41 results on '"Brehmer, Yvonne"'
Search Results
2. Capability in Research on Cognition and Well-being in Ageing and Retirement
- Author
-
Johansson, Boo, Berg, Anne Ingeborg, Bjälkebring, Pär, Praetorius Björk, Marcus, Brehmer, Yvonne, Buratti, Sandra, Hansson, Isabelle, Hassing, Linda, Henning, Georg, Kivi, Marie, König, Stefanie, Lindwall, Magnus, Rolstad, Sindre, Skoog, Johan, Thorvaldsson, Valgeir, Elena Zulka, Linn, Powell, Jason L., Series Editor, Chen, Sheying, Series Editor, Falk Erhag, Hanna, editor, Lagerlöf Nilsson, Ulrika, editor, Rydberg Sterner, Therese, editor, and Skoog, Ingmar, editor
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Expecting Relocation to a Nursing Home: Longitudinal Links with Functional Limitations, Self-Rated Health, and Life Satisfaction.
- Author
-
Verspeek, Emmie A. M., Brehmer, Yvonne, Jongerling, Joran, Hering, Alexandra, and van Scheppingen, Manon A.
- Abstract
Introduction: Developing realistic expectations of future old age constitutes an adaptational process which facilitates the anticipation of and adjustment to challenges, such as relocation to a nursing home. Developing such expectations might minimize the negative impacts of relocation. This pre-registered study examined (1) to which extent lower levels and declines in health (i.e., functional limitations and self-rated health) and life satisfaction before relocation were associated with higher levels and increases in expectations to relocate and (2) to which extent higher expectations to relocate were associated with more positive changes in health and life satisfaction after relocation. Methods: Using data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS; 2006–2018), we selected older adults (aged 65 years and older) who relocated to a nursing home. We used latent growth curve models to assess the longitudinal links between self-reported measures of health, life satisfaction, and expectations to relocate to a nursing home from up to 7 years before (n = 1,048) until up to 5 years after relocation (n = 307). Results: As hypothesized, more functional limitations and lower self-rated health were related to higher expectations of relocation. Surprisingly, changes in expectations to relocate were not related to changes in health and life satisfaction before relocation. Moreover, expectations to relocate were not associated with changes in health and life satisfaction after relocation. Conclusion: The absence of a link between expectations to relocate to a nursing home with changes in health and well-being suggests that these expectations did not constitute adaptational processes before or after this transition. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. The Relationship Between Experiencing Childhood Trauma and Psychopathic Personality Traits: The Mediating Role of Insecure Attachment.
- Author
-
van Beeck, Manon, Bogaerts, Stefan, Sellbom, Martin, Somma, Antonella, Fossati, Andrea, Brehmer, Yvonne, Jankovic, Marija, and Garofalo, Carlo
- Subjects
INJURY complications ,WOUNDS & injuries ,STATISTICAL correlation ,ATTACHMENT behavior ,QUESTIONNAIRES ,DESCRIPTIVE statistics ,SURVEYS ,EMOTIONAL trauma ,PSYCHOANALYTIC theory ,INTERPERSONAL relations ,FACTOR analysis ,DATA analysis software ,ADVERSE childhood experiences ,PATHOLOGICAL psychology ,ANTISOCIAL personality disorders ,ADULTS - Abstract
While psychopathy has been a topic of interest in the psychological field for a long time, it is still unclear which developmental factors are related to its onset. Theories have suggested that experiencing trauma in childhood may contribute to the development of psychopathic traits. Experiencing trauma may also contribute to the development of an insecure attachment style, which in turn might cause the emergence of psychopathic traits. The current study investigated links between experiencing childhood trauma and the three psychopathy domains of the Extended Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy scale (E-LSRP; Egocentricity, Callousness, and Antisocial), and whether these links were mediated by the four insecure attachment dimensions of the Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Need for Approval, Preoccupation with Relationships, Relationships as Secondary, and Discomfort with Closeness), in a large sample of non-clinical participants (N = 2023, M
age = 32.7, 70.0% female). The results suggested that Relationships as Secondary, but not Discomfort with Closeness, positively mediated the relationship between childhood trauma and the three domains of psychopathy, while Need for Approval negatively mediated this relationship. Preoccupation with Relationships was a significant mediator in the links between childhood trauma and both the Egocentricity and Antisocial domains, but not the Callousness domain. The current findings suggested that there might be both shared and differential mechanisms linking childhood trauma and psychopathic traits through insecure attachment styles. These findings can guide further investigations in longitudinal research. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. The importance of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex for associative memory in older adults: A latent structural equation analysis
- Author
-
Brehmer, Yvonne, Nilsson, Jonna, Berggren, Rasmus, Schmiedek, Florian, and Lövdén, Martin
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Structure-function associations of successful associative encoding
- Author
-
Becker, Nina, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, Salami, Alireza, Laukka, Erika J., and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Association between personality traits, leisure activities, and cognitive levels and decline across 12 years in older adults.
