25 results on '"Matthew Heath"'
Search Results
2. Cerebral Blood Flow and the Immediate and Sustained Effects of Passive and Active Exercise
- Author
-
Benjamin Tari, Joshua Ahn, Connor Dalton, Sun Young Choo, and Matthew Heath
- Subjects
History ,Polymers and Plastics ,Business and International Management ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering - Published
- 2022
3. Modulation of Pro-Allergic T Cell Responses by PQ Grass 27600 SU as a Treatment for Seasonal Allergic Rhinitis: A RDBPC Pilot Study
- Author
-
Janice Layhadi, Lily Wu, Paulina Filipaviciute, Kemi Oluwayi, Sviatlana Starchenka, Olesya Rusyn, Oliver Armfield, Katarzyna Lis, Matthias Kramer, Matthew Heath, Murray Skinner, Pieter-Jan de Kam, and Mohamed Shamji
- Subjects
Immunology ,Immunology and Allergy - Published
- 2023
4. Differential Transcriptome Immunomodulatory Profile of an Adjuvanted Modified Grass Immunotherapy in Subjects with Seasonal Allergic Rhinitis and Rhinoconjuctivitis
- Author
-
Pieter-Jan de Kam, Sviatlana Starchenka, Kemi Oluwayi, Matthew Heath, Murray Skinner, Oliver Armfield, Olesya Rusyn, and Katarzyna Lis
- Subjects
Immunology ,Immunology and Allergy - Published
- 2023
5. Induction of Local and Systemic Blocking Antibodies by PQ Grass 27600 SU for the Treatment of Seasonal Allergic Rhinitis: A RDBPC Pilot Study
- Author
-
Mohamed Shamji, Janice Layhadi, Anna Cutrina Pons, Lily Wu, Sviatlana Starchenka, Olesya Rusyn, Oliver Armfield, Katarzyna Lis, Kemi Oluwayi, Matthias Kramer, Matthew Heath, Murray Skinner, and Pieter-Jan de Kam
- Subjects
Immunology ,Immunology and Allergy - Published
- 2023
6. Modulation of T and B cell Responses by Virus-like particle (VLP) Expressing Peanut Allergen Ara h 2: A Novel Vaccine Candidate for Peanut Allergy
- Author
-
Janice Layhadi, Elizabeth Palmer, Gabija Drazdauskaité, Paulina Filipaviciute, Alistair Devine, Lily Wu, Paul Turner, Nandinee Patel, Kemi Oluwayi, Olesya Rusyn, Murray Skinner, Matthew Heath, Pieter-Jan de Kam, and Mohamed Shamji
- Subjects
Immunology ,Immunology and Allergy - Published
- 2022
7. Visuomotor mental rotation of a saccade: The contingent negative variation scales to the angle of rotation
- Author
-
Matthew Heath, Francisco L. Colino, Jillian Chan, and Olave E. Krigolson
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Rotation ,Contingent Negative Variation ,Stimulus (physiology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Mental rotation ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Evoked Potentials ,Mathematics ,Angle of rotation ,Analysis of Variance ,Superior colliculus ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,Oblique case ,Sensory Systems ,Contingent negative variation ,Ophthalmology ,Saccade ,Imagination ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
The visuomotor mental rotation (VMR) of a saccade requires a response to a region of space that is dissociated from a stimulus by a pre-specified angle, and work has shown a monotonic increase in reaction times as a function of increasing oblique angles of rotation. These results have been taken as evidence of a continuous process of rotation and have generated competing hypotheses. One hypothesis asserts that rotation is mediated via frontoparietal structures, whereas a second states that a continuous shift in the activity of direction-specific neurons in the superior colliculus (SC) supports rotation. Research to date, however, has not examined the neural mechanisms underlying VMR saccades and both hypotheses therefore remain untested. The present study measured the behavioural data and event-related brain potentials (ERP) of standard (i.e., 0° of rotation) and VMR saccades involving 35°, 70° and 105° of rotation. Behavioural results showed that participants adhered to task-based rotation demands and ERP findings showed that the amplitude of the contingent negative variation (CNV) linearly decreased with increasing angle of rotation. The cortical generators of the CNV are linked to frontoparietal structures supporting movement preparation. Although our ERP design does not allow us to exclude a possible role of the SC in the rotation of a VMR saccade, they do demonstrate that such actions are supported by a continuous and cortically based rotation process.
