Panzeri, and Jochem van Kempen
- Subjects
Sensory system ,Local field potential ,Macaque ,laminar interaction ,biology.animal ,feedforward processing ,Animals ,Visual Pathways ,Visual Cortex ,Physics ,feedback processing ,Multidisciplinary ,biology ,Feed forward ,Information flow ,Cognition ,Biological Sciences ,Macaca mulatta ,attention ,Modulation ,Receptive field ,Evoked Potentials, Visual ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Significance Attention is thought to modulate sensory processing by changing communication between cortical areas within specific frequency bands. Using local field potential recordings, we tested this influential model through laminar recordings in macaque V1 and V4. Attention modulated communication unexpectedly. In V1, it decreased communication across spectral frequencies except for granular-to-supragranular interactions. In V4, it increased communication across all spectral frequencies. Critically, attention increased V1–V4 feedforward communication across all frequency bands, decreased V4–V1 feedback communication in low-frequency bands, and increased beta and gamma feedback communication. These findings challenge existing theories of frequency specificity of feedforward and feedback interactions., Achieving behavioral goals requires integration of sensory and cognitive information across cortical laminae and cortical regions. How this computation is performed remains unknown. Using local field potential recordings and spectrally resolved conditional Granger causality (cGC) analysis, we mapped visual information flow, and its attentional modulation, between cortical layers within and between macaque brain areas V1 and V4. Stimulus-induced interlaminar information flow within V1 dominated upwardly, channeling information toward supragranular corticocortical output layers. Within V4, information flow dominated from granular to supragranular layers, but interactions between supragranular and infragranular layers dominated downwardly. Low-frequency across-area communication was stronger from V4 to V1, with little layer specificity. Gamma-band communication was stronger in the feedforward V1-to-V4 direction. Attention to the receptive field of V1 decreased communication between all V1 layers, except for granular-to-supragranular layer interactions. Communication within V4, and from V1 to V4, increased with attention across all frequencies. While communication from V4 to V1 was stronger in lower-frequency bands (4 to 25 Hz), attention modulated cGCs from V4 to V1 across all investigated frequencies. Our data show that top-down cognitive processes result in reduced communication within cortical areas, increased feedforward communication across all frequency bands, and increased gamma-band feedback communication.
- Published
- 2021
19. Protective cranial implant caps for macaques
- Author
-
Michael C. Schmid, Christopher I. Petkov, Caroline Bergmann, Alexander Thiele, Stuart Mason, Jennifer Nacef, Brook A.L. Perry, Ashley Waddle, Brian Hynes, and Anna S. Mitchell
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Perforation (oil well) ,Welfare ,Neurophysiology ,Neuroimaging ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cognition ,biology.animal ,Medicine ,Animals ,Primate ,Primate chair ,Behavior ,Non human primate ,biology ,integumentary system ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Skull ,Brain ,Prostheses and Implants ,Non-human primate ,Macaca mulatta ,Surgery ,030104 developmental biology ,Wound management ,Neuroscience research ,Implant ,business ,Wound healing ,Head ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cranial implant ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Highlights • A non-human primate protective head cap that promotes wound healing after cranial implants. • Use of the head cap reduced wound dehiscence and the need to re-suture surgical wounds. • The head cap is easily adjustable to cover most primate cranial implants. • The head cap facilitates primate cranial implant wound management in neuroscience., Background Neuroscience studies with macaque monkeys may require cranial implants to stabilize the head or gain access to the brain for scientific purposes. Wound management that promotes healing after the cranial implant surgery in non-human primates can be difficult as it is not necessarily possible to cover the wound margins. New method Here, we developed an easily modifiable head cap that protects the sutured skin margins after cranial implant surgery and contributes to wound healing. The protective head cap was developed in response to monkeys picking at sutured skin margins around an implant, complicating healing. The user-friendly protective cap, made from Klarity- R™ Sheet (3.2 mm thick with 36 % or 42 % perforation) is affixed to the implant post-surgically. Once secured and while the monkey is still anesthetized, the plastic sheeting is molded around the implant. The protective head cap restricts the monkey’s finger access to its’ wound margins while allowing air to circulate to promote wound healing. Results and comparison with existing methods Across two UK primate facilities, the protective head cap promoted wound healing. In monkeys that did not wear the head cap, re-suturing was necessary in ∼30 % of cases. In contrast, none of the monkeys that wore the head cap required re-suturing. The monkeys wearing the head cap also had reduced numbers of days of prescribed antibiotics and analgesia. Conclusion This bespoken, easily adaptable, protective head cap supports postoperative wound healing, and enhances the welfare of monkeys involved in neuroscience research.
- Published
- 2021
20. Accelerating the Evolution of Nonhuman Primate Neuroimaging
- Author
-
Olivier Coulon, Michael P. Milham, Patrik Lindenfors, Karl-Heinz Nenning, Xiaojin Liu, Ravi S. Menon, Stephanie J. Forkel, Adam Messinger, Zheng Wang, Alexander Thiele, Luciano Simone, Benjamin Jung, Chika Sato, Jamie Nagy, Sean Froudist-Walsh, Kelvin Mok, Renée Hartig, Julien Sein, Alessandro Gozzi, Julien Vezoli, Tomoko Sakai, Lynn Uhrig, Martine Meunier, Christienne G. Damatac, Bonhwang Koo, Roberto Toro, Rogier B. Mars, Henrietta Howells, Lea Roumazeilles, Ming Zhan, Ann-Marie Mallon, Román Rossi-Pool, Elinor L. Sullivan, Yannick Becker, Doris Y. Tsao, Antoine Grigis, Lei Ai, Céline Amiez, Sara Wells, Reza Rajimehr, Aki Nikolaidis, Anna S. Mitchell, Simon M. Reader, Michele A. Basso, Béchir Jarraya, Amir Raz, Wim Vanduffel, Charles R.E. Wilson, Brian E. Russ, Christopher R. Madan, Orlin S. Todorov, Wasana Madushanka, Carole Guedj, Mark A. Pinsk, Clémentine Bodin, Hugo Merchant, Jennifer Nacef, Damien A. Fair, Anna W. Roe, Sze Chai Kwok, Stephen J. Sawiak, Essa Yacoub, Bastien Cagna, Kevin N. Laland, Wilbert Zarco, Charles E. Schroeder, Ting Xu, P. Christiaan Klink, Stanislas Dehaene, Takuya Hayashi, Matthew F. S. Rushworth, Amir Shmuel, Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Katja Heuer, Ioana-Sabina Rautu, Andrew S. Fox, Austin Benn, Sabine Kastner, Thomas Brochier, Emmanuel Procyk, Marco Pagani, David C. Van Essen, Frank Q. Ye, Dirk Jan Ardesch, Régis Trapeau, Jakob Seidlitz, Marike Schiffer, Bassem Hiba, John H. Morrison, David A. Rudko, Paula L. Croxson, Patrick Friedrich, Augix Guohua Xu, Lazar Fleysher, Piotr Majka, Jonathan Smallwood, Aihua Chen, Timothy D. Griffiths, Fabien Balezeau, Stefan Everling, Michael C. Schmid, Robert Leech, Leslie G. Ungerleider, Mark G. Baxter, Afonso C. Silva, Clare Kelly, Zhi-ming Shen, Daniel S. Margulies, Mark J. Prescott, Pascal Belin, Erwin L. A. Blezer, Igor Kagan, Suliann Ben Hamed, David A. Leopold, Adrien Meguerditchian, Wendy Jarrett, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten, Nikoloz Sirmpilatze, Julia Sliwa, Henry Kennedy, Vikas Pareek, Yong-di Zhou, Michael Ortiz-Rios, Sherif Hamdy El-Gohary, Susann Boretius, Christopher I. Petkov, Pamela Garcia-Saldivar, Bella Williams, Jordy Tasserie, Hank P. Jedema, Jerome Sallet, Pieter R. Roelfsema, Winrich A. Freiwald, Eduardo A. Garza-Villarreal, Noam Harel, Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Kevin Marche, Colline Poirier, Yang Gao, Henry C. Evrard, Ashkan Alvand, ANS - Cellular & Molecular Mechanisms, Laboratoire des Sciences de l'Information et des Systèmes (LSIS), Aix Marseille Université (AMU)-Université de Toulon (UTLN)-Arts et Métiers Paristech ENSAM Aix-en-Provence-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institut cellule souche et cerveau (SBRI), Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique (INRA)-Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Collège de France - Chaire Psychologie cognitive expérimentale, Collège de France (CdF (institution)), Institut des sciences cognitives Marc Jeannerod - Centre de neuroscience cognitive - UMR5229 (ISC-MJ), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Consortium, PRIMatE Data Exchange Global Collaboration Workshop and, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research (NKI), New York State Office of Mental Health, Newcastle University [Newcastle], Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences [Leipzig] (IMPNSC), Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, Medical Oncology, Department of Internal Medicine, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore [Roma] (Unicatt), Voice Neurocognition Laboratory, University of Glasgow, Oregon Health and Science University [Portland] (OHSU), Manchester Royal Infirmary, University of Manchester [Manchester], Princeton Neuroscience Institute [Princeton], University of