26 results on '"Cohen EH"'
Search Results
2. The gene for protein S maps near the centromere of human chromosome 3
- Author
-
Watkins, PC, Eddy, R, Fukushima, Y, Byers, MG, Cohen, EH, Dackowski, WR, Wydro, RM, and Shows, TB
- Abstract
Two different mapping approaches were used to determine the human chromosomal location of the gene for protein S. A human protein S cDNA was used as a hybridization probe to analyze a panel of somatic cell hybrids containing different human chromosomes. Cosegregation of protein S-specific DNA restriction fragments with human chromosome 3 was observed. Three cell hybrids containing only a portion of chromosome 3 were analyzed in order to further localize protein S. Based on the somatic cell hybrid analysis, protein S is assigned to a region of chromosome 3 that contains a small part of the long arm and short arm of the chromosome including the centromere (3p21----3q21). In situ hybridization of the protein S cDNA probe to human metaphase chromosomes permitted a precise localization of protein S to the region of chromosome 3 immediately surrounding the centromere (3p11.1---- 3q11.2). Protein S is the first protein involved in blood coagulation that has been mapped to human chromosome 3.
- Published
- 1988
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Neural mechanisms of object-based attention.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Tong F
- Subjects
- Adult, Brain Mapping, Computer Simulation, Face, Feedback, Physiological, Housing, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Models, Neurological, Multivariate Analysis, Photic Stimulation, Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Visual Cortex physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
What neural mechanisms underlie the ability to attend to a complex object in the presence of competing overlapping stimuli? We evaluated whether object-based attention might involve pattern-specific feedback to early visual areas to selectively enhance the set of low-level features corresponding to the attended object. Using fMRI and multivariate pattern analysis, we found that activity patterns in early visual areas (V1-V4) are strongly biased in favor of the attended object. Activity patterns evoked by single faces and single houses reliably predicted which of the 2 overlapping stimulus types was being attended with high accuracy (80-90% correct). Superior knowledge of upright objects led to improved attentional selection in early areas. Across individual blocks, the strength of the attentional bias signal in early visual areas was highly predictive of the modulations found in high-level object areas, implying that pattern-specific attentional filtering at early sites can determine the quality of object-specific signals that reach higher level visual areas. Through computational modeling, we show how feedback of an average template to V1-like units can improve discrimination of exemplars belonging to the attended category. Our findings provide a mechanistic account of how feedback to early visual areas can contribute to the attentional selection of complex objects., (© The Author 2013. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Extended glycan diversity in a bacterial protein glycosylation system linked to allelic polymorphisms and minimal genetic alterations in a glycosyltransferase gene.
- Author
-
Børud B, Anonsen JH, Viburiene R, Cohen EH, Samuelsen AB, and Koomey M
- Subjects
- Alleles, Amino Acid Sequence, Amino Acid Substitution, Glycosylation, Molecular Sequence Data, Mutagenesis, Site-Directed, Mutant Proteins genetics, Mutant Proteins metabolism, Neisseria genetics, Phylogeny, Sequence Homology, Amino Acid, Bacterial Proteins metabolism, Glycosyltransferases genetics, Glycosyltransferases metabolism, Neisseria enzymology, Neisseria metabolism, Polysaccharides metabolism
- Abstract
Glycans manifest in conjunction with the broad spectrum O-linked protein glycosylation in species within the genus Neisseria display intra- and interstrain diversity. Variability in glycan structure and antigenicity are attributable to differences in the content and expression status of glycan synthesis genes. Given the high degree of standing allelic polymorphisms in these genes, the level of glycan diversity may exceed that currently defined. Here, we identify unique protein-associated disaccharide glycoforms that carry N-acetylglucosamine (GlcNAc) at their non-reducing end. This altered structure was correlated with allelic variants of pglH whose product was previously demonstrated to be responsible for the expression of glucose (Glc)-containing disaccharides. Allele comparisons and site-specific mutagenesis showed that the presence of a single residue, alanine at position 303 in place of a glutamine, was sufficient for GlcNAc versus Glc incorporation. Phylogenetic analyses revealed that GlcNAc-containing disaccharides may be widely distributed within the pgl systems of Neisseria particularly in strains of N. meningitidis. Although analogous minimal structural alterations in glycosyltransferases have been documented in association with lipopolysaccharide and capsular polysaccharide variability, this appears to be the first example in which such changes have been implicated in glycan diversification within a bacterial protein glycosylation system., (© 2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.)