- Published
- 2018
8. Exercise intensity-specific changes to cerebral blood velocity do not modulate a postexercise executive function benefit
- Author
-
Mustafa Shirzad, Glen R. Belfry, Benjamin Tari, Matthew Heath, and Nikan Behboodpour
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Executive Function ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Internal medicine ,medicine.artery ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,medicine ,Humans ,Aerobic exercise ,Exercise ,Lactate threshold ,Cardiorespiratory fitness ,030229 sport sciences ,Transcranial Doppler ,Intensity (physics) ,Cardiorespiratory Fitness ,Cerebral blood flow ,Middle cerebral artery ,Cardiology ,Exercise intensity ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Executive function is transiently improved (i.e., 2peak test to determine cardiorespiratory fitness and estimated lactate threshold (LT), followed by separate 10-min sessions of light- (i.e., 25 W), moderate- (i.e., 80% estimated LT), and heavy-intensity (i.e., 15% of the difference between LT and V̇O2peak) aerobic exercise. An estimate of CBF during exercise was achieved via transcranial Doppler ultrasound and near-infrared spectroscopy to quantify blood velocity (BV) through the middle cerebral artery and deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb), respectively. Executive function was assessed before and after each session via the executive-mediated antisaccade task (i.e., saccade mirror-symmetrical to a target). Results demonstrated that BV increased in relation to increasing exercise intensity, whereas HHb decreased by a comparable magnitude independent of intensity. In terms of executive function, null hypothesis and equivalence tests indicated a comparable magnitude postexercise reduction in antisaccade reaction time across exercise intensities. Accordingly, the magnitude of CBF change during exercise does not impact the magnitude of a postexercise executive function benefit.
- Published
- 2021
9. Reduced order model verification of a DC microgrid for controller design and determination of storage requirements
- Author
-
Eddy H. Trinklein, David G. Wilson, Gordon G. Parker, Matthew Heath, Wayne W. Weaver, Rush D. Robinett, and Michael D. Cook
- Subjects
Controller design ,Computer science ,020209 energy ,020208 electrical & electronic engineering ,Energy Engineering and Power Technology ,02 engineering and technology ,Energy storage ,Reduced order ,Control theory ,Boost converter ,Distributed data store ,0202 electrical engineering, electronic engineering, information engineering ,Microgrid ,Electrical and Electronic Engineering ,Design methods ,Closed loop - Abstract
Energy storage requirements and its management are important considerations for dc microgrid designs that have a high penetration of stochastic distributed sources and loads. Modern control methods, such as Hamiltonian Surface Shaping and Power Flow Control (HSSPFC), often rely on a reduced order model of the microgrid for controller design. This paper explores (1) the reduced order boost converter model for use in development of advanced control schemes via a detailed, switching mode model implemented on a Typhoon HIL 602 with a controller-in-the-loop (CIL) and (2) a design methodology that may be used for determining converter distributed storage requirements for the closed loop controls.
- Published
- 2020
10. Relative Efficacy Of Outpatient Infusion Therapy On Pediatric Patients With Post-Concussive Headaches
- Author
-
Katz, Erin, primary, Gould, Sara, additional, Cignetti, Carly, additional, Hale, Matthew Heath, additional, and Sarrett, Laura, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Target frequency influences antisaccade endpoint bias: Evidence for perceptual averaging
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Caitlin Gillen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Range effect ,Antisaccade ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Task (project management) ,Young Adult ,Perception ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Humans ,Eccentricity (behavior) ,media_common ,Analysis of Variance ,Cognition ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Perceptual averaging ,Saccade ,Fixation (visual) ,Regression Analysis ,Female ,Visual Fields ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Oculomotor ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Perceptual judgments related to stimulus-sets are represented computationally different than individual items. In particular, the perceptual averaging hypothesis contends that the visual system represents target properties (e.g., eccentricity) via a statistical summary of the individual targets included within a stimulus-set. Here we sought to determine whether perceptual averaging governs the visual information mediating an oculomotor task requiring top-down control (i.e., antisaccade). To that end, participants completed antisaccades (i.e., saccade mirror-symmetrical to a target) – and complementary prosaccades (i.e., saccade to veridical target location) – to different target eccentricities (10.5°, 15.5° and 20.5°) located left and right of a common fixation. Importantly, trials were completed in blocks wherein eccentricities were presented with equal frequency (i.e., control condition) and when the ‘proximal’ (10.5°: i.e., proximal-weighting condition) and ‘distal’ (20.5°: i.e., distal-weighting condition) targets were respectively presented five times as often as the other eccentricities. If antisaccades are governed by a statistical summary then amplitudes should be biased in the direction of the most frequently presented target within a block. As expected, pro- and antisaccade across each target eccentricity were associated with an undershooting bias and prosaccades were refractory to the manipulation of target frequency. Most notably, antisaccades in the proximal-weighting condition had a larger undershooting bias than the control condition, whereas the converse was true for the distal-weighing condition; that is, antisaccades were biased in the direction of the most frequently presented target. Thus, we propose that perceptual averaging extends to motor tasks requiring top-down cognitive control.