- Author
-
Olaru, Gabriel, primary, Laukka, Erika J., additional, Dekhtyar, Serhiy, additional, Sarwary, Arian, additional, and Brehmer, Yvonne, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Stimulus material selection for the Dutch famous faces test for older adults
- Author
-
van den Elzen, Evi H. T., primary, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, Van Deun, Katrijn, additional, and Mark, Ruth E., additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Personality and limitations in instrumental activities of daily living in old age : Reciprocal associations across 12 years
- Author
-
Chereches, Flavia S., Brehmer, Yvonne, Olaru, Gabriel, Chereches, Flavia S., Brehmer, Yvonne, and Olaru, Gabriel
- Abstract
Personality traits have been reported to predict difficulties in performing instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) in old age, such as preparing meals or shopping. However, little is known about the reciprocal effects on personality. In this study, we examined bidirectional relationships between personality traits and the capacity to perform IADL using four waves of longitudinal data from 3540 older adults (aged 65 years and older) from the Health and Retirement Study. We applied a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model to separate between- and within-person effects across time and compared it to a traditional cross-lagged panel model. At the between-person level, higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness were associated with more IADL limitations. Within individuals across time, increases in neuroticism and decreases in conscientiousness and extraversion were associated with increases in IADL limitations 4 years later. In contrast, increases in IADL limitations only predicted increases in neuroticism and decreases in extraversion. These results indicate that some personality traits affect and are affected by limitations in functional capacities in old age. Results of the within-person model build a strong foundation for future personality interventions as a pathway to maintain high functioning in old age.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Stimulus material selection for the Dutch famous faces test for older adults
- Author
-
van den Elzen, Evi H. T., Brehmer, Yvonne, Van Deun, Katrijn, Mark, Ruth E., van den Elzen, Evi H. T., Brehmer, Yvonne, Van Deun, Katrijn, and Mark, Ruth E.
- Abstract
Worldwide, approximately 22% of all individuals aged 50 years and older are currently estimated to fall somewhere on the Alzheimer’s disease (AD) continuum, which can be roughly divided into preclinical AD, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and AD dementia. While episodic memory loss (among other aspects) is typically required for a diagnosis of AD dementia, MCI is said to have occurred when cognitive impairment (including memory loss) is worse than expected for the person’s age but not enough to be classified as dementia. On the other hand, preclinical AD can currently only be detected using biomarkers; clinical symptoms are not apparent using traditional neuropsychological tests. The main aim of the current paper was to explore the possibility of a test which could distinguish preclinical AD from normal aging. Recent scientific evidence suggests that the Famous Faces Test (FFT) could differentiate preclinical AD from normal aging up to 5 years before a clinical AD diagnosis. Problematic with existing FFTs is the selection of stimulus material. Faces famous in a specific country and a specific decade might not be equally famous for individuals in another country or indeed for people of different ages. The current article describes how famous faces were systematically selected and chosen for the Dutch older (60+) population using five steps. The goal was to design and develop short versions of the FFT for Dutch older adults of equivalent mean difficulty. In future work, these nine parallel versions will be necessary for (a) cross-sectional comparison as well as subsequent longitudinal assessment of cognitively normal and clinical groups and (b) creating personalized norms for the normal aged controls that could be used to compare performance within individuals with clinical diagnoses. The field needs a simple, cognitive test which can distinguish the earliest stages of the dementia continuum from normal aging.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Association Between Personality Traits, Leisure Activities, and Cognitive Levels and Decline Across 12 Years in Older Adults
- Author
-
Olaru, Gabriel, Laukka, Erika J., Dekhtyar, Serhiy, Sarwary, Arian, Brehmer, Yvonne, Olaru, Gabriel, Laukka, Erika J., Dekhtyar, Serhiy, Sarwary, Arian, and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Abstract
The engagement in cognitively stimulating activities has been found to be associated with slower rates of cognitive decline in old age. In which type of activities people engage in may depend on their personality traits, which thus might have an impact on later cognitive fitness. To study these potential links, we examined the associations between Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness; different types of leisure activities (e.g., social, mental, physical); and cognitive ability levels and decline in older adults. Analyses were based on a sample of young-old (60-72 years old; n = 1,609) and old-old (78 years or older; n = 1,085) adults from the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen, who participated in up to five repeated measurements of cognitive abilities spanning 12 years. We used latent growth curve models to estimate cognitive levels and decline, as well as the correlations with initial personality trait levels and leisure activity engagement. In both groups, lower Neuroticism, higher Extraversion, and higher Openness levels were moderately associated with stronger engagement in all types of activities. Lower Neuroticism, higher Extraversion, and a more activity lifestyle were weakly to moderately associated with slower cognitive decline in the old-old age group. There, personality traits and activities explained 9.3% of the variance in cognitive decline after controlling for age, sex, education, and chronic diseases (which explained 9.0%). Taken together, this study provides further evidence for the connection between personality traits, activity engagement, and later cognitive decline in old age.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Stimulus material selection for the Dutch famous faces test for older adults
- Author
-
van den Elzen, Evi H T, Brehmer, Yvonne, Van Deun, Katrijn, Mark, Ruth E, van den Elzen, Evi H T, Brehmer, Yvonne, Van Deun, Katrijn, and Mark, Ruth E
- Abstract
Worldwide, approximately 22% of all individuals aged 50 years and older are currently estimated to fall somewhere on the Alzheimer's disease (AD) continuum, which can be roughly divided into preclinical AD, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and AD dementia. While episodic memory loss (among other aspects) is typically required for a diagnosis of AD dementia, MCI is said to have occurred when cognitive impairment (including memory loss) is worse than expected for the person's age but not enough to be classified as dementia. On the other hand, preclinical AD can currently only be detected using biomarkers; clinical symptoms are not apparent using traditional neuropsychological tests. The main aim of the current paper was to explore the possibility of a test which could distinguish preclinical AD from normal aging. Recent scientific evidence suggests that the Famous Faces Test (FFT) could differentiate preclinical AD from normal aging up to 5 years before a clinical AD diagnosis. Problematic with existing FFTs is the selection of stimulus material. Faces famous in a specific country and a specific decade might not be equally famous for individuals in another country or indeed for people of different ages. The current article describes how famous faces were systematically selected and chosen for the Dutch older (60+) population using five steps. The goal was to design and develop short versions of the FFT for Dutch older adults of equivalent mean difficulty. In future work, these nine parallel versions will be necessary for (a) cross-sectional comparison as well as subsequent longitudinal assessment of cognitively normal and clinical groups and (b) creating personalized norms for the normal aged controls that could be used to compare performance within individuals with clinical diagnoses. The field needs a simple, cognitive test which can distinguish the earliest stages of the dementia continuum from normal aging.