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Symmetry in context: salience of mirror symmetry in natural patterns.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Zaidi Q
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Models, Theoretical, Photic Stimulation methods, Sensory Thresholds physiology, Young Adult, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology
- Abstract
Symmetry is a biologically relevant, mathematically involving, and aesthetically compelling visual phenomenon. Mirror symmetry detection is considered particularly rapid and efficient, based on experiments with random noise. Symmetry detection in natural settings, however, is often accomplished against structured backgrounds. To measure salience of symmetry in diverse contexts, we assembled mirror symmetric patterns from 101 natural textures. Temporal thresholds for detecting the symmetry axis ranged from 28 to 568 ms indicating a wide range of salience (1/Threshold). We built a model for estimating symmetry-energy by connecting pairs of mirror-symmetric filters that simulated cortical receptive fields. The model easily identified the axis of symmetry for all patterns. However, symmetry-energy quantified at this axis correlated weakly with salience. To examine context effects on symmetry detection, we used the same model to estimate approximate symmetry resulting from the underlying texture throughout the image. Magnitudes of approximate symmetry at flanking and orthogonal axes showed strong negative correlations with salience, revealing context interference with symmetry detection. A regression model that included the context-based measures explained the salience results, and revealed why perceptual symmetry can differ from mathematical characterizations. Using natural patterns thus produces new insights into symmetry perception and its possible neural circuits.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. The utility of shape attributes in deciphering movements of non-rigid objects.
- Author
-
Cohen EH, Jain A, and Zaidi Q
- Subjects
- Depth Perception, Humans, Psychophysics, Discrimination, Psychological physiology, Form Perception physiology, Motion Perception physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology
- Abstract
Most moving objects in the world are non-rigid, changing shape as they move. To disentangle shape changes from movements, computational models either fit shapes to combinations of basis shapes or motion trajectories to combinations of oscillations but are biologically unfeasible in their input requirements. Recent neural models parse shapes into stored examples, which are unlikely to exist for general shapes. We propose that extracting shape attributes, e.g., symmetry, facilitates veridical perception of non-rigid motion. In a new method, identical dots were moved in and out along invisible spokes, to simulate the rotation of dynamically and randomly distorting shapes. Discrimination of rotation direction measured as a function of non-rigidity was 90% as efficient as the optimal Bayesian rotation decoder and ruled out models based on combining the strongest local motions. Remarkably, for non-rigid symmetric shapes, observers outperformed the Bayesian model when perceived rotation could correspond only to rotation of global symmetry, i.e., when tracking of shape contours or local features was uninformative. That extracted symmetry can drive perceived motion suggests that shape attributes may provide links across the dorsal-ventral separation between motion and shape processing. Consequently, the perception of non-rigid object motion could be based on representations that highlight global shape attributes.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. A human monoclonal antibody against insulin-like growth factor-II blocks the growth of human hepatocellular carcinoma cell lines in vitro and in vivo.