- Published
- 2014
12. Weber’s law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: Feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Shirin Davarpanah Jazi
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Illusion ,Differential Threshold ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Frame of reference ,Task (project management) ,Young Adult ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Feedback, Sensory ,Perception ,Psychophysics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,media_common ,Hand Strength ,GRASP ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Touch Perception ,Space Perception ,Law ,Female ,Percept ,Psychology ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
The goal of the present investigation was to test the somatosensory processing model's (SPM) assertion that tactile actions and perceptions are mediated via egocentric and allocentric frames of reference, respectively (Dijkerman & de Hann's 2007: Behavioral and Brain Sciences). To accomplish that objective, Experiment 1 required that participants use their right hand to grasp and manually estimate differently sized objects placed on the forearm and palm of their left hand. Following each manual estimation trial, participants grasped the target object to equate tasks (i.e., grasping vs. manual estimation) for terminal tactile feedback. Notably, the different object locations (i.e., forearm and palm) were used to examine whether location-specific differences in mechanoreceptor density impacts the percept of object size (i.e., Weber's illusion). In addition, we computed just-noticeable-difference (JND) scores to determine whether grasping and manual estimations adhere to, or violate, the allocentric principles of Weber's law. Results for the grasping task elicited a null expression of Weber's illusion and JNDs for this task violated Weber's law. Results for the manual estimation task similarly exhibited a null expression of Weber's illusion; however, JNDs for the palm but not the forearm condition adhered to Weber's law. Experiment 2 showed that withdrawing terminal tactile feedback during forearm condition manual estimations resulted in responses that adhered to Weber's law. Thus, results provide some support for the SPM's contention that grasping and manual estimations are mediated via ego- and allocentric frames of reference, respectively. However, results further indicate that the dissociation is not complete and is, in part, influenced by the sensory consequences (i.e., terminal tactile feedback) associated with the response.
- Published
- 2014
13. Repetitive antisaccade execution does not increase the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Jeffrey Weiler
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Task switching ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Corollary ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,05 social sciences ,General Medicine ,Visual field ,Inhibition, Psychological ,Saccade ,Female ,Antisaccade task ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
An antisaccade is the execution of a saccade to the mirror-symmetrical location (i.e., same amplitude but opposite visual field) of a single and exogenously presented visual target. Such a response requires top-down decoupling of the normally direct spatial relations between stimulus and response and results in increased planning times and directional errors compared to their spatially compatible prosaccade counterparts. Moreover, antisaccades are associated with diffuse changes in cortical and subcortical saccade networks: a finding that has, in part, been attributed to pre-setting the oculomotor system to withhold a stimulus-driven prosaccade. Moreover, recent work has shown that a corollary cost of oculomotor pre-setting is that the planning time for a to-be-completed prosaccade is longer when preceded by an antisaccade (i.e., the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost). Notably, this result has been attributed to antisaccades imparting a residual inhibition of the oculomotor networks that support the planning of stimulus-driven prosaccades. In the current investigation, we sought to determine if the number of antisaccades preceding a prosaccade increases this residual inhibition and thus influences the magnitude of the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost. To that end, participants alternated between pro- and antisaccades after every second (i.e., AABB schedule) and every fourth (i.e., AAAABBBB schedule) trial. In addition, participants completed pro- and antisaccades in separate blocks of trials. Results demonstrated that task-switch prosaccades produced longer reaction times than their task-repetition and blocked condition counterparts, whereas antisaccade reaction times did not vary across task-repetition, task-switch and blocked condition trials. Most notably, the magnitude of the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost was not modulated across the different task-switching schedules. Thus, we propose that the top-down requirements of the antisaccade task do not produce additive inhibition of stimulus-driven saccade networks.