Pennsylvania [Philadelphia], National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), Harvard Medical School [Boston] (HMS), Washington University in St Louis, Laboratoire de psychologie cognitive (LPC), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Aix Marseille Université (AMU), Institut des sciences cognitives Marc Jeannerod - Centre de neuroscience cognitive - UMR5229 (CNC), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon, Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone (INT), Aix Marseille Université (AMU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, King‘s College London, New York University [New York] (NYU), NYU System (NYU), State Key Laboratory of Novel Software Technology, University of Nanjing, Center for Nanotechnology Innovation, @NEST (CNI), National Enterprise for nanoScience and nanoTechnology (NEST), Scuola Normale Superiore di Pisa (SNS)-Scuola Universitaria Superiore Sant'Anna [Pisa] (SSSUP)-Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (IIT)-Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche [Pisa] (CNR PISA)-Scuola Normale Superiore di Pisa (SNS)-Scuola Universitaria Superiore Sant'Anna [Pisa] (SSSUP)-Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (IIT)-Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche [Pisa] (CNR PISA), Unité Analyse et Traitement de l'Information (UNATI), Service NEUROSPIN (NEUROSPIN), Direction de Recherche Fondamentale (CEA) (DRF (CEA)), Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Université Paris-Saclay-Direction de Recherche Fondamentale (CEA) (DRF (CEA)), Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Université Paris-Saclay, Neuroimagerie cognitive - Psychologie cognitive expérimentale (UNICOG-U992), Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Paris Saclay (COmUE), Institut cellule souche et cerveau (U846 Inserm - UCBL1), Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), East China Normal University [Shangaï] (ECNU), The Computational, Cognitive and Clinical Neuroimaging Lab, Imperial College London, Medical Research Council Harwell (Mammalian Genetics Unit and Mary Lyon Centre), Medical Research Counc, Station de primatologie (SP), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institute of Language, Communication and the Brain (ILCB), The University of Western Ontario, Instituto de Neurobiología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM), Institute of integrative biology (Liverpool), University of Liverpool, McGovern Institute for Brain Research [Cambridge], Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), University of Cambridge [UK] (CAM), Rockefeller University [New York], McConnell Brain Imaging Centre (MNI), Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, McGill University = Université McGill [Montréal, Canada]-McGill University = Université McGill [Montréal, Canada], Laboratory for Neuro- and Psychofysiology, katho, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moëlle Epinière = Brain and Spine Institute (ICM), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-CHU Pitié-Salpêtrière [AP-HP], Sorbonne Université-Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université-Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University of York [York, UK], Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière (CRICM), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Pierre et Marie Curie - Paris 6 (UPMC), Génétique Humaine et Fonctions Cognitives, Institut Pasteur [Paris]-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Tianjin University of Science and Technology (TUST), Child Mind Institute, Center for Magnetic Resonance Research [Minneapolis] (CMRR), University of Minnesota Medical School, University of Minnesota System-University of Minnesota System, Institut cellule souche et cerveau / Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute (U1208 Inserm - UCBL1 / SBRI), Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai [New York] (MSSM), University Medical Center [Utrecht], Radboud university [Nijmegen], Chaire Psychologie cognitive expérimentale, Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet [Saint-Étienne] (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University of St Andrews [Scotland], Stockholm University, Hôpital du Bocage, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Dijon - Hôpital François Mitterrand (CHU Dijon), Princeton University, Utrecht University [Utrecht], Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Wellcome Trust Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (WIN - FMRIB), University of Oxford [Oxford], National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences [Durham] (NIEHS-NIH), National Institutes of Health [Bethesda] (NIH), Oregon National Primate Research Center (ONPRC), California Institute of Technology (CALTECH), Johns Hopkins University (JHU), The PRIMatE Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) Global Collaboration Workshop and Consortium, ANR-16-CONV-0002,ILCB,ILCB: Institute of Language Communication and the Brain(2016), Amsterdam Neuroscience - Complex Trait Genetics, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Cellular & Molecular Mechanisms, Complex Trait Genetics, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Arts et Métiers Paristech ENSAM Aix-en-Provence-Université de Toulon (UTLN)-Aix Marseille Université (AMU), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique (INRA), and Vanduffel, Wim
- Subjects
Primates ,0301 basic medicine ,[SDV.IB.IMA]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Bioengineering/Imaging ,education ,Neuroimaging ,Article ,[SPI]Engineering Sciences [physics] ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,London ,Psychology ,Animals ,Humans ,Sociology ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,Cognitive science ,Science & Technology ,Human Connectome Project ,[SDV.NEU.PC]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Psychology and behavior ,Action, intention, and motor control ,Information Dissemination ,General Neuroscience ,Neurosciences ,Brain ,[SDV.NEU.SC]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Cognitive Sciences ,Congresses as Topic ,Nonhuman primate ,030104 developmental biology ,[SDV.NEU]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC] ,Neurosciences & Neurology ,Life Sciences & Biomedicine ,[SPI.SIGNAL]Engineering Sciences [physics]/Signal and Image processing ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Contains fulltext : 217200.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Closed access) Nonhuman primate neuroimaging is on the cusp of a transformation, much in the same way its human counterpart was in 2010, when the Human Connectome Project was launched to accelerate progress. Inspired by an open data-sharing initiative, the global community recently met and, in this article, breaks through obstacles to define its ambitions. 4 p.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. An Open Resource for Non-human Primate Imaging
- Author
-
Jennifer Nacef, Kevin N. Laland, Wilbert Zarco, Charles E. Schroeder, Jamie Nagy, Julien Sein, P. Christiaan Klink, Kelvin Mok, Michael P. Milham, Stanislas Dehaene, Charles R.E. Wilson, Orlin S. Todorov, Erwin L. A. Blezer, Winrich A. Freiwald, Béchir Jarraya, Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, John H. Morrison, David A. Rudko, Zheng Wang, Essa Yacoub, Leslie G. Ungerleider, Brian E. Russ, Amir Shmuel, Emmanuel Procyk, Stefan Everling, Elinor L. Sullivan, Doris Y. Tsao, Noam Harel, Bassem Hiba, Thomas Brochier, Jakob Seidlitz, Sabine Kastner, Michael Ortiz Rios, Martine Meunier, Alexander Thiele, Carole Guedj, Paula L. Croxson, Sze Chai Kwok, Yong-di Zhou, Christopher I. Petkov, Ting Xu, Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Frank Q. Ye, Fabien Balezeau, Aihua Chen, Ravi S. Menon, Adam Messinger, Mark G. Baxter, Matthew F. S. Rushworth, Sean Froudist-Walsh, Rogier B. Mars, Reza Rajimehr, Christienne G. Damatac, Bonhwang Koo, Lei Ai, Colline Poirier, David A. Leopold, Simon M. Reader, Daniel S. Margulies, Mark A. Pinsk, Benjamin Jung, Lazar Fleysher, Timothy D. Griffiths, Michael C. Schmid, Céline Amiez, Suliann Ben Hamed, Jerome Sallet, Pieter R. Roelfsema, Damian A. Fair, Patrik Lindenfors, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research (NKI), New York State Office of Mental Health, Child Mind Institute, Institut cellule souche et cerveau / Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute (SBRI), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), Newcastle University [Newcastle], Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai [New York] (MSSM), University Medical Center [Utrecht], Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone (INT), Aix Marseille Université (AMU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), East China Normal University [Shangaï] (ECNU), Radboud University [Nijmegen], Neuroimagerie cognitive - Psychologie cognitive expérimentale (UNICOG-U992), Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Paris-Saclay, Collège de France - Chaire Psychologie cognitive expérimentale, Collège de France (CdF (institution)), The University of Western Ontario, Oregon Health and Science University [Portland] (OHSU), Rockefeller University [New York], New York University [New York] (NYU), NYU System (NYU), Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon - Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet - Saint-Étienne (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institut des sciences