- Published
- 2023
13. Three-year changes in leisure activities are associated with concurrent changes in white matter microstructure and perceptual speed in individuals aged 80 years and older
- Author
-
Köhncke, Ylva, Laukka, Erika J., Brehmer, Yvonne, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, Li, Tie-Qiang, Fratiglioni, Laura, Bäckman, Lars, and Lövdén, Martin
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Personality and limitations in instrumental activities of daily living in old age: Reciprocal associations across 12 years
- Author
-
Chereches, Flavia S., primary, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, and Olaru, Gabriel, additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Plasticity of brain and cognition in older adults
- Author
-
Brehmer, Yvonne, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, Wenger, Elisabeth, and Lövdén, Martin
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Training-induced changes in subsequent-memory effects: No major differences among children, younger adults, and older adults
- Author
-
Brehmer, Yvonne, Shing, Yee Lee, Heekeren, Hauke R., Lindenberger, Ulman, and Bäckman, Lars
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary
- Author
-
Bellander, Martin, Berggren, Rasmus, Mårtensson, Johan, Brehmer, Yvonne, Wenger, Elisabeth, Li, Tie-Qiang, Bodammer, Nils C., Shing, Yee-Lee, Werkle-Bergner, Markus, and Lövdén, Martin
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Helping Out or Helping Yourself? : Volunteering and Life Satisfaction Across the Retirement Transition
- Author
-
Bjalkebring, Par, Henning, Georg, Västfjäll, Daniel, Dickert, Stephan, Brehmer, Yvonne, Buratti, Sandra, Hansson, Isabelle, Johansson, Boo, Bjalkebring, Par, Henning, Georg, Västfjäll, Daniel, Dickert, Stephan, Brehmer, Yvonne, Buratti, Sandra, Hansson, Isabelle, and Johansson, Boo
- Abstract
It has been suggested that volunteering leads to increases in well-being, particularly in older and retiring adults, and that volunteering could be used as a public health intervention to increase well-being. However, the causal relationship has been questioned. We investigated the association between voluntary work and life satisfaction in a bivariate dual-change score model, using 4 years of longitudinal data from 1,123 participants from the Health, Aging and Retirement Transitions in Sweden (HEARTS) study. Both the frequency of volunteering and the level of life satisfaction increased across the retirement transition. However, baseline life satisfaction and volunteering were only marginally associated. Further, the coupling parameters suggest that higher levels of volunteering were followed by decreases in life satisfaction and that higher levels of life satisfaction were followed by increases in volunteering. These findings suggest that increasing levels of volunteering might not be a fruitful strategy for improving life satisfaction for all older adults-if people engage too much in voluntary work, it might even be detrimental for their life satisfaction. More research is needed to better understand when and for whom increased levels of volunteering might have positive effects on life satisfaction., Funding Agencies|International Postdoctoral Grant from the Swedish Research Council (VR)Swedish Research Council [DNR 2016-00507]; German Research FoundationGerman Research Foundation (DFG) [441444293]; FORTE (Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare) [DNR 2013-2291]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Foreign language learning in older age does not improve memory or intelligence : Evidence from a randomized controlled study.
- Author
-
Berggren, Rasmus, Nilsson, Jonna, Brehmer, Yvonne, Schmiedek, Florian, Lövdén, Martin, Berggren, Rasmus, Nilsson, Jonna, Brehmer, Yvonne, Schmiedek, Florian, and Lövdén, Martin
- Abstract
Foreign language learning in older age has been proposed as a promising avenue for combatting age-related cognitive decline. We tested this hypothesis in a randomized controlled study in a sample of 160 healthy older participants (aged 65-75 years) who were randomized to 11 weeks of either language learning or relaxation training. Participants in the language learning condition obtained some basic knowledge in the new language (Italian), but between-groups differences in improvements on latent factors of verbal intelligence, spatial intelligence, working memory, item memory, or associative memory were negligible. We argue that this is not due to either poor measurement, low course intensity, or low statistical power, but that basic studies in foreign languages in older age are likely to have no or trivially small effects on cognitive abilities. We place this in the context of the cognitive training and engagement literature and conclude that while foreign language learning may expand the behavioral repertoire, it does little to improve cognitive processing abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Helping out or helping yourself? Volunteering and life satisfaction across the retirement transition.
- Author
-
Bjälkebring, Pär, primary, Henning, Georg, additional, Västfjäll, Daniel, additional, Dickert, Stephan, additional, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, Buratti, Sandra, additional, Hansson, Isabelle, additional, and Johansson, Boo, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Foreign language learning in older age does not improve memory or intelligence: Evidence from a randomized controlled study.
- Author
-
Berggren, Rasmus, primary, Nilsson, Jonna, additional, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, Schmiedek, Florian, additional, and Lövdén, Martin, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Successful aging: Individual and societal challenges (?)
- Author
-
Brehmer, Yvonne and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Published
- 2019
23. The importance of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex for associative memory in older adults: A latent structural equation analysis
- Author
-
Brehmer, Yvonne, Nilsson, Jonna, Berggren, Rasmus, Schmiedek, Florian, Lövdén, Martin, Brehmer, Yvonne, Nilsson, Jonna, Berggren, Rasmus, Schmiedek, Florian, and Lövdén, Martin
- Abstract
Older adults show relatively minor age-related decline in memory for single items, while their memory for associations is markedly reduced. Inter-individual differences in memory function in older adults are substantial but the neurobiological underpinnings of such differences are not well understood. In particular, the relative importance of inter-individual differences in the medio-temporal lobe (MTL) and the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) for associative and item recognition in older adults is still ambiguous. We therefore aimed to first establish the distinction between inter-individual differences in associative memory (recollection-based) performance and item memory (familiarity-based) performance in older adults and subsequently link these two constructs to differences in cortical thickness in the MTL and lateral PFC regions, in a latent structural equation modelling framework. To this end, a sample of 160 older adults (65–75 years old) performed three intentional item-associative memory tasks, of which a subsample (n = 72) additionally had cortical thickness measures in MTL and PFC regions of interest available. The results provided support for a distinction between familiarity-based item memory and recollection-based associative memory performance in older adults. Cortical thickness in the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex was positively correlated with associative recognition performance, above and beyond any relationship between item recognition performance and cortical thickness in the same region and between associative recognition performance and brain structure in the MTL (parahippocampus). The findings highlight the relative importance of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in allowing for intentional recollection-based associative memory functioning in older adults.