- Published
- 2014
14. Relative Efficacy Of Outpatient Infusion Therapy On Pediatric Patients With Post-Concussive Headaches
- Author
-
Erin Katz, Sara Gould, Carly Cignetti, Matthew Heath Hale, and Laura Sarrett
- Subjects
Outpatient Infusion Therapy ,Relative efficacy ,business.industry ,Anesthesia ,Rehabilitation ,Medicine ,Physical Therapy, Sports Therapy and Rehabilitation ,Headaches ,medicine.symptom ,business - Published
- 2018
15. Goal-directed grasping: The dimensional properties of an object influence the nature of the visual information mediating aperture shaping
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Scott A. Holmes
- Subjects
Male ,Visual perception ,Adolescent ,Movement ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stereoscopy ,Vergence ,Visual control ,law.invention ,Young Adult ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,law ,Perception ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Contrast (vision) ,Computer vision ,Size Perception ,media_common ,Communication ,Hand Strength ,business.industry ,Object (philosophy) ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Visual Perception ,Task analysis ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,Psychology ,business ,Goals ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
An issue of continued debate in the visuomotor control literature surrounds whether a 2D object serves as a representative proxy for a 3D object in understanding the nature of the visual information supporting grasping control. In an effort to reconcile this issue, we examined the extent to which aperture profiles for grasping 2D and 3D objects adheres to, or violates, the psychophysical properties of Weber’s law. Specifically, participants grasped differently sized 2D and 3D objects (20, 30, 40, and 50 mm of width) and we computed the just-noticeable-difference scores associated with aperture profiles at decile increments of normalized grasping time. The aperture profiles for 2D objects showed an early through late (i.e., 10% through 90%) adherence to Weber’s law, whereas the late stages of grasping 3D objects (i.e., >50% of grasping time) produced a fundamental violation of the law’s principles. As such, results suggest that grasping a 2D object is a top-down and cognitive task mediated via relative visual information. In contrast, the enriched shape information provided by a 3D object (i.e., stereoscopic vergence and disparity cues) allows for later aperture specification via absolute (Euclidean) visual information. Most notably, our results establish that the dimensional properties of an object influence the visual information mediating motor output, and further indicate that 2D and 3D objects are not representative proxies for one another in understanding the visual control of grasping.
- Published
- 2013
16. Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades
- Author
-
Jeffrey Weiler and Matthew Heath
- Subjects
Adult ,Feedback, Physiological ,Male ,Schedule ,Task switching ,Adolescent ,General Neuroscience ,Neural Inhibition ,Response suppression ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,Oculomotor Muscle ,Oculomotor Muscles ,Oculomotor control ,Reflex ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Saccade ,Saccades ,Humans ,Female ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,Antisaccade task ,Neuroscience - Abstract
The antisaccade task requires the suppression of a reflexive prosaccade (i.e., response suppression) and the remapping of a target location to mirror-symmetrical space (i.e., vector inversion). Moreover, antisaccades are associated with increased activation of cortical oculomotor networks: a finding attributed to the top-down requirements of response suppression and vector inversion. The goal of the present study was to determine if the increased cortical activity associated with antisaccades elicits a residual inhibition of oculomotor planning networks. To that end, each trial in this investigation entailed the onset of a single and exogenously presented target (i.e., archetypical antisaccade task) and participants were instructed to alternate between pro- and antisaccades in blocked and random task-switching schedules. In the blocked schedule, the saccade tasks (i.e., pro- and antisaccades) alternated on every second trial (AABB paradigm) whereas in the random schedule the saccade tasks were pseudo-randomly interleaved on a trial-by-trial basis. Reaction times for task-switch prosaccades were longer and more variable than their task-repetition counterparts, whereas antisaccades did not vary as a function of task-switch and task-repetition trials: a finding that was consistent across blocked and random presentation schedules. In other words, results demonstrate a unidirectional switch-cost for prosaccades. As such, we propose that the top-down processes required to complete an antisaccade results in residual inhibition of oculomotor networks supporting a subsequent prosaccade.