cognitives Marc Jeannerod - Centre de neuroscience cognitive - UMR5229 (ISC-MJ), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University of Minnesota Medical School, University of Minnesota System, National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), Princeton Neuroscience Institute [Princeton], Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), University of St Andrews [Scotland], National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke [Bethesda] (NINDS), National Institutes of Health [Bethesda] (NIH), Stockholm University, Wellcome Trust Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (WIN - FMRIB), University of Oxford, McConnell Brain Imaging Centre (MNI), Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, McGill University = Université McGill [Montréal, Canada]-McGill University = Université McGill [Montréal, Canada], University of California [Davis] (UC Davis), University of California (UC), Princeton University, McGovern Institute for Brain Research [Cambridge], Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), Utrecht University [Utrecht], McGill University = Université McGill [Montréal, Canada], Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences [Durham] (NIEHS-NIH), Oregon National Primate Research Center (ONPRC), California Institute of Technology (CALTECH), Tianjin University of Science and Technology (TUST), Center for Magnetic Resonance Research [Minneapolis] (CMRR), University of Minnesota System-University of Minnesota System, Johns Hopkins University (JHU), Centre de Recherche en Éthique [UdeM - Montréal] (CREUM), Université de Montréal (UdeM), Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences [Leipzig] (IMPNSC), Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, Institut du Cerveau = Paris Brain Institute (ICM), Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-CHU Pitié-Salpêtrière [AP-HP], Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, University of St Andrews. School of Biology, University of St Andrews. Centre for Biological Diversity, University of St Andrews. Scottish Oceans Institute, University of St Andrews. Institute of Behavioural and Neural Sciences, University of St Andrews. Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolution, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN), Equipe Impact, Centre de Recherche en Neurosciences de Lyon, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Academic Medical Center, Institut cellule souche et cerveau / Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute (U1208 Inserm - UCBL1 / SBRI - USC 1361 INRAE), Radboud university [Nijmegen], Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Université Paris Saclay (COmUE)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Chaire Psychologie cognitive expérimentale, Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet [Saint-Étienne] (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institut des sciences cognitives Marc Jeannerod - Centre de neuroscience cognitive - UMR5229 (CNC), University of Oxford [Oxford], University of California, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moëlle Epinière = Brain and Spine Institute (ICM), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-CHU Pitié-Salpêtrière [AP-HP], Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Seidlitz, Jakob [0000-0002-8164-7476], Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, Integrative Neurophysiology, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Cellular & Molecular Mechanisms, Univ Lyon, Université Lyon 1, Inserm, Stem Cell and Brain Research Institute U1208, Bron, France, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Aix Marseille Université (AMU), Neuroimagerie cognitive (UNICOG-U992), Commissariat à l'énergie atomique et aux énergies alternatives (CEA)-Université Paris-Saclay-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Paris-Sud - Paris 11 (UP11), The Rockefeller University, McGill University-McGill University, McGill University, Centre de Recherche en Éthique de l'Université de Montréal (CREUM), Université de Montréal [Montréal], Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-CHU Pitié-Salpêtrière [APHP]-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), and Ben Hamed, Suliann
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Primates ,Computer science ,Neuroscience(all) ,[SDV]Life Sciences [q-bio] ,QH301 Biology ,Datasets as Topic ,Neuroimaging ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,QH301 ,0302 clinical medicine ,Resource (project management) ,All institutes and research themes of the Radboud University Medical Center ,SDG 17 - Partnerships for the Goals ,Connectome ,Animals ,Psychology ,[SDV.NEU] Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC] ,Independent data ,Biological sciences ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,Non human primate ,Neurodevelopmental disorders Donders Center for Medical Neuroscience [Radboudumc 7] ,Neurology & Neurosurgery ,Quality assessment ,Action, intention, and motor control ,Information Dissemination ,General Neuroscience ,Neurosciences ,Perception, Action and Control [DI-BCB_DCC_Theme 2] ,Brain ,DAS ,Data science ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,030104 developmental biology ,Data exchange ,Biomedical Imaging ,Cognitive Sciences ,[SDV.NEU]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC] ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Summary Non-human primate neuroimaging is a rapidly growing area of research that promises to transform and scale translational and cross-species comparative neuroscience. Unfortunately, the technological and methodological advances of the past two decades have outpaced the accrual of data, which is particularly challenging given the relatively few centers that have the necessary facilities and capabilities. The PRIMatE Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) addresses this challenge by aggregating independently acquired non-human primate magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) datasets and openly sharing them via the International Neuroimaging Data-sharing Initiative (INDI). Here, we present the rationale, design, and procedures for the PRIME-DE consortium, as well as the initial release, consisting of 25 independent data collections aggregated across 22 sites (total = 217 non-human primates). We also outline the unique pitfalls and challenges that should be considered in the analysis of non-human primate MRI datasets, including providing automated quality assessment of the contributed datasets., Highlights • Openly shared, large non-human primate neuroimaging data resource • Multiple imaging modalities contributed from investigators around the world • Quality assessments of the dataset • Discussed pitfalls and challenges in analyzing the non-human primate MRI data, The PRIMatE Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) consortium is an open science resource for the neuroimaging community aiming to facilitate efforts to map the non-human primate connectome. It aggregates and shares anatomical, functional, and diffusion MRI datasets from laboratories throughout the world.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Attention and normalization circuits in macaque V1
- Author
-
Alexander Thiele, Claudia Distler, Mehdi Sanayei, and Jose L. Herrero
- Subjects
Normalization (statistics) ,Male ,Visual perception ,orientation tuning ,Surround suppression ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Models, Neurological ,Action Potentials ,Macaque ,Bayesian information criterion ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Animals ,Visual Cortex ,Neurons ,striate cortex ,biology ,Mechanism (biology) ,General Neuroscience ,Bayes Theorem ,Macaca mulatta ,attention ,surround suppression ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,normalization ,Visual Perception ,Akaike information criterion ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Microelectrodes ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Attention affects neuronal processing and improves behavioural performance. In extrastriate visual cortex these effects have been explained by normalization models, which assume that attention influences the circuit that mediates surround suppression. While normalization models have been able to explain attentional effects, their validity has rarely been tested against alternative models. Here we investigate how attention and surround/mask stimuli affect neuronal firing rates and orientation tuning in macaque V1. Surround/mask stimuli provide an estimate to what extent V1 neurons are affected by normalization, which was compared against effects of spatial top down attention. For some attention/surround effect comparisons, the strength of attentional modulation was correlated with the strength of surround modulation, suggesting that attention and surround/mask stimulation (i.e. normalization) might use a common mechanism. To explore this in detail, we fitted multiplicative and additive models of attention to our data. In one class of models, attention contributed to normalization mechanisms, whereas in a different class of models it did not. Model selection based on Akaike's and on Bayesian information criteria demonstrated that in most cells the effects of attention were best described by models where attention did not contribute to normalization mechanisms. This demonstrates that attentional influences on neuronal responses in primary visual cortex often bypass normalization mechanisms.