- Author
-
Dransfield DT, Cohen EH, Chang Q, Sparrow LG, Bentley JD, Dolezal O, Xiao X, Peat TS, Newman J, Pilling PA, Phan T, Priebe I, Brierley GV, Kastrapeli N, Kopacz K, Martik D, Wassaf D, Rank D, Conley G, Huang Y, Adams TE, and Cosgrove L
- Subjects
- Animals, Antibodies, Monoclonal pharmacology, Cell Adhesion drug effects, Cell Line, Tumor, Cell Proliferation drug effects, Disease Progression, Humans, Immunohistochemistry, Mice, Signal Transduction drug effects, Tumor Stem Cell Assay, Xenograft Model Antitumor Assays, Antibodies, Monoclonal therapeutic use, Carcinoma, Hepatocellular drug therapy, Carcinoma, Hepatocellular pathology, Insulin-Like Growth Factor II immunology, Liver Neoplasms drug therapy, Liver Neoplasms pathology
- Abstract
Elevated expression of insulin-like growth factor-II (IGF-II) is frequently observed in a variety of human malignancies, including breast, colon, and liver cancer. As IGF-II can deliver a mitogenic signal through both IGF-IR and an alternately spliced form of the insulin receptor (IR-A), neutralizing the biological activity of this growth factor directly is a potential alternative option to IGF-IR-directed agents. Using a Fab-displaying phage library and a biotinylated precursor form of IGF-II (1-104 amino acids) as a target, we isolated Fabs specific for the E-domain COOH-terminal extension form of IGF-II and for mature IGF-II. One of these Fabs that bound to both forms of IGF-II was reformatted into a full-length IgG, expressed, purified, and subjected to further analysis. This antibody (DX-2647) displayed a very high affinity for IGF-II/IGF-IIE (K(D) value of 49 and 10 pmol/L, respectively) compared with IGF-I (approximately 10 nmol/L) and blocked binding of IGF-II to IGF-IR, IR-A, a panel of insulin-like growth factor-binding proteins, and the mannose-6-phosphate receptor. A crystal complex of the parental Fab of DX-2647 bound to IGF-II was resolved to 2.2 A. DX-2647 inhibited IGF-II and, to a lesser extent, IGF-I-induced receptor tyrosine phosphorylation, cellular proliferation, and both anchorage-dependent and anchorage-independent colony formation in various cell lines. In addition, DX-2647 slowed tumor progression in the Hep3B xenograft model, causing decreased tumoral CD31 staining as well as reduced IGF-IIE and IGF-IR phosphorylation levels. Therefore, DX-2647 offers an alternative approach to targeting IGF-IR, blocking IGF-II signaling through both IGF-IR and IR-A.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Crystallization and preliminary X-ray analysis of the complexes between a Fab and two forms of human insulin-like growth factor II.
- Author
-
Newman J, Cohen EH, Cosgrove L, Kopacz K, Dransfield DT, Adams TE, and Peat TS
- Subjects
- Crystallization, Crystallography, X-Ray, Glycosylation, Humans, Protein Isoforms chemistry, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments chemistry, Insulin-Like Growth Factor II chemistry
- Abstract
Elevated expression of insulin-like growth factor II (IGF-II) is frequently observed in a variety of human malignancies, including breast, colon and liver cancer. As IGF-II can deliver a mitogenic signal through both the type 1 insulin-like growth factor receptor (IGF-IR) and an alternately spliced form of the insulin receptor (IR-A), neutralizing the biological activity of this growth factor directly is an attractive therapeutic option. One method of doing this would be to find antibodies that bind tightly and specifically to the peptide, which could be used as protein therapeutics to lower the peptide levels in vivo and/or to block the peptide from binding to the IGF-IR or IR-A. To address this, Fabs were selected from a phage-display library using a biotinylated precursor form of the growth factor known as IGF-IIE as a target. Fabs were isolated that were specific for the E-domain C-terminal extension and for mature IGF-II. Four Fabs selected from the library were produced, complexed with IGF-II and set up in crystallization trials. One of the Fab-IGF-II complexes (M64-F02-IGF-II) crystallized readily, yielding crystals that diffracted to 2.2 A resolution and belonged to space group P2(1)2(1)2(1), with unit-cell parameters a = 50.7, b = 106.9, c = 110.7 A. There was one molecule of the complete complex in the asymmetric unit. The same Fab was also crystallized with a longer form of the growth factor, IGF-IIE. This complex crystallized in space group P2(1)2(1)2(1), with unit-cell parameters a = 50.7, b = 107, c = 111.5 A, and also diffracted X-rays to 2.2 A resolution.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Perceptual segmentation and the perceived orientation of dot clusters: the role of robust statistics.