- Published
- 2019
24. No Evidence for Improved Associative Memory Performance Following Process-Based Associative Memory Training in Older Adults
- Author
-
Bellander, Martin, Eschen, Anne, Lövdén, Martin, Martin, Mike, Bäckman, Lars, Brehmer, Yvonne, University of Zurich, and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Subjects
2805 Cognitive Neuroscience ,cognitive training ,associative memory ,1302 Aging ,10093 Institute of Psychology ,older adults ,transfer ,episodic memory ,education ,UFSP13-4 Dynamics of Healthy Aging ,150 Psychology - Abstract
Studies attempting to improve episodic memory performance with strategy instructions and training have had limited success in older adults: their training gains are limited in comparison to those of younger adults and do not generalize to untrained tasks and contexts. This limited success has been partly attributed to age-related impairments in associative binding of information into coherent episodes. We therefore investigated potential training and transfer effects of process-based associative memory training (i.e., repeated practice). Thirty-nine older adults (Mage = 68.8) underwent 6 weeks of either adaptive associative memory training or item recognition training. Both groups improved performance in item memory, spatial memory (object-context binding) and reasoning. A disproportionate effect of associative memory training was only observed for item memory, whereas no training-related performance changes were observed for associative memory. Self-reported strategies showed no signs of spontaneous development of memory-enhancing associative memory strategies. Hence, the results do not support the hypothesis that process-based associative memory training leads to higher associative memory performance in older adults., Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, ISSN:1663-4365
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Invariants et variabilités dans les sciences cognitives
- Author
-
Alexandre, Frédéric, Brehmer, Yvonne, Buisseret, Pierre, Carlier, Michèle, Carré, René, Deneubourg, Jean-Louis, Fuchs, Catherine, Gautrais, Jacques, Geert, Paul van, Hickmann, Maya, Hombert, Jean-Marie, Kail, Michèle, Kirman, Alan P., Kreutzer, Michel, Lahire, Bernard, Laks, Bernard, Lautrey, Jacques, Li, Shu-Chen, Liénard, Jean-Sylvain, Lindenberger, Ulman, Mazoyer, Bernard, Molenaar, Peter C. M., Ohlmann, Théophile, Rieben, Laurence, Roubertoux, Pierre L., Taylor, Anne-Christine, Theraulaz, Guy, Tzourio-Mazoyer, Nathalie, and Xerri, Christian
- Subjects
History ,SCI090000 ,variabilités ,Public Health & Health Care Science ,sociologie ,sciences cognitives ,GTR ,sciences du langage - Abstract
« Comme toutes les sciences, les sciences cognitives sont confrontées à la variabilité des phénomènes qu'elles étudient, et cherchent à dégager de cette variabilité un ensemble de régularités, d'invariants, sur lesquels ancrer les connaissances. Cette quête d'invariance implique des choix quant aux formes de variabilité à prendre en considération. Certaines, jugées pertinentes pour l'objet d'étude, sont utilisées ou manipulées pour en extraire des invariants, tandis que d'autres, jugées sans importance, sont négligées ou neutralisées. Concernant ces choix, les opinions et les pratiques sont changeantes selon les époques, l'état d'avancement des disciplines ou les courants théoriques au sein d'une même discipline. Des formes de variabilité ignorées à une époque peuvent devenir intéressantes un peu plus tard. Il semble précisément que nous soyons à une époque où le regard porté sur la variabilité évolue, notamment dans les sciences cognitives. La recherche d'universaux a souvent conduit à centrer l'analyse sur les tendances moyennes et à attribuer la variabilité observée autour de ces tendances aux erreurs de mesure ou à des bruits parasites sans grande importance. Or, dans beaucoup de disciplines concernées par la cognition, le rôle reconnu à la variabilité dans les mécanismes adaptatifs et, plus particulièrement, dans les processus d'auto-organisation, conduit à reconsidérer son statut. Cette évolution des idées suscite un regain d'intérêt pour l'étude des différentes formes de variabilité - intra-individuelle, interindividuelle, intergroupes, inter-langues, interculturelles, etc. - et conduit souvent à questionner, repenser, les invariants dans le domaine de la cognition. La recherche de nouvelles formes d'articulation entre les variabilités et les invariants apparaît donc comme un des thèmes émergents autour desquels peuvent se nouer - entre les sciences cognitives - des échanges fructueux aux plans épistémologique, théorique et méthodologique. »
- Published
- 2017
26. La variabilité dans le vieillissement comportemental : conséquence et agent du changement ontogénétique
- Author
-
Lindenberger, Ulman, Li, Shu-Chen, and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Abstract
Une propriété fondamentale du vieillissement comportemental est la variabilité inter-et intra-individuelle (Lindenberger & Baltes, 1994). Certaines personnes semblent jeunes pour leur âge, d'autres paraissent âgées. Une excellente illustration de cette variabilité est donnée par des personnages tels que Johann Wolfgang von Goethe ou Sophocle, dont les capacités intellectuelles exceptionnelles sont restées préservées jusqu'à un âge très avancé alors que certains individus voient leurs capacité...