- Published
- 2015
23. An open resource for nonhuman primate imaging
- Author
-
Matthew F. S. Rushworth, Lei Ai, Jennifer Nacef, Doris Y. Tsao, Michael P. Milham, John H. Morrison, David A. Leopold, Wilbert Zarco, Charles E. Schroeder, Frank Q. Ye, Michael Ortiz Rios, Jakob Seidlitz, Alexander Thiele, Winrich A. Freiwald, Reza Rajimehr, Mark G. Baxter, Sabine Kastner, Jerome Sallet, Ting Xu, Christopher I. Petkov, Colline Poirier, Timothy D. Griffiths, Rogier B. Mars, Bonhwang Koo, Michael C. Schmid, Paula L. Croxson, Leslie G. Ungerleider, Jamie Nagy, Noam Harel, Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Mark A. Pinsk, Ravi S. Menon, Adam Messinger, Stefan Everling, Brian E. Russ, Christienne G. Damatac, Benjamin Jung, Essa Yacoub, Fabien Balezeau, and Daniel S. Margulies
- Subjects
medicine.diagnostic_test ,biology ,Computer science ,05 social sciences ,Magnetic resonance imaging ,Macaque ,Data science ,050105 experimental psychology ,Nonhuman primate ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Resource (project management) ,Neuroimaging ,biology.animal ,medicine ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Non-human primate neuroimaging is a rapidly growing area of research that promises to transform and scale translational and cross-species comparative neuroscience.Unfortunately, the technological and methodological advances of the past two decades have outpaced the accrual of data, which is particularly challenging given the relatively few centers that have the necessary facilities and capabilities. The PRIMate Data Exchange (PRIME-DE) addresses this challenge by aggregating independently acquired non-human primate magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) datasets and openly sharing them via the International Neuroimaging Data-sharing Initiative (INDI). Here, we present the rationale, design and procedures for the PRIME-DE consortium, as well as the initial release, consisting of 13 independent data collections aggregated across 11 sites (total = 98 macaque monkeys). We also outline the unique pitfalls and challenges that should be considered in the analysis of the non-human primate MRI datasets, including providing automated quality assessment of the contributed datasets.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Insights to HIV-1 coreceptor usage by estimating HLA adaptation with Bayesian generalized linear mixed models.
- Author
-
Anna Hake, Anja Germann, Corena de Beer, Alexander Thielen, Martin Däumer, Wolfgang Preiser, Hagen von Briesen, and Nico Pfeifer
- Subjects
Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
The mechanisms triggering the human immunodeficiency virus type I (HIV-1) to switch the coreceptor usage from CCR5 to CXCR4 during the course of infection are not entirely understood. While low CD4+ T cell counts are associated with CXCR4 usage, a predominance of CXCR4 usage with still high CD4+ T cell counts remains puzzling. Here, we explore the hypothesis that viral adaptation to the human leukocyte antigen (HLA) complex, especially to the HLA class II alleles, contributes to the coreceptor switch. To this end, we sequence the viral gag and env protein with corresponding HLA class I and II alleles of a new cohort of 312 treatment-naive, subtype C, chronically-infected HIV-1 patients from South Africa. To estimate HLA adaptation, we develop a novel computational approach using Bayesian generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs). Our model allows to consider the entire HLA repertoire without restricting the model to pre-learned HLA-polymorphisms. In addition, we correct for phylogenetic relatedness of the viruses within the model itself to account for founder effects. Using our model, we observe that CXCR4-using variants are more adapted than CCR5-using variants (p-value = 1.34e-2). Additionally, adapted CCR5-using variants have a significantly lower predicted false positive rate (FPR) by the geno2pheno[coreceptor] tool compared to the non-adapted CCR5-using variants (p-value = 2.21e-2), where a low FPR is associated with CXCR4 usage. Consequently, estimating HLA adaptation can be an asset in predicting not only coreceptor usage, but also an approaching coreceptor switch in CCR5-using variants. We propose the usage of Bayesian GLMMs for modeling virus-host adaptation in general.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Attention-Induced Variance and Noise Correlation Reduction in Macaque V1 Is Mediated by NMDA Receptors
- Author
-
Jose L. Herrero, Mehdi Sanayei, Alexander Thiele, and Marc Alwin Gieselmann
- Subjects
Male ,Sensory processing ,Neuroscience(all) ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Field Dependence-Independence ,Macaque ,Receptors, N-Methyl-D-Aspartate ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,Glutamatergic ,0302 clinical medicine ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Animals ,Attention ,Evoked Potentials ,030304 developmental biology ,Visual Cortex ,0303 health sciences ,Analysis of Variance ,biology ,Noise (signal processing) ,General Neuroscience ,Brain Waves ,Macaca mulatta ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,NMDA receptor ,Analysis of variance ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Ionotropic effect - Abstract
Summary Attention improves perception by affecting different aspects of the neuronal code. It enhances firing rates, it reduces firing rate variability and noise correlations of neurons, and it alters the strength of oscillatory activity. Attention-induced rate enhancement in striate cortex requires cholinergic mechanisms. The neuropharmacological mechanisms responsible for attention-induced variance and noise correlation reduction or those supporting changes in oscillatory activity are unknown. We show that ionotropic glutamatergic receptor activation is required for attention-induced rate variance, noise correlation, and LFP gamma power reduction in macaque V1, but not for attention-induced rate modulations. NMDA receptors mediate attention-induced variance reduction and attention-induced noise correlation reduction. Our results demonstrate that attention improves sensory processing by a variety of mechanisms that are dissociable at the receptor level., Highlights • NMDA receptors aid variance and covariance reduction • Cholinergic and NMDA receptors serve different functions in attentional modulation • Attention affects second-order statistics of neuronal activity in V1 • NMDA and AMPA receptor blockade oppositely affect V1 gamma frequency oscillations, Attention improves neuronal processing by increasing response strength and fidelity. Herrero et al. show that NMDA receptor availability is selectively required for increased response fidelity. The study isolates attentional mechanisms at the receptor level.