- Published
- 2012
17. Visually and memory-guided grasping: Aperture shaping exhibits a time-dependent scaling to Weber’s law
- Author
-
Gordon Binsted, Scott A. Holmes, Matthew Heath, and Ali Mulla
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Grasping ,genetic structures ,Just-noticeable difference ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Movement ,Differential Threshold ,Stimulus (physiology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Standard deviation ,Feedback ,Memory guided ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Memory ,Stimulus magnitude ,Perception ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Scaling ,Size Perception ,Vision, Ocular ,media_common ,Analysis of Variance ,05 social sciences ,GRASP ,Hand ,Weber’s law ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Memory, Short-Term ,Action ,Law ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
The ‘just noticeable difference’ (JND) represents the minimum amount by which a stimulus must change to produce a noticeable variation in one’s perceptual experience and is related to initial stimulus magnitude (i.e., Weber’s law). The goal of the present study was to determine whether aperture shaping for visually derived and memory-guided grasping elicit a temporally dependent or temporally independent adherence to Weber’s law. Participants were instructed to grasp differently sized objects (20, 30, 40, 50 and 60mm) in conditions wherein vision of the grasping environment was available throughout the response (i.e., closed-loop), when occluded at movement onset (i.e., open-loop), and when occluded for a brief (i.e., 0ms) or longer (i.e., 2000ms) delay in advance of movement onset. Within-participant standard deviations of grip aperture (i.e., the JNDs) computed at decile increments of normalized grasping time were used to determine participant’s sensitivity to detecting changes in object size. Results showed that JNDs increased linearly with increasing object size from 10% to 40% of grasping time; that is, the trial-to-trial stability (i.e., visuomotor certainty) of grip aperture (i.e., the comparator) decreased with increasing object size (i.e., the initial stimulus). However, a null JND/object size scaling was observed during the middle and late stages of the response (i.e., >50% of grasping time). Most notably, the temporal relationship between JNDs and object size scaling was similar across the different visual conditions used here. Thus, our results provide evidence that aperture shaping elicits a time-dependent early, but not late, adherence to the psychophysical principles of Weber’s law.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is determined by the complexity of the sensorimotor transformations mediating the response
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Kristina A. Neely
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Rotation ,Movement ,Mental rotation ,Task (project management) ,Young Adult ,Mental Processes ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Molecular Biology ,Analysis of Variance ,Communication ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Work (physics) ,Motor control ,Function (mathematics) ,Process substitution ,Space Perception ,Imagination ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Analysis of variance ,Psychology ,business ,Rotation (mathematics) ,Algorithm ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Developmental Biology - Abstract
In the visuomotor mental rotation (VMR) task, participants point to a location that deviates from a visual target by a predetermined angle. A seminal investigation of the VMR task reported a linear increase in reaction time (RT) as a function of increasing angle, for 5°, 10°, 15°, 35°, 70°, 105°, and 140° (Georgopoulos and Massey, 1987). This finding led to the development of the mental rotation model (MRM) and the assertion that response preparation is mediated via the imagined rotation of a movement vector. To determine if the MRM can be extrapolated to perceptually familiar angles (e.g., 90° and 180°) within a range of equally spaced angles, we evaluated two independent sets of angles: 5°, 10°, 15°, 35°, 70°, 105°, and 140° (experiment one) and 30°, 60°, 90°, 120°, 150°, 180°, and 210° (experiment two). Consistent with the MRM, experiment one revealed a linear increase in RT as a function of increasing angle; however, a non-linear relation was revealed for experiment two. RTs were fastest for 180°, followed by 30°, 90°, 60°, 150°, 210°, and 120°. Such results demonstrate that response preparation was not uniquely mediated via a mental rotation process. Instead, the present work provides evidence of a temporally demanding and cognitively mediated response substitution process, wherein the computational demands of response preparation are determined by the complexity of the sensorimotor transformations mediating the response.