- Published
- 2017
27. No evidence that foreign language learning in old age improves cognitive function: A randomized controlled study
- Author
-
Berggren, Rasmus, primary, Nilsson, Jonna, additional, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, Schmiedek, Florian, additional, and Lövdén, Martin, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Dopamine Receptor Genes Modulate Associative Memory in Old Age
- Author
-
Papenberg, Göran, Becker, Nina, Ferencz, Beata, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, Laukka, Erika J., Bäckman, Lars, Brehmer, Yvonne, Papenberg, Göran, Becker, Nina, Ferencz, Beata, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, Laukka, Erika J., Bäckman, Lars, and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Abstract
Previous research shows that associative memory declines more than item memory in aging. Although the underlying mechanisms of this selective impairment remain poorly understood, animal and human data suggest that dopaminergic modulation may be particularly relevant for associative binding. We investigated the influence of dopamine (DA) receptor genes on item and associative memory in a population-based sample of older adults (n = 525, aged 60 years), assessed with a face-scene item associative memory task. The effects of single-nucleotide polymorphisms of DA D1 (DRD1; rs4532), D2 (DRD2/ANKK1/Taq1A; rs1800497), and D3 (DRD3/Ser9Gly; rs6280) receptor genes were examined and combined into a single genetic score. Individuals carrying more beneficial alleles, presumably associated with higher DA receptor efficacy (DRD1 C allele; DRD2 A2 allele; DRD3 T allele), performed better on associative memory than persons with less beneficial genotypes. There were no effects of these genes on item memory or other cognitive measures, such as working memory, executive functioning, fluency, and perceptual speed, indicating a selective association between DA genes and associative memory. By contrast, genetic risk for Alzheimer disease (AD) was associated with worse item and associative memory, indicating adverse effects of APOE epsilon 4 and a genetic risk score for AD (PICALM, BIN1, CLU) on episodic memory in general. Taken together, our results suggest that DA may be particularly important for associative memory, whereas AD-related genetic variations may influence overall episodic memory in older adults without dementia.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Differential Effects of Encoding Instructions on Brain Activity Patterns of Item and Associative Memory
- Author
-
Becker, Nina, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, Persson, Jonas, Laukka, Erika J., Brehmer, Yvonne, Becker, Nina, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, Persson, Jonas, Laukka, Erika J., and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Abstract
Evidence from neuroimaging studies suggests a critical role of hippocampus and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in associative relative to item encoding. Here, we investigated similarities and differences in functional brain correlates for associative and item memory as a function of encoding instruction. Participants received either incidental (animacy judgments) or intentional encoding instructions while fMRI was employed during the encoding of associations and items. In a subsequent recognition task, memory performance of participants receiving intentional encoding instructions was higher compared with those receiving incidental encoding instructions. Furthermore, participants remembered more items than associations, regardless of encoding instruction. Greater brain activation in the left anterior hippocampus was observed for intentionally compared with incidentally encoded associations, although activity in this region was not modulated by the type of instruction for encoded items. Furthermore, greater activity in the left anterior hippocampus and left IFG was observed during intentional associative compared with item encoding. The same regions were related to subsequent memory of intentionally encoded associations and were thus task relevant. Similarly, connectivity of the anterior hippocampus to the right superior temporal lobe and IFG was uniquely linked to subsequent memory of intentionally encoded associations. Our study demonstrates the differential involvement of anterior hippocampus in intentional relative to incidental associative encoding. This finding likely reflects that the intent to remember triggers a specific binding process accomplished by this region.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Differential Effects of Encoding Instructions on Brain Activity Patterns of Item and Associative Memory
- Author
-
Becker, Nina, primary, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, additional, Persson, Jonas, additional, Laukka, Erika J., additional, and Brehmer, Yvonne, additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Dopamine Receptor Genes Modulate Associative Memory in Old Age
- Author
-
Papenberg, Goran, primary, Becker, Nina, additional, Ferencz, Beata, additional, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, additional, Laukka, Erika J., additional, Bäckman, Lars, additional, and Brehmer, Yvonne, additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Neural activation patterns of successful episodic encoding : Reorganization during childhood, maintenance in old age
- Author
-
Shing, Yee Lee, Brehmer, Yvonne, Heekeren, Hauke R., Bäckman, Lars, Lindenberger, Ulman, Shing, Yee Lee, Brehmer, Yvonne, Heekeren, Hauke R., Bäckman, Lars, and Lindenberger, Ulman
- Abstract
The two-component framework of episodic memory (EM) development posits that the contributions of medial temporal lobe (MTL) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) to successful encoding differ across the lifespan. To test the framework's hypotheses, we compared subsequent memory effects (SME) of 10-12 year-old children, younger adults, and older adults using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Memory was probed by cued recall, and SME were defined as regional activation differences during encoding between subsequently correctly recalled versus omitted items. In MTL areas, children's SME did not differ in magnitude from those of younger and older adults. In contrast, children's SME in PFC were weaker than the corresponding SME in younger and older adults, in line with the hypothesis that PFC contributes less to successful encoding in childhood. Differences in SME between younger and older adults were negligible. The present results suggest that, among individuals with high memory functioning, the neural circuitry contributing to successful episodic encoding is reorganized from middle childhood to adulthood. Successful episodic encoding in later adulthood, however, is characterized by the ability to maintain the activation patterns that emerged in young adulthood.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Neural activation patterns of successful episodic encoding: Reorganization during childhood, maintenance in old age
- Author
-
Shing, Yee Lee, primary, Brehmer, Yvonne, additional, Heekeren, Hauke R., additional, Bäckman, Lars, additional, and Lindenberger, Ulman, additional
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Structural brain correlates of associative memory in older adults
- Author
-
Becker, Nina, Laukka, Erika J., Kalpouzos, Gregoria, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, Bäckman, Lars, Brehmer, Yvonne, Becker, Nina, Laukka, Erika J., Kalpouzos, Gregoria, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, Bäckman, Lars, and Brehmer, Yvonne
- Abstract
Associative memory involves binding two or more items into a coherent memory episode. Relative to memory for single items, associative memory declines greatly in aging. However, older individuals vary substantially in their ability to memorize associative information. Although functional studies link associative memory to the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and prefrontal cortex (PFC), little is known about how volumetric differences in MTL and PFC might contribute to individual differences in associative memory. We investigated regional gray-matter volumes related to individual differences in associative memory in a sample of healthy older adults (n = 54; age = 60 years). To differentiate item from associative memory, participants intentionally learned face-scene picture pairs before performing a recognition task that included single faces, scenes, and face-scene pairs. Gray-matter volumes were analyzed using voxel-based morphometry region-of-interest (ROI) analyses. To examine volumetric differences specifically for associative memory, item memory was controlled for in the analyses. Behavioral results revealed large variability in associative memory that mainly originated from differences in false-alarm rates. Moreover, associative memory was independent of individuals' ability to remember single items. Older adults with better associative memory showed larger gray-matter volumes primarily in regions of the left and right lateral PFC. These findings provide evidence for the importance of PFC in intentional learning of associations, likely because of its involvement in organizational and strategic processes that distinguish older adults with good from those with poor associative memory.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Structural brain correlates of associative memory in older adults
- Author
-
Becker, Nina, primary, Laukka, Erika J., additional, Kalpouzos, Grégoria, additional, Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe, additional, Bäckman, Lars, additional, and Brehmer, Yvonne, additional
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Sense of purpose and meaning in life during the transition to grandparenthood.