- Published
- 2013
26. Separable Codes for Attention and Luminance Contrast in the Primary Visual Cortex
- Author
-
Jasper Poort, Arezoo Pooresmaeili, Alexander Thiele, Pieter R. Roelfsema, ANS - Amsterdam Neuroscience, Biomedical Engineering and Physics, Integrative Neurophysiology, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam - Attention & Cognition, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN), and Hubrecht Institute for Developmental Biology and Stem Cell Research
- Subjects
Male ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Photic Stimulation ,Population ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Macaque ,Contrast Sensitivity ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Premovement neuronal activity ,Animals ,Attention ,Visual Pathways ,education ,Visual Cortex ,Neurons ,education.field_of_study ,biology ,General Neuroscience ,Articles ,Macaca mulatta ,Electrodes, Implanted ,Electrophysiology ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The visual system encodes the features of visual stimuli as well as their behavioral relevance. Stimuli with a high luminance contrast evoke more activity in the visual cortex than stimuli with a low contrast. At the same time, attended stimuli evoke more activity than nonattended stimuli. There is a debate about how visual features and attention jointly determine neuronal activity in the visual cortex. Some studies suggested that attention increases apparent contrast (Reynolds et al., 2000), others that attention amplifies responses by a constant factor (Williford and Maunsell, 2006), and yet others that attention and contrast have largely additive effects (Buracas and Boynton, 2007; Thiele et al., 2009). The influence of attention on contrast sensitivity differs between neurons, raising the possibility that attention and contrast could be coded conjointly in a population of neurons. Here we investigate this possibility by recording neuronal activity at multiple sites in the primary visual cortex of macaque monkeys using multielectrode recording techniques and support vector machines to decode attended stimuli as well as stimulus contrast. We find that many, but not all, V1 neurons are influenced by attention and that the effects of attention and contrast are additive on average. Stimulus contrast can be decoded from neuronal responses not strongly modulated by attention, whereas the attended stimulus can be decoded as the difference in activity of cells that are influenced by attention and cells that are not. The success of the approach suggests that visual attention and stimulus contrast are represented by largely separable codes. Copyright © 2010 the authors.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Attention – oscillations and neuropharmacology
- Author
-
Gustavo Deco and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Postsynaptic Current ,Sensory system ,Review Article ,Stimulus (physiology) ,modelling ,03 medical and health sciences ,Glutamatergic ,0302 clinical medicine ,Rhythm ,Neuropharmacology ,neuronal coherence ,Animals ,Humans ,Attention ,Cognitive behaviour ,030304 developmental biology ,Neurons ,0303 health sciences ,General Neuroscience ,Brain ,macaque monkey ,spatial integration ,Macaca mulatta ,acetylcholine ,Cholinergic ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,N-methyl-d-aspartate - Abstract
Attention is a rich psychological and neurobiological construct that influences almost all aspects of cognitive behaviour. It enables enhanced processing of behaviourally relevant stimuli at the expense of irrelevant stimuli. At the cellular level, rhythmic synchronization at local and long-range spatial scales complements the attention-induced firing rate changes of neurons. The former is hypothesized to enable efficient communication between neuronal ensembles tuned to spatial and featural aspects of the attended stimulus. Recent modelling studies suggest that the rhythmic synchronization in the gamma range may be mediated by a fine balance between N-methyl-D-aspartate and alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate postsynaptic currents, whereas other studies have highlighted the possible contribution of the neuromodulator acetylcholine. This review summarizes some recent modelling and experimental studies investigating mechanisms of attention in sensory areas and discusses possibilities of how glutamatergic and cholinergic systems could contribute to increased processing abilities at the cellular and network level during states of top-down attention.
- Published
- 2009
28. A perceptual pitch boundary in a non-human primate
- Author
-
Roy D. Patterson, Simon Baumann, Olivier Joly, Timothy D. Griffiths, Colline Poirier, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
Auditory perception ,Speech recognition ,media_common.quotation_subject ,lcsh:BF1-990 ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,psychophysics ,Perception ,Psychophysics ,otorhinolaryngologic diseases ,Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Original Research Article ,General Psychology ,media_common ,harmonic sounds ,pitch discrimination ,monkey model ,05 social sciences ,humanities ,auditory perception ,lcsh:Psychology ,Pitch Discrimination ,Harmonic ,Repetition (music) ,Percept ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Pitch (Music) - Abstract
Pitch is an auditory percept critical to the perception of music and speech, and for these harmonic sounds, pitch is closely related to the repetition rate of the acoustic wave. This paper reports a test of the assumption that non-human primates and especially rhesus monkeys perceive the pitch of these harmonic sounds much as humans do. A new procedure was developed to train macaques to discriminate the pitch of harmonic sounds and thereby demonstrate that the lower limit for pitch perception in macaques is close to 30 Hz, as it is in humans. Moreover, when the phases of successive harmonics are alternated to cause a pseudo-doubling of the repetition rate, the lower pitch boundary in macaques decreases substantially, as it does in humans. The results suggest that both species use neural firing times to discriminate pitch, at least for sounds with relatively low repetition rates.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Suppressive Lateral Interactions at Parafoveal Representations in Primary Visual Cortex
- Author
-
Alexander Thiele, Matthew W. Self, Pieter R. Roelfsema, Arezoo Pooresmaeili, Jose L. Herrero, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Biomedical Engineering and Physics, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN), Hubrecht Institute for Developmental Biology and Stem Cell Research, Integrative Neurophysiology, and Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam - Attention & Cognition
- Subjects
Fovea Centralis ,Visual perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Stimulus (physiology) ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,Orientation ,medicine ,Animals ,030304 developmental biology ,media_common ,Visual Cortex ,Neurons ,0303 health sciences ,Analysis of Variance ,General Neuroscience ,Fovea centralis ,Perceptual salience ,Macaca mulatta ,Target Response ,Electrophysiology ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Visual Perception ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Photic Stimulation ,psychological phenomena and processes - Abstract
The perceptual salience and visibility of image elements is influenced by other elements in their vicinity. The perceptual effect of image elements on an adjacent target element depends on their relative orientation. Collinear flanking elements usually improve sensitivity for the target element, whereas orthogonal elements have a weaker effect. It is believed that the collinear flankers exert these effects through lateral interactions between neurons in the primary visual cortex (area V1), but the precise mechanisms underlying these contextual interactions remain unknown. Here, we directly examined this question by recording the effects of flankers on the responses of V1 neurons at parafoveal representations while monkeys performed a fixation task or a contrast detection task. We found, unexpectedly, that collinear flankers reduce the monkeys' perceptual sensitivity for a central target element. This behavioral effect was explained by a flanker-induced increase in the activity of V1 neurons in the absence of the central target stimulus, which reduced the amplitude of the target response. Our results indicate that the dominant effect of collinear flankers in parafoveal vision is suppression and suggest that these suppressive effects are caused by a decrease in the dynamic range of neurons coding the central target. Copyright © 2010 the authors.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Additive effects of attention and stimulus contrast in primary visual cortex
- Author
-
Arezoo Pooresmaeili, Louise S. Delicato, Pieter R. Roelfsema, Alexander Thiele, Jose L. Herrero, Hubrecht Institute for Developmental Biology and Stem Cell Research, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience (NIN), Integrative Neurophysiology, Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam - Attention & Cognition, Amsterdam Neuroscience, and Biomedical Engineering and Physics
- Subjects