- Published
- 2010
19. Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is not a function of the angle of rotation
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Kristina A. Neely
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Rotation ,Visual space ,Mental rotation ,Transformation (music) ,Young Adult ,Memory ,Neuropsychologia ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Angle of rotation ,General Neuroscience ,Memoria ,Cognition ,Middle Aged ,Space Perception ,Imagination ,Female ,Cues ,Visual Fields ,Psychology ,Rotation (mathematics) ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The goal of the present investigation was to determine whether the anti-pointing task (i.e., pointing to a location 180 degrees from a visual cue [M. Heath, A. Maraj, A. Gradkowski, G. Binsted, Anti-pointing is mediated by a perceptual bias of target location in left and right visual space, Exp. Brain Res. 192 (2009) 275-286]) and a 90 degrees -rotated-pointing task are supported by a similar cognitive strategy. Previous work evaluating visuomotor mental rotation (MR) has reported a monotonic increase in reaction time (RT) as a function of the angle of rotation [A.P. Georgopoulos, G. Pellizzer, The mental and the neural: psychological and neural studies mental rotation and memory scanning, Neuropsychologia 33 (1995) 1531-1547]. Interestingly, however, anti-pointing movements have not been evaluated in concert with intermediary angles of rotation. We therefore examined RT for center-out pointing movements in four tasks: pro-pointing (PRO), anti-pointing (ANTI), and 90 degrees clockwise (CW90) and counter-clockwise (CCW90) pointing. We found that response latencies for PRO responses were faster than ANTI responses, which in turn were faster than CW90 and CCW90 responses. These findings counter the notion that the angle of rotation influences the speed of visuomotor MR. Instead, we posit that visuomotor MR is supported by a serial process requiring the suppression of a stimulus-driven response followed by voluntary response generation. Further, we suggest that preparation of the voluntary response is cognitively less demanding for the ANTI task because the sensorimotor transformations underlying such an action are completed within the same plane as the stimulus-driven response. In contrast, the cognitive demands associated with CW90 and CCW90 are more complex because the action requires the transformation of response parameters in a movement plane orthogonal to the original - and suppressed - stimulus-driven response.
- Published
- 2009
20. Transitive gesture production in apraxia: Visual and nonvisual sensory contributions
- Author
-
Matthew Heath, Tom A. Schweizer, Mike J. Dixon, David A. Westwood, Sandra E. Black, and Eric A. Roy
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,genetic structures ,Apraxias ,InformationSystems_INFORMATIONINTERFACESANDPRESENTATION(e.g.,HCI) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Object (grammar) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Severity of Illness Index ,Apraxia ,Perceptual Disorders ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Control (linguistics) ,ComputingMethodologies_COMPUTERGRAPHICS ,Haptic technology ,Transitive relation ,Kinesthetic learning ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,body regions ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
The production of transitive limb gestures is optimized when the appropriate tool can be physically manipulated. Little research has addressed the independent contributions of visual and nonvisual sources of sensory information to this phenomenon. In this study, 12 control, 37 LHD, and 50 RHD stroke patients performed transitive limb gestures to pantomime (to verbal command with the object visible) and object manipulation. Performance was more accurate in the object manipulation condition, suggesting that haptic and kinesthetic cues are important for transitive gesture production. Various patterns of performance were observed in the stroke groups, indicating that selective damage to the haptic/kinesthetic processing system is possible and common following unilateral stroke.
- Published
- 2001
21. Patterns of apraxia associated with the production of intransitive limb gestures following left and right hemisphere stroke
- Author
-
David A. Westwood, Sandra E. Black, Matthew Heath, and Eric A. Roy
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,Apraxias ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Severity of Illness Index ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,Functional Laterality ,CONSECUTIVE SAMPLE ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Stroke ,Gestures ,Brain ,Extremities ,medicine.disease ,body regions ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
The model of apraxia proposed by Roy (1996) states that three patterns of apraxia should be observed across pantomime and imitation conditions. In the present analysis the frequency and severity of each pattern of apraxia were examined in a consecutive sample of left-(LHD) and right-hemisphere-damaged (RHD) patients during the production of intransitive limb gestures. The results indicated that a significant proportion of LHD and RHD patients were selectively impaired in formulating the ideational component of intransitive limb gestures.