- Author
-
Chereches, Flavia S, Ballhausen, Nicola, Brehmer, Yvonne, and Olaru, Gabriel
- Abstract
\n The grandparent role has been often described as one of the most fulfilling aspects of older age. Yet, no longitudinal studies investigated the effect of the transition to grandparenthood on older adults’ sense of purpose and meaning in life. In this pre-registered study, we investigated the effect of becoming a first-time grandparent and a first-time caregiving grandparent on changes in sense of purpose and meaning in life. We used data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), covering up to 12 years around the transition (
N HRS = 843,N ELSA = 587). To disentangle potential grandparenthood effects from age-related developmental changes, we used propensity score matching to compare grandparents to matched non-grandparents. Using latent change score models, we only found increases in sense of purpose in the years leading up to the transition, but not afterwards. No consistent findings of mean-level changes were observed in the control groups. Thus, our findings suggest that becoming a (caregiving) grandparent does not necessarily contribute to higher meaning and sense of purpose in life than before or compared to non-grandparents.Older adults often report that being a grandparent is one of the most fulfilling roles of older age. However, there are no studies to investigate how older adults’ sense of purpose and meaning in life changes over time when they first become grandparents. Similarly, no past research has investigated how the two constructs change when first-time grandparents start to provide care for their grandchildren for the first time. In this study, we use data from two large population studies (Health and Retirement Study and English Longitudinal Study of Ageing;N HRS = 843,N ELSA = 587 ) covering up to 12 years to see if newly grandparents increase in sense of purpose and meaning in life during the transition to (active) grandparenthood. To make sure that any potential effects are not due to life events or changes that normally take place during old age, we compared grandparents to older adults that were not (yet) grandparents, or did not have children, but were similar to our target group (i.e., grandparents) with respect to other characteristics—for example, age, gender, and health status. We did not find evidence for any change in sense of purpose and meaning in life after older adults become grandparents, but only in the years leading up to the transition to grandparenthood. For the groups of non-grandparents, we did not find any notable differences in how sense of purpose and meaning in life changed during the study period compared to the groups of grandparents. Starting to provide grandchildren as a first-time grandparent also did not lead to any changes in sense of purpose and meaning in life. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Towards an earlier recognition of cognitive deficit after stroke: Raising awareness for the link between motor and cognitive impairment
- Author
-
Verstraeten, S.M.M., Sitskoorn, Margriet, Mark, Ruth, Bakker, C.D., Bongers, Inge, Brehmer, Yvonne, Engelborghs, Sebastiaan, Hiel, J.A.P., and Cognitive Neuropsychology
- Abstract
Onderzoeksvraag De focus van het onderzoek ligt op het vergroten van kennis over de onderlinge samenhang van motorische en cognitieve stoornissen na een CVA, met als onderliggende vraag: kan meer kennis over de relatie tussen motorische en cognitieve stoornissen bijdragen aan het eerder herkennen van patiënten met cognitieve stoornissen? Belang van het onderzoek Een CVA is de op een na meest voorkomende oorzaak van overlijden. Zowel motorische als cognitieve beperkingen komen veel voor, zijn veelal blijvend en kunnen een negatieve invloed hebben op het persoonlijk en maatschappelijk functioneren, maar ook op de ervaren kwaliteit van leven. Het bevorderen van herstel na een CVA en het (h)erkennen van de beperkingen is dan ook belangrijk. Hierbij dient aandacht besteed te worden aan zowel de motorische als cognitieve gevolgen van een CVA. In de eerste periode na het doormaken van een CVA ligt de focus doorgaans op lichamelijk herstel en worden problemen in het cognitieve functioneren vaak (nog) niet herkend. Onderzoeksmethode Aan de hand van data van twee onderzoeksprotocollen (‘COMPlaints After Stroke’, COMPAS en ‘Motor and Cognitive Impairment after Stroke’, MoCIS) waarin CVA-patiënten twee jaar werden gevolgd werd de relatie tussen motorische en cognitieve stoornissen onderzocht en werd tevens bekeken of motorische stoornissen problemen in het cognitieve functioneren kunnen voorspellen. Conclusie Concluderend kan gesteld worden dat de methoden die momenteel in de dagelijkse praktijk gebruikt worden om zicht te krijgen op het cognitieve functioneren (door te vragen naar cognitieve klachten of het gebruik van beoordelingsschalen die een inschatting geven van de gevolgen van het CVA) onvoldoende zijn. Het huidige onderzoek toont een relatief consistent verband aan tussen motorisch en cognitief functioneren en geeft tevens aanwijzingen dat het motorisch functioneren het cognitief functioneren kan voorspellen. Aanbevelingen Geadviseerd wordt om kort na het doormaken van het CVA het motorische functioneren in kaart te brengen met objectieve taken en deze resultaten te gebruiken om CVA patiënten met cognitieve problemen eerder te herkennen, zodat de juiste zorg op tijd kan worden ingezet. Hierbij kan gedacht worden aan extra uitleg over de mogelijke cognitieve gevolgen van een CVA en het geven van tips en adviezen over hoe hier het best mee omgegaan kan worden.