Visual perception ,Directed attention fatigue ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Stimulus (physiology) ,attentional modulation ,Brain mapping ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,medicine ,Animals ,Premovement neuronal activity ,Attention ,Second-order stimulus ,Visual Cortex ,030304 developmental biology ,Brain Mapping ,contrast sensitivity ,0303 health sciences ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,response gain ,Articles ,contrast gain ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Visual Perception ,area V1 ,Evoked Potentials, Visual ,Macaca ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Previous studies have proposed a variety of mechanisms by which attention influences neuronal activity. Here we investigated the mechanisms of attention in the striate cortex of monkeys performing a spatial or an object-based attention task at various stimulus contrasts and compared neuronal contrast response functions with and without attention. Our data are best described by an “additive” interaction: The influence of attention on the neuronal response is relatively independent of the stimulus contrast, at least when the stimulus has enough contrast to become visible. This shows that attention adds to the neuronal responses in a largely contrast invariant manner. These data support recent functional magnetic resonance imaging studies and suggest that feedback from higher areas exerts a constant attentional drive that is mostly task not stimulus driven.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Modelling local field potential features during network gamma oscillations
- Author
-
Mark O. Cunningham, Marcus Kaiser, Xing Chen, Miles A. Whittington, Matthew Ainsworth, Alwin Gieselmann, Alexander Thiele, Richard Tomsett, and Mehdi Sanayei
- Subjects
0303 health sciences ,Artificial neural network ,Interneuron ,Computer science ,Oscillation ,General Neuroscience ,Spectral density ,Local field potential ,Signal ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Basket cell ,Poster Presentation ,medicine ,Soma ,Biological system ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,030304 developmental biology - Abstract
While the physics of local field potential (LFP) generation are well-established, the complexity of neural network dynamics means that interpreting LFP measurements in terms of the underlying neural activity is very difficult [1]. Recent studies have therefore investigated forward models of the LFP: calculating the LFP due to known arrangements of neuronal current sources [1]. We use this approach to study the spatio-temporal features of the LFP recorded during persistent gamma-oscillations in vitro. The mechanism by which this activity regime is generated is well-known [2], and many multi-electrode array recordings are available that allow us to compare the spatio-temporal properties of the experimentally measured and simulated LFPs. For simplicity, we implemented a model of a neocortical slice containing only layer 2/3, using our VERTEX simulation tool. The model contained a gamma oscillation-generating region surrounded by regions that did not participate in the gamma oscillation (i.e. the neurons in these regions fired randomly). These enforced conditions allowed us to study the spatial profile of the LFP from differently sized gamma oscillation-generating patches, while minimising the number of variables we changed between simulations. Additionally, we could make observations about properties of the LFP during random activity in the network. We describe three key results from our simulations. Firstly, we found that perisomatic synaptic currents on pyramidal neurons resulting from basket interneuron firing dominate the LFP during gamma oscillations, in agreement with recent experimental results [3,4]. We predict that basket interneurons will also contribute the majority of the LFP signal during random, uncorrelated activity because of the location of their synapses at pyramidal neuron somas. This contribution is amplified during oscillatory activity because of increased basket cell synchrony relative to other neuron types. Secondly, we investigated how gamma-frequency power from a localised gammagenerating region in the model spread due to volume conduction. We found that the spatial spread of the oscillation in the LFP increased above and below the neurons’ somadepth, and depended on the level of the random surround activity. Finally, we investigated frequency scaling in the LFP power spectrum across space. We showed that LFPs at the level of the pyramidal cells’ apical dendrites exhibit a low-pass filtering effect (as predicted previously [5]), which is absent from LFPs recorded at the soma level because of the relative dominance and localisation of the basket cell input. We confirmed that this matched the frequency scaling in our comparison experimental data recorded in macaque neocortical slices, and, based on the frequency characteristics in our model, propose that the experimentally recorded LFP could contain contributions from spiking activity at frequencies down to, and perhaps below, 100 Hz.
- Published
- 2014
32. Robustness, sensitivity and reproducibility of eDNA metabarcoding as an environmental biomonitoring tool in coastal salmon aquaculture – An inter-laboratory study
- Author
-
Verena Dully, Heinrich Balliet, Larissa Frühe, Martin Däumer, Alexander Thielen, Sheena Gallie, Iain Berrill, and Thorsten Stoeck
- Subjects
Benthic bacterial communities ,eDNA metabarcoding ,Environmental compliance monitoring ,Infaunal Quality Index ,Macroinvertebrates ,Salmon aquaculture ,Ecology ,QH540-549.5 - Abstract
Environmental DNA metabarcoding of benthic bacterial communities emerged as a very powerful technology to assess environmental disturbance effects of coastal salmon aquaculture. A prerequisite for the implementation of this approach into compliance monitoring regulations is its robustness and reproducibility of obtained results. In the framework of regular compliance monitoring of a Scottish salmon farm, we therefore collected sediment samples along a transect from the salmon cages to reference sites in duplicates (biological replicates). Aliquots of both biological replicate samples were then processed by two different laboratories using independently the same eDNA metabarcoding protocol (technical replicates). Measures of alpha diversity and beta diversity, as well as taxonomic profiles of benthic bacterial communities were highly congruent among technical replicates, which even showed less variations then the biological replicates that were processed within each laboratory. Both technical replicate datasets identified the same bacterial indicator taxon groups and ASVs that are characteristic for the environmental quality (EQ) categories to which each of the samples was assigned based on traditional macroinvertebrate biomonitoring of the same samples. In a supervised machine learning (SML) approach, we could classify all individual samples from one technical replicate dataset into the correct EQ category using all samples of the other technical replicate dataset as a training dataset for the SML algorithm. We conclude that eDNA metabarcoding is sufficiently robust that different laboratories come to the same conclusions regarding officially regulated action criteria for environmental impact assessments in salmon aquaculture.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Impact of the HIV-1 genetic background and HIV-1 population size on the evolution of raltegravir resistance
- Author
-
Axel Fun, Thomas Leitner, Linos Vandekerckhove, Martin Däumer, Alexander Thielen, Bernd Buchholz, Andy I. M. Hoepelman, Elizabeth H. Gisolf, Pauline J. Schipper, Annemarie M. J. Wensing, and Monique Nijhuis
- Subjects
Immunologic diseases. Allergy ,RC581-607 - Abstract
Abstract Background Emergence of resistance against integrase inhibitor raltegravir in human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1) patients is generally associated with selection of one of three signature mutations: Y143C/R, Q148K/H/R or N155H, representing three distinct resistance pathways. The mechanisms that drive selection of a specific pathway are still poorly understood. We investigated the impact of the HIV-1 genetic background and population dynamics on the emergence of raltegravir resistance. Using deep sequencing we analyzed the integrase coding sequence (CDS) in longitudinal samples from five patients who initiated raltegravir plus optimized background therapy at viral loads > 5000 copies/ml. To investigate the role of the HIV-1 genetic background we created recombinant viruses containing the viral integrase coding region from pre-raltegravir samples from two patients in whom raltegravir resistance developed through different pathways. The in vitro selections performed with these recombinant viruses were designed to mimic natural population bottlenecks. Results Deep sequencing analysis of the viral integrase CDS revealed that the virological response to raltegravir containing therapy inversely correlated with the relative amount of unique sequence variants that emerged suggesting diversifying selection during drug pressure. In 4/5 patients multiple signature mutations representing different resistance pathways were observed. Interestingly, the resistant population can consist of a single resistant variant that completely dominates the population but also of multiple variants from different resistance pathways that coexist in the viral population. We also found evidence for increased diversification after stronger bottlenecks. In vitro selections with low viral titers, mimicking population bottlenecks, revealed that both recombinant viruses and HXB2 reference virus were able to select mutations from different resistance pathways, although typically only one resistance pathway emerged in each individual culture. Conclusions The generation of a specific raltegravir resistant variant is not predisposed in the genetic background of the viral integrase CDS. Typically, in the early phases of therapy failure the sequence space is explored and multiple resistance pathways emerge and then compete for dominance which frequently results in a switch of the dominant population over time towards the fittest variant or even multiple variants of similar fitness that can coexist in the viral population.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Analysis of Human Gut Microbiota Composition Associated to the Presence of Commensal and Pathogen Microorganisms in Côte d’Ivoire