- Author
-
Cohen EH, Singh M, and Maloney LT
- Subjects
- Humans, Photic Stimulation, Form Perception physiology, Models, Statistical, Orientation physiology
- Abstract
We investigated perceptual segmentation in the context of a perceived-orientation task. Stimuli were dot clusters formed by the union of a large elliptical sub-cluster and a secondary circular sub-cluster. We manipulated the separation between the two sub-clusters, their common dot density, and the size of the secondary sub-cluster. As the separation between sub-clusters increased, the orientation perceived by observers shifted gradually from the global principal axis of the entire cluster to that of the main sub-cluster alone. Thus, with increasing separation, the dots within the secondary sub-cluster were assigned systematically lower weights in the principal-axis computation. In addition, this shift occurred at smaller separations for higher dot densities-consistent with the idea that reliable segmentation is possible with smaller separations when the dot density is high. We propose that the visual system employs a robust statistical estimator in this task and that data points are weighted differentially based on the likelihood that they arose from a separate generative process. However, unlike in standard robust estimation, weights based on residuals are insufficient to characterize human segmentation. Rather, these must be computed based on more comprehensive generative models of dot clusters.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Fundamental failures of shape constancy resulting from cortical anisotropy.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Zaidi Q
- Subjects
- Algorithms, Bias, Cues, Humans, Models, Neurological, Neural Inhibition physiology, Neural Pathways cytology, Neural Pathways physiology, Neurons physiology, Neuropsychological Tests, Photic Stimulation, Psychometrics, Visual Cortex cytology, Anisotropy, Illusions physiology, Orientation physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Space Perception physiology, Visual Cortex physiology
- Abstract
Contrary to the conventional assumption that humans perceive shapes of rigid objects as constant despite retinal-image variations caused by changes in orientation and position, we show that the depths of three-dimensional (3-D) textured shapes appear to vary when the image is rotated. In paired comparisons of static stimuli, depth amplitude was perceived to be greater at vertical than at oblique orientations. A similar oblique bias was found for simple two-dimensional (2-D) obtuse angles. Using projective geometry to link angle magnitude to the orientation flows that convey 3-D shape from texture cues, we show quantitatively that the 2-D bias predicts the 3-D bias. Our finding that perception of angular magnitude is dependent on orientation has broad implications for shape constancy because orientation flows have also been implicated in 3-D perception from reflections and shading, and contour curvature is fundamental to uncovering depth and part-structure of shapes. We examined the roles played in the observed biases by anisotropies in numbers and tuning widths of orientation-tuned cells in striate cortex as well as the distribution of oriented energy in natural scenes. An optimal stimulus decoding model for 2-D angles revealed that the narrower tuning of cells for horizontal orientations combined with cross-orientation inhibition explains the orientation-dependent angle distortion and hence the 3-D shape inconstancy. Variations in properties within neural populations can thus have direct effects on visual perceptions and need to be included in neural decoding models.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Geometric determinants of shape segmentation: tests using segment identification.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Singh M
- Subjects
- Adult, Humans, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Perceptual Closure, Perceptual Masking, Photic Stimulation methods, Psychophysics, Form Perception
- Abstract
The geometric determinants of shape decomposition were studied using a performance-based method. Observers' identification of contour segments was shown to be systematically modulated by their curvature properties, and by the geometric properties of the enclosed region. Specifically, negative minima of contour curvature provided the best segment boundaries. Segments with negative-minima boundaries were identified with greater accuracy than those with positive maxima or inflection boundaries of comparable length. Additionally, segment identification was shown to be determined by contour length, the turning angle at part boundaries, and the width at the part's base (hence the part's protrusion). The results indicate that part decomposition is an automatic process. Moreover, this process is graded, i.e. parts are more strongly segmented, or more likely to be perceived, according to the strength of many geometric determinants.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. The relationship between spatial pooling and attention in saccadic and perceptual tasks.
- Author
-
Cohen EH, Schnitzer BS, Gersch TM, Singh M, and Kowler E
- Subjects
- Color Perception physiology, Fixation, Ocular physiology, Humans, Judgment, Models, Neurological, Perceptual Masking physiology, Photic Stimulation methods, Psychometrics, Reaction Time, Attention physiology, Saccades physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
Saccades aimed at spatially extended targets land reliably at central locations determined by pooling information across the target shape [Melcher, D., & Kowler, E. (1999). Shape, surfaces and saccades. Vision Research, 39, 2929-2946; Vishwanath, D., & Kowler, E. (2003). Localization of shapes: Eye movements and perception compared. Vision Research, 43, 1637-1653]. Previous findings of saccadic errors when attempting to look at a target in the midst of distractors encouraged suggestions that pooling occurs indiscriminately, with little or no influence of a selective filter to eliminate the influence of nearby distractors. To determine the effectiveness of filtering, saccadic localization was studied for saccades made to a set of target elements (discs) interleaved with an equivalent set of distractors of a different color. With such interleaved elements, selection and spatial pooling are constrained to occur over the same spatial region. The results showed that filtering was effective and saccadic landing position was determined mainly by the target elements. Concurrent perceptual judgments made about the same stimuli (estimating the mean size of either target or distractor discs) showed better performance for the target discs than distractors, confirming that perceptual attention was allocated to the set of target elements. These results: (1) support the role of attention in setting the input to the spatial pooling process that guides saccades to spatially extended targets, and (2) show that perceptual judgments of mean value, often thought to impose modest attentional demands, are not immune to the constraints of this pre-saccadic filter.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Identification and characterization of a human monoclonal antagonistic antibody AL-57 that preferentially binds the high-affinity form of lymphocyte function-associated antigen-1.