- Published
- 2023
38. Back to the drawing board: Potential indicators of giftedness in human figure drawings
- Author
-
Mathijssen, Sven, Denissen, Jaap, Hoogeveen, A.J.M., Feltzer, Martin, Ziegler, A., O'Reilly, C., Brehmer, Yvonne, Sutherland, M., Ruiter, S., and Developmental Psychology
- Abstract
Terug naar de tekentafel: Mogelijke aanwijzingen voor hoogbegaafdheid in kindertekeningen Mogelijke talenten van kinderen worden niet altijd gezien in het onderwijs. Dat is jammer, omdat deze kinderen dan niet het onderwijs krijgen dat zij nodig hebben om deze talenten tot bloei te laten komen. Als kinderen geen passend onderwijs krijgen, kunnen zij ongelukkig worden en hun talenten niet goed ontwikkelen. Bij kinderen met kenmerken van hoogbegaafdheid is soms onduidelijk wat er precies nodig is. Om dat uit te zoeken, wordt vaak psychologisch onderzoek gedaan, maar dat is erg tijdrovend, duur en niet voor iedereen mogelijk. Daarom is met dit promotieonderzoek nagegaan of er aanwijzingen voor hoogbegaafdheid in kindertekeningen te vinden zijn. Kinderen tekenen namelijk uit zichzelf al van jongs af aan mensfiguren en deinzen daarom niet snel terug voor een tekenopdracht. Daarnaast kost het laten maken van een menstekening weinig tijd en geld. Een menstekening kan mogelijk als screeningsinstrument voor hoogbegaafdheid dienen. Een goede screening werkt namelijk als een soort trechter. In dit geval: alle kinderen in een klas kunnen een tekening maken en opvallende tekeningen kunnen een aanwijzing zijn voor hoogbegaafdheid. Dat zegt nog niet wat die kinderen nodig hebben, maar het kan wel het signaal afgeven dat het ‘normale’ reguliere onderwijs niet past bij deze kinderen. Daarna kan breder worden gekeken (bijvoorbeeld met psychologisch onderzoek), om na te gaan wat zij precies nodig hebben op school. In dit promotieonderzoek zijn de menstekeningen van twee groepen kinderen vergeleken: kinderen van regulier onderwijs en kinderen van hoogbegaafdenonderwijs. Eerst zijn de zogenaamde teken-IQ’s van deze kinderen vergeleken. Sommige onderzoekers stellen: hoe meer kenmerken in een tekening, hoe hoger het teken-IQ en hoe slimmer het kind. De teken-IQ’s van de twee groepen kinderen in het eerste onderzoek verschilden echter niet. Een hoog teken-IQ leek daarom geen aanwijzing te zijn voor hoogbegaafdheid. Wel viel op dat de groep kinderen die hoogbegaafdenonderwijs kregen kenmerken tekenden die de andere groep niet tekende. In een vervolgonderzoek is daarom ingezoomd op ‘bijzondere kenmerken’: kenmerken die alleen of vaker werden getekend door hoogbegaafde kinderen. Ruim 200 kinderen van 4 tot en met 6 jaar oud maakten een menstekening. Zij volgden allemaal regulier onderwijs. Twee jaar later is aan de ouders gevraagd of hun kinderen onderwijsaanpassingen hadden gekregen. Kinderen die aanpassingen gericht op hoogbegaafdheid kregen werden ingedeeld in de groep ‘mogelijk hoogbegaafd’. Hun tekeningen zijn vergeleken met de kinderen die regulier onderwijs bleven volgen. Uit het onderzoek bleek dat het tekenen van één of meer van die bijzondere kenmerken inderdaad een voorspeller was voor het behoren tot de ‘mogelijk hoogbegaafd’ groep. Dat gold wel alleen voor 4- en 5-jarigen; voor 6-jarigen bleken er geen voorspellende bijzondere kenmerken te zijn. De bijzondere kenmerken verschilden voor 4- en 5-jarigen. Wat overeenkwam is dat het ging om kenmerken die aantonen wat er is getekend (zoals irissen in de ogen of meerdere mensfiguren) of onrealistische groottes (zoals hele kleine hoofden of hele grote handen). Deze kenmerken hoefden niet per se mooi getekend te zijn. Juist kenmerken die een tekening realistisch maken (zoals juiste groottes en stevige lijnen) bleken geen voorspellers van hoogbegaafdheid te zijn. Voor de praktijk is voorzichtigheid nog wel geboden, want er is nog meer onderzoek nodig. Maar het betekent wel dat menstekeningen kunnen helpen bij het vroegtijdig signaleren van kinderen wiens talenten mogelijk niet volledig tot bloei zouden komen in het reguliere onderwijs. Om na te gaan wat een kind nodig heeft, is vaak een brede kijk nodig. Dat is niet altijd mogelijk om te doen voor álle kinderen. Maar als er bijzondere kenmerken in een menstekening worden gezien, kan dat wél een reden zijn om tijd en geld te investeren en na te gaan wat een kind nodig heeft op school. Op deze manier kunnen menstekeningen een belangrijke bijdrage leveren aan een goede schoolontwikkeling en emotionele ontwikkeling van jonge kinderen met kenmerken van hoogbegaafdheid Back to the drawing board: Potential indicators of giftedness in children’s drawings Potential talents of children are not always seen in education. That is unfortunate, because then these children will not receive the education they need to let their talents flourish. If children do not receive appropriate education, they may become unhappy and their talents may not develop properly. For children with high abilities, it is not always clear what is needed. To investigate this, psychological evaluation is often conducted, but this is time-consuming, costly, and not possible for everyone. Therefore, this doctoral research investigated whether indicators of giftedness might be identified from children’s drawings. Children draw human figures from a very early age and therefore do not quickly back down from a drawing task. In addition, letting children draw a human figure does not cost a lot of time and money. A human figure drawing may serve as a screening instrument for giftedness. A good screening tool functions like a funnel. In this case: all children in a classroom can make a drawing and exceptionalities in drawings might be indicators of giftedness. This does not give information about what those children need, but they may indicate that the ‘normal’ regular curriculum does not fit the needs of these children. To determine what these children need in school, further investigation can be done, for example through psychological evaluation. In this doctoral research, the