- Author
-
Veronica Di Cristanziano, Fedja Farowski, Federica Berrilli, Maristella Santoro, David Di Cave, Christophe Glé, Martin Daeumer, Alexander Thielen, Maike Wirtz, Rolf Kaiser, Kirsten Alexandra Eberhardt, Maria J. G. T. Vehreschild, and Rossella D’Alfonso
- Subjects
intestinal co-infection ,biotype ,virus ,bacteria ,G. duodenalis ,Blastocystis ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Background: The human gut microbiota is a microbial ecosystem contributing to the maintenance of host health with functions related to immune and metabolic aspects. Relations between microbiota and enteric pathogens in sub-Saharan Africa are scarcely investigated. The present study explored gut microbiota composition associated to the presence of common enteric pathogens and commensal microorganisms, e.g., Blastocystis and Entamoeba species, in children and adults from semi-urban and non-urban localities in Côte d’Ivoire. Methods: Seventy-six stool samples were analyzed for microbiota composition by 16S rRDNA sequencing. The presence of adeno-, entero-, parechoviruses, bacterial and protozoal pathogens, Blastocystis, and commensal Entamoeba species, was analyzed by different molecular assays. Results: Twelve individuals resulted negative for any tested microorganisms, 64 subjects were positive for one or more microorganisms. Adenovirus, enterovirus, enterotoxigenic Escherichia coli (ETEC), and Blastocystis were frequently detected. Conclusions: The bacterial composition driven by Prevotellaceae and Ruminococcaceae confirmed the biotype related to the traditional dietary and cooking practices in low-income countries. Clear separation in UniFrac distance in subjects co-harboring Entamoeba hartmanni and Blastocystis was evidenced. Alpha diversity variation in negative control group versus only Blastocystis positive suggested its possible regulatory contribution on intestinal microbiota. Pathogenic bacteria and virus did not affect the positive outcome of co-harbored Blastocystis.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Emergent HIV-1 Drug Resistance Mutations Were Not Present at Low-Frequency at Baseline in Non-Nucleoside Reverse Transcriptase Inhibitor-Treated Subjects in the STaR Study
- Author
-
Danielle P. Porter, Martin Daeumer, Alexander Thielen, Silvia Chang, Ross Martin, Cal Cohen, Michael D. Miller, and Kirsten L. White
- Subjects
rilpivirine ,efavirenz ,resistance ,virologic failure ,minority variants ,Microbiology ,QR1-502 - Abstract
At Week 96 of the Single-Tablet Regimen (STaR) study, more treatment-naïve subjects that received rilpivirine/emtricitabine/tenofovir DF (RPV/FTC/TDF) developed resistance mutations compared to those treated with efavirenz (EFV)/FTC/TDF by population sequencing. Furthermore, more RPV/FTC/TDF-treated subjects with baseline HIV-1 RNA >100,000 copies/mL developed resistance compared to subjects with baseline HIV-1 RNA ≤100,000 copies/mL. Here, deep sequencing was utilized to assess the presence of pre-existing low-frequency variants in subjects with and without resistance development in the STaR study. Deep sequencing (Illumina MiSeq) was performed on baseline and virologic failure samples for all subjects analyzed for resistance by population sequencing during the clinical study (n = 33), as well as baseline samples from control subjects with virologic response (n = 118). Primary NRTI or NNRTI drug resistance mutations present at low frequency (≥2% to 20%) were detected in 6.6% of baseline samples by deep sequencing, all of which occurred in control subjects. Deep sequencing results were generally consistent with population sequencing but detected additional primary NNRTI and NRTI resistance mutations at virologic failure in seven samples. HIV-1 drug resistance mutations emerging while on RPV/FTC/TDF or EFV/FTC/TDF treatment were not present at low frequency at baseline in the STaR study.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. A novel electrode–pipette design for simultaneous recording of extracellular spikes and iontophoretic drug application in awake behaving monkeys
- Author
-
Alexander Thiele, Louise S. Delicato, Mark Roberts, and Mark A. Gieselmann
- Subjects
Materials science ,Antimetabolites ,Neuroscience(all) ,Scopolamine ,Muscarinic Antagonists ,Article ,law.invention ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,law ,Extracellular ,Animals ,Cell isolation ,Electrodes ,Visual Cortex ,030304 developmental biology ,0303 health sciences ,Iontophoresis ,General Neuroscience ,Pipette ,Haplorhini ,Drug application ,Glass electrode ,Acetylcholine ,3. Good health ,Electrophysiology ,Pharmaceutical Preparations ,Electrode ,cardiovascular system ,Fluorouracil ,Extracellular Space ,Neuroscience ,Craniotomy ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Macaque - Abstract
We developed a novel design of an electrode–pipette combination (EPC) which allows access to brain structures in awake behaving primates without the need for guide tubes or to mechanically open the dura prior to electrode insertion. The EPC consists of an etched tungsten in glass electrode flanked by two pipettes which allow for local and highly controlled iontophoretic administration of neuroactive substances. These EPCs have excellent single cell isolation properties and are sturdy enough to penetrate the primate dura for up to 8 weeks following either a craniotomy or a dura scrape (i.e. even after substantial built up of fibrous scar tissue). We show that the EPCs can be used to selectively manipulate the cholinergic system in primate V1 during passive fixation and while animals perform an attentionally demanding task.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Characterisation of the BOLD response time course at different levels of the auditory pathway in non-human primates
- Author
-
Alexander Thiele, Simon Baumann, David Hunter, Adrian Rees, Timothy D. Griffiths, and Li Sun
- Subjects
Auditory perception ,Inferior colliculus ,Male ,Visual perception ,Auditory Pathways ,Time Factors ,genetic structures ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Auditory cortex ,Macaque ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Auditory system ,Animals ,030304 developmental biology ,Auditory Cortex ,0303 health sciences ,biology ,Geniculate Bodies ,Medial geniculate body ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Inferior Colliculi ,Oxygen ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Neurology ,Auditory Perception ,Macaca ,Psychology ,Noise ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Non-human-primate fMRI is becoming increasingly recognised as the missing link between the widely applied methods of human imaging and intracortical animal electrophysiology. A crucial requirement for the optimal application of this method is the precise knowledge of the time course of the Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) signal. We mapped the BOLD signal time course in the inferior colliculus (IC), medial geniculate body (MGB) and in tonotopically defined fields in the auditory cortex of two macaques. The results show little differences in the BOLD-signal time courses within the auditory pathway. However, we observed systematic differences in the magnitude of the change in the BOLD signal with significantly stronger signal changes in field A1 of the auditory cortex compared to field R. The measured time course of the signal was in good agreement with similar studies in human auditory cortex but showed considerable differences to data reported from macaque visual cortex. Consistent with the studies in humans we measured a peak in the BOLD response around 4 s after the onset of 2-s broadband noise stimuli while previous studies recorded from the primary visual cortex of the same species reported the earliest peaks to short visual stimuli several seconds later. The comparison of our results with previous studies does not support differences in haemodynamic responses within the auditory system between human and non-human primates. Furthermore, the data will aid optimal design of future auditory fMRI studies in non-human primates.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. A Follow-Up of the Multicenter Collaborative Study on HIV-1 Drug Resistance and Tropism Testing Using 454 Ultra Deep Pyrosequencing.