- Published
- 2001
22. Task Demands and Limb Apraxia in Stroke
- Author
-
Paula A. Square, Eric A. Roy, Kira Barbour, Tom A. Schweizer, Mike J. Dixon, Sandra E. Black, Matthew Heath, Dave Westwood, and Linda Kalbfleisch
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,Apraxias ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Neurological disorder ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Severity of Illness Index ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,Functional Laterality ,Developmental psychology ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Stroke ,Aged ,media_common ,Cognitive disorder ,Limb apraxia ,medicine.disease ,nervous system diseases ,body regions ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Laterality ,Arm ,Imitation ,Psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
The present study was designed to examine the frequency and severity of apraxia in patients with left- or right-hemisphere stroke in both pantomime and imitation conditions and to compare the frequency of apraxia in each stroke group across the three patterns of apraxia described in Roy's model (Roy, 1996). Ninety-nine stroke patients and 15 age-matched healthy adults performed eight transitive gestures to pantomime and to imitation. Gestural performance was quantified as accuracy on five performance dimensions; a composite score, an arithmetic combination of the five performance dimensions, was used as an index of the overall accuracy. Analyses revealed a comparable proportion of patients in each stroke group were classified as apraxic in the imitation condition, but a higher proportion of left stroke patients were apraxic in the pantomime condition. The severity of apraxia in each stroke group and the performance dimensions affected were, however, comparable. Analyses of the patterns of apraxia (pantomime alone, imitation alone or apraxia in both conditions) revealed a higher frequency of apraxia in both stroke groups for the pattern reflecting apraxia in both conditions, indicating that a disruption at the movement execution stage of gesture performance was most common.
- Published
- 2000
23. Cerebral Specialization for Speech Production in Persons with Down Syndrome
- Author
-
Matthew Heath and Digby Elliott
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Speech production ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Down syndrome ,Speech perception ,Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Audiology ,Functional Laterality ,Language and Linguistics ,Lateralization of brain function ,Developmental psychology ,Speech and Hearing ,otorhinolaryngologic diseases ,medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,Analysis of Variance ,Mouth ,Dichotic listening ,Videotape Recording ,medicine.disease ,Dominance (ethology) ,Laterality ,Female ,Down Syndrome ,Psychology - Abstract
The study of cerebral specialization in persons with Down syndrome (DS) has revealed an anomalous pattern of organization. Specifically, dichotic listening studies (e.g., Elliott & Weeks, 1993) have suggested a left ear/right hemisphere dominance for speech perception for persons with DS. In the current investigation, the cerebral dominance for speech production was examined using the mouth asymmetry technique. In right-handed, nonhandicapped subjects, mouth asymmetry methodology has shown that during speech, the right side of the mouth opens sooner and to a larger degree then the left side (Graves, Goodglass, & Landis, 1982). The phenomenon of right mouth asymmetry (RMA) is believed to reflect the direct access that the musculature on the right side of the face has to the left hemisphere's speech production systems. This direct access may facilitate the transfer of innervatory patterns to the muscles on the right side of the face. In the present study, the lateralization for speech production was investigated in 10 right-handed participants with DS and 10 nonhandicapped subjects. A RMA at the initiation and end of speech production occurred for subjects in both groups. Surprisingly, the degree of asymmetry between groups did not differ, suggesting that the lateralization of speech production is similar for persons with and persons without DS. These results support the biological dissociation model (Elliott, Weeks, & Elliott, 1987), which holds that persons with DS display a unique dissociation between speech perception (right hemisphere) and speech production (left hemisphere).
- Published
- 1999
24. The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory
- Author
-
Digby Elliott, Matthew Heath, and Gordon Binsted
- Subjects
Computer science ,Movement (music) ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Control (management) ,Biophysics ,Motor control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Body movement ,General Medicine ,Visual feedback ,Speed accuracy ,Trajectory ,Contrast (vision) ,Orthopedics and Sports Medicine ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,media_common - Abstract
A number of recent models of limb control have attempted to explain speed-accuracy trade-off in goal-directed movements on the basis of the characteristics of the muscular impulses that are specified prior to movement initiation. In contrast, studies from our laboratory have demonstrated that, even for very rapid aiming movements, the characteristics of the movement trajectory change with the availability of visual information about the position of the limb and the target during the movement. Moreover, when the movement of the limb is perturbed at movement initiation by an electromagnetic force, performers can rapidly adjust their aiming movements in order to hit the target if visual feedback is available. The performer can also rapidly adjust to unexpected changes in target size and amplitude. Visually based adjustments to the movement trajectory can be either discrete or continuous. PsycINFO classification: 2330; 2343
- Published
- 1999
25. Visual information and the online control of action: A test of duplex visual processing
- Author
-
Matthew Heath
- Subjects
Visual processing ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Speech recognition ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Duplex (telecommunications) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology - Published
- 2008
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.