human figure drawings of two groups of children are compared: children who received regular education and children who received gifted education. First, the so-called drawing IQs of children were compared. Some researchers state: the more drawn items, the higher the drawing IQ, and the smarter the child. However, the drawing IQs of the two groups in the first study did not differ. A high drawing IQ therefore did not seem to be an indicator for giftedness. Remarkably, children who received gifted education drew some items that were not drawn by the other group. In a follow-up study, the focus was on ‘exceptional items’: items that were drawn only or more often by children with high abilities. Over 200 children aged 4 to 6 years made a human figure drawing. They all received regular education. Two years later, the parents were asked if any adaptations were made to the curriculum of their children. Children who received adaptations aimed at giftedness were divided into the group ‘potentially gifted’. Their drawings were compared to children who received regular education. The findings suggested that drawing one or more of those exceptional items indeed was a predictor of belonging to the ‘potentially gifted’ group. This only applied to 4 and 5-year-olds, though. For 6-year-olds there appeared to be no predictive exceptional items. The exceptional items differed between 4 and 5-year-olds. However, in both age groups these involved items that indicated what was drawn (such as irises in the eyes or multiple human figures) or unrealistic sizes (such as very small heads or very large hands). These items did not necessarily have to look good. In particular items that make the drawing look realistic (such as correct proportions and firm lines) turned out to be no indicators of giftedness. For the practical field, caution is still advised, because follow-up research is needed. However, the findings imply that human figure drawings may contribute to early identification of children whose talents may not fully flourish in regular education. To determine what a child needs, a broad view is required. Seeing every child from a broad point of view is not always possible. However, if exceptional items in human figure drawings are observed, this might be a reason to invest time and money to investigate what a child needs in school. This way, human figure drawings can make an important contribution to the school development and emotional development of young children with high abilities.
- Published
- 2023
39. Personality and identity development during the transition to working life
- Author
-
den Boer, L., Denissen, Jaap, Klimstra, Theo, Brehmer, Yvonne, Kooij, Dorien, van Aken, M.A.G., Luyckx, K., Kunnen, E.S., and Developmental Psychology
- Published
- 2021
40. Is There a Link Between Older Adults' Frequency of (Face-to-Face and Remote) Contact with Grandchildren and Cognitive Functioning over 12 Years?
- Author
-
Chereches FS, Ballhausen N, Olaru G, Laukka EJ, and Brehmer Y
- Abstract
Objective: Past research has linked more frequent social contacts with better cognition and slower cognitive decline in older adults. An open question is whether face-to-face and remote contact with one's grandchildren can be beneficial., Methods: We analyzed data from the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care in Kungsholmen (SNAC-K) covering a span of 12 years and two age cohorts (young-old < 78, N = 1100; old-old ≥ 78 years, N = 705). We used latent growth curve models, to examine whether frequency of face-to-face or remote grandchild contact was associated with cognitive levels and decline and applied (random intercept) cross-lagged models to investigate if these associations were reciprocal., Results: Face-to-face contact with grandchildren was positively linked to levels of cognition in young-old adults only. We found no associations with cognitive decline. Results of cross-lagged models suggested that grandparents with better cognition had more face-to-face (for young-old adults only) or remote (for old-old adults only) grandchild contact at subsequent waves. However, more grandchild contact was not associated with later cognition., Discussion: Our findings suggest that grandparents with better cognition engage more with their grandchildren, but that frequency of grandchild contact is not a protective factor against later cognitive decline in older adults., (© The Author(s) 2024. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. No Evidence for Improved Associative Memory Performance Following Process-Based Associative Memory Training in Older Adults.
- Author
-
Bellander M, Eschen A, Lövdén M, Martin M, Bäckman L, and Brehmer Y
- Abstract
Studies attempting to improve episodic memory performance with strategy instructions and training have had limited success in older adults: their training gains are limited in comparison to those of younger adults and do not generalize to untrained tasks and contexts. This limited success has been partly attributed to age-related impairments in associative binding of information into coherent episodes. We therefore investigated potential training and transfer effects of process-based associative memory training (i.e., repeated practice). Thirty-nine older adults ( M
age = 68.8) underwent 6 weeks of either adaptive associative memory training or item recognition training. Both groups improved performance in item memory, spatial memory (object-context binding) and reasoning. A disproportionate effect of associative memory training was only observed for item memory, whereas no training-related performance changes were observed for associative memory. Self-reported strategies showed no signs of spontaneous development of memory-enhancing associative memory strategies. Hence, the results do not support the hypothesis that process-based associative memory training leads to higher associative memory performance in older adults.- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.