- Author
-
Elizabeth P St John, Birgitte B Simen, Gregory S Turenchalk, Michael S Braverman, Isabella Abbate, Jeroen Aerssens, Olivier Bouchez, Christian Gabriel, Jacques Izopet, Karolin Meixenberger, Francesca Di Giallonardo, Ralph Schlapbach, Roger Paredes, James Sakwa, Gudrun G Schmitz-Agheguian, Alexander Thielen, Martin Victor, Karin J Metzner, Martin P Däumer, and HIV-1 Alpha Study Group
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Ultra deep sequencing is of increasing use not only in research but also in diagnostics. For implementation of ultra deep sequencing assays in clinical laboratories for routine diagnostics, intra- and inter-laboratory testing are of the utmost importance.A multicenter study was conducted to validate an updated assay design for 454 Life Sciences' GS FLX Titanium system targeting protease/reverse transcriptase (RTP) and env (V3) regions to identify HIV-1 drug-resistance mutations and determine co-receptor use with high sensitivity. The study included 30 HIV-1 subtype B and 6 subtype non-B samples with viral titers (VT) of 3,940-447,400 copies/mL, two dilution series (52,129-1,340 and 25,130-734 copies/mL), and triplicate samples. Amplicons spanning PR codons 10-99, RT codons 1-251 and the entire V3 region were generated using barcoded primers. Analysis was performed using the GS Amplicon Variant Analyzer and geno2pheno for tropism. For comparison, population sequencing was performed using the ViroSeq HIV-1 genotyping system.The median sequencing depth across the 11 sites was 1,829 reads per position for RTP (IQR 592-3,488) and 2,410 for V3 (IQR 786-3,695). 10 preselected drug resistant variants were measured across sites and showed high inter-laboratory correlation across all sites with data (P20% were missed, variants 2-10% were detected at most sites (even at low VT), and variants 1-2% were detected by some sites. All mutations detected by population sequencing were also detected by UDS.This assay design results in an accurate and reproducible approach to analyze HIV-1 mutant spectra, even at variant frequencies well below those routinely detectable by population sequencing.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. HIV-1 tropism testing in subjects achieving undetectable HIV-1 RNA: diagnostic accuracy, viral evolution and compartmentalization.
- Author
-
Christian Pou, Francisco M Codoñer, Alexander Thielen, Rocío Bellido, Susana Pérez-Álvarez, Cecilia Cabrera, Judith Dalmau, Marta Curriu, Yolanda Lie, Marc Noguera-Julian, Jordi Puig, Javier Martínez-Picado, Julià Blanco, Eoin Coakley, Martin Däumer, Bonaventura Clotet, and Roger Paredes
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
BACKGROUND: Technically, HIV-1 tropism can be evaluated in plasma or peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs). However, only tropism testing of plasma HIV-1 has been validated as a tool to predict virological response to CCR5 antagonists in clinical trials. The preferable tropism testing strategy in subjects with undetectable HIV-1 viremia, in whom plasma tropism testing is not feasible, remains uncertain. METHODS & RESULTS: We designed a proof-of-concept study including 30 chronically HIV-1-infected individuals who achieved HIV-1 RNA
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Added value of deep sequencing relative to population sequencing in heavily pre-treated HIV-1-infected subjects.
- Author
-
Francisco M Codoñer, Christian Pou, Alexander Thielen, Federico García, Rafael Delgado, David Dalmau, Miguel Álvarez-Tejado, Lidia Ruiz, Bonaventura Clotet, and Roger Paredes
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
To explore the potential of deep HIV-1 sequencing for adding clinically relevant information relative to viral population sequencing in heavily pre-treated HIV-1-infected subjects.In a proof-of-concept study, deep sequencing was compared to population sequencing in HIV-1-infected individuals with previous triple-class virological failure who also developed virologic failure to deep salvage therapy including, at least, darunavir, tipranavir, etravirine or raltegravir. Viral susceptibility was inferred before salvage therapy initiation and at virological failure using deep and population sequencing genotypes interpreted with the HIVdb, Rega and ANRS algorithms. The threshold level for mutant detection with deep sequencing was 1%.7 subjects with previous exposure to a median of 15 antiretrovirals during a median of 13 years were included. Deep salvage therapy included darunavir, tipranavir, etravirine or raltegravir in 4, 2, 2 and 5 subjects, respectively. Self-reported treatment adherence was adequate in 4 and partial in 2; one individual underwent treatment interruption during follow-up. Deep sequencing detected all mutations found by population sequencing and identified additional resistance mutations in all but one individual, predominantly after virological failure to deep salvage therapy. Additional genotypic information led to consistent decreases in predicted susceptibility to etravirine, efavirenz, nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitors and indinavir in 2, 1, 2 and 1 subject, respectively. Deep sequencing data did not consistently modify the susceptibility predictions achieved with population sequencing for darunavir, tipranavir or raltegravir.In this subset of heavily pre-treated individuals, deep sequencing improved the assessment of genotypic resistance to etravirine, but did not consistently provide additional information on darunavir, tipranavir or raltegravir susceptibility. These data may inform the design of future studies addressing the clinical value of minority drug-resistant variants in treatment-experienced subjects.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. V3 loop sequence space analysis suggests different evolutionary patterns of CCR5- and CXCR4-tropic HIV.
- Author
-
Katarzyna Bozek, Alexander Thielen, Saleta Sierra, Rolf Kaiser, and Thomas Lengauer
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
The V3 loop of human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1) is critical for coreceptor binding and is the main determinant of which of the cellular coreceptors, CCR5 or CXCR4, the virus uses for cell entry. The aim of this study is to provide a large-scale data driven analysis of HIV-1 coreceptor usage with respect to the V3 loop evolution and to characterize CCR5- and CXCR4-tropic viral phenotypes previously studied in small- and medium-scale settings. We use different sequence similarity measures, phylogenetic and clustering methods in order to analyze the distribution in sequence space of roughly 1000 V3 loop sequences and their tropism phenotypes. This analysis affords a means of characterizing those sequences that are misclassified by several sequence-based coreceptor prediction methods, as well as predicting the coreceptor using the location of the sequence in sequence space and of relating this location to the CD4(+) T-cell count of the patient. We support previous findings that the usage of CCR5 is correlated with relatively high sequence conservation whereas CXCR4-tropic viruses spread over larger regions in sequence space. The incorrectly predicted sequences are mostly located in regions in which their phenotype represents the minority or in close vicinity of regions dominated by the opposite phenotype. Nevertheless, the location of the sequence in sequence space can be used to improve the accuracy of the prediction of the coreceptor usage. Sequences from patients with high CD4(+) T-cell counts are relatively highly conserved as compared to those of immunosuppressed patients. Our study thus supports hypotheses of an association of immune system depletion with an increase in V3 loop sequence variability and with the escape of the viral sequence to distant parts of the sequence space.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Decoding spiking activity in V4, but not V1, correlates with behavioural performance in perceptual learning task
- Author
-
Scott C. Lowe, Xing Chen, Mark C. W. van Rossum, Stefano Panzeri, and Alexander Thiele
- Subjects
0303 health sciences ,Neural correlates of consciousness ,Visual perception ,biology ,Implanted electrodes ,Computer science ,General Neuroscience ,Speech recognition ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Macaque ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perceptual learning ,biology.animal ,Poster Presentation ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Decoding methods ,030304 developmental biology - Abstract
When an individual repeatedly performs a simple sensory task, such as discrimination between similar visual stimuli, performance gradually increases until it asymptotically approaches saturation. This phenomenon is known as perceptual learning, however the neural correlates of this process are not well understood. Here we consider the results of an experiment in perceptual learning of visual contrast discrimination in cortical areas V1 and V4. In the experiment, a Gabor (V4 recordings) or sinusoidal (V1 recordings) stimulus, with a contrast chosen at random from a set of 14 possibilities, was presented to a macaque monkey. Recordings were made using chronically implanted electrodes in a multi-unit array. The animal was tasked with determining whether the contrast was higher or lower than a control stimulus of 30% contrast, and a correct response was met with a water reward. Experimentation continued for ~20 days until performance saturated. A population-wide linear-discriminant decoding technique based on the mean firing rates during 500ms of stimulus presentation from ~20 channels in V4 was found to achieve similar levels of performance at completing the discrimination task, and to yield a similar rate of improvement in performance, as the monkey's behavioural responses. However, the same analysis in V1 found decoder performance was the same throughout the learning process, despite the animal's improvement in performance. This suggests contrast information present in V1 remains consistent throughout learning, whilst V4 improves in its ability to readout this information from V1.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.