- Author
-
Huang L, Shimaoka M, Rondon IJ, Roy I, Chang Q, Po M, Dransfield DT, Ladner RC, Edge AS, Salas A, Wood CR, Springer TA, and Cohen EH
- Subjects
- Antibodies, Monoclonal chemistry, Antigen-Antibody Reactions, Binding Sites, Cell Adhesion drug effects, Cell Line, Cell Proliferation drug effects, Humans, Immunoglobulin G pharmacology, Intercellular Adhesion Molecule-1 drug effects, Keratinocytes drug effects, Lymphocyte Function-Associated Antigen-1 immunology, Molecular Sequence Data, Phytohemagglutinins antagonists & inhibitors, Phytohemagglutinins pharmacology, Structure-Activity Relationship, Antibodies, Monoclonal pharmacology, Leukocytes, Mononuclear drug effects, Lymphocyte Function-Associated Antigen-1 drug effects
- Abstract
LFA-1 (alpha(L)beta(2)) mediates cell-cell and cell-extracellular matrix adhesions essential for immune and inflammatory responses. One critical mechanism regulating LFA-1 activity is the conformational change of the ligand-binding alpha(L) I domain from low-affinity (LA), closed form, to the high-affinity (HA), open form. Most known integrin antagonists bind both forms. Antagonists specific for the HA alpha(L) I domain have not been described. Here, we report the identification and characterization of a human antibody AL-57, which binds to the alpha(L) I domain in a HA but not LA conformation. AL-57 was discovered by selection from a human Fab-displaying library using a locked-open HA I domain as target. AL-57 Fab-phage bound HA I domain-expressing K562 cells (HA cells) in a Mg(2+)-dependent manner. AL-57 IgG also bound HA cells and PBMCs, activated by Mg(2+)/EGTA, PMA, or DTT. The binding profile of AL-57 IgG on PBMCs was the same as that of ICAM-1, the main ligand of LFA-1. In contrast, an anti-alpha(L) murine mAb MHM24 did not distinguish between the HA and LA forms. Moreover, AL-57 IgG blocked ICAM-1 binding to HA cells with a potency greater than MHM24. It also inhibited ICAM-1 binding to PBMCs, blocked adhesion of HA cells to keratinocytes, and inhibited PHA-induced lymphocyte proliferation with potencies comparable with MHM24. These results indicate that specifically targeting the HA I domain is sufficient to inhibit LFA-1-mediated, adhesive functions. AL-57 represents a therapeutic candidate for treatment of inflammatory and autoimmune diseases.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. AL-57, a ligand-mimetic antibody to integrin LFA-1, reveals chemokine-induced affinity up-regulation in lymphocytes.
- Author
-
Shimaoka M, Kim M, Cohen EH, Yang W, Astrof N, Peer D, Salas A, Ferrand A, and Springer TA
- Subjects
- Amino Acid Sequence, Animals, Antibodies, Monoclonal genetics, Binding Sites, Cell Line, Chemokine CXCL12, Epitopes, Humans, Ligands, Lymphocyte Function-Associated Antigen-1 chemistry, Lymphocyte Function-Associated Antigen-1 genetics, Molecular Sequence Data, Protein Binding, Protein Structure, Tertiary, Surface Plasmon Resonance, Up-Regulation, Antibodies, Monoclonal metabolism, Antibody Affinity, Chemokines, CXC metabolism, Lymphocyte Function-Associated Antigen-1 metabolism, Lymphocytes metabolism
- Abstract
Affinity of integrin lymphocyte function-associated antigen 1 (LFA-1) is enhanced by conformational changes from the low-affinity closed form to the high-affinity (HA) open form of the ligand-binding inserted (I) domain as shown by work with purified I domains. However, affinity up-regulation of LFA-1 on the cell surface by physiological agonists such as chemokines has yet to be demonstrated by monovalent reagents. We characterize a mAb, AL-57 (activated LFA-1 clone 57), that has been developed by phage display that selectively targets the HA open conformation of the LFA-1 I domain. AL-57 discriminates among low-affinity, intermediate-affinity, and HA states of LFA-1. Furthermore, AL-57 functions as a ligand mimetic that binds only upon activation and requires Mg2+ for binding. Compared with the natural ligand intercellular adhesion molecule-1, AL-57 shows a tighter binding to the open I domain and a 250-fold slower off rate. Monovalent Fab AL-57 demonstrates affinity increases on a subset (approximately 10%) of lymphocyte cell surface LFA-1 molecules upon stimulation with CXCL-12 (CXC chemokine ligand 12). Affinity up-regulation correlates with global conformational changes of LFA-1 to the extended form. Affinity increase stimulated by CXCL-12 is transient and peaks 2 to 5 min after stimulation.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Perceived orientation of complex shape reflects graded part decomposition.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Singh M
- Subjects
- Adult, Humans, Mathematics, Size Perception physiology, Form Perception physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
Although the orientation of line segments and simple shapes is a well-studied area of vision, little is known about geometric factors that influence perceived orientation of complex multipart shapes. The study of these factors is of interest because it allows for an insight into the basic problem of how local geometric attributes are integrated perceptually into a global shape representation. We examined the perceived orientation of two-part shapes using an adjustment method and a 2AFC task. In particular, we investigated the influence of the perceptual salience, or distinctiveness, of a part--as defined by the turning angles at its boundaries--and its area relative to the main "base" part. In contrast to previous results on simple shapes, our results exhibited large and systematic deviations of perceived orientation from the principal axis of the shape. For shapes with sharp part boundaries, perceived global orientation deviated maximally from the principal axis and was approximated by the axis of the main base part of the shape. With weakening part boundaries, the perceived orientation gradually approached the principal axis of the entire shape, reflecting that both parts were taken into account in estimating orientation. The results are consistent with a differentially weighted principal-axis computation in which the attached part is given systematically lower weighting with increasing turning angles at the part boundaries. They thus allow a quantitative characterization of part salience in terms of part independence: Turning angles at a part's boundaries determine the extent to which its influence is perceptually separable from the rest of the shape. We suggest that Robust Statistics may provide a useful framework for quantifying the influence of part segmentation on visual estimation.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Oligonucleotide-assisted cleavage and ligation: a novel directional DNA cloning technology to capture cDNAs. Application in the construction of a human immune antibody phage-display library.
- Author
-
Schoonbroodt S, Frans N, DeSouza M, Eren R, Priel S, Brosh N, Ben-Porath J, Zauberman A, Ilan E, Dagan S, Cohen EH, Hoogenboom HR, Ladner RC, and Hoet RM
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Animals, Antibodies, Monoclonal immunology, Antibody Specificity, Biotechnology methods, Candida albicans enzymology, Candida albicans immunology, Female, Glyceraldehyde-3-Phosphate Dehydrogenases immunology, Humans, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments chemistry, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments immunology, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments isolation & purification, Immunoglobulin Heavy Chains chemistry, Immunoglobulin Heavy Chains genetics, Immunoglobulin Variable Region genetics, Mice, Mice, Inbred BALB C, Middle Aged, Oligonucleotides metabolism, Polymerase Chain Reaction, Sequence Analysis, DNA, Cloning, Molecular methods, DNA, Complementary, Genes, Immunoglobulin, Oligonucleotides chemistry, Peptide Library
- Abstract
The use of oligonucleotide-assisted cleavage and ligation (ONCL), a novel approach to the capture of gene repertoires, in the construction of a phage-display immune antibody library is described. ONCL begins with rapid amplification of cDNA ends to amplify all members equally. A single, specific cut near 5' and/or 3' end of each gene fragment (in single stranded form) is facilitated by hybridization with an appropriate oligonucleotide adapter. Directional cloning of targeted DNA is accomplished by ligation of a partially duplex DNA molecule (containing suitable restriction sites) and amplification with primers in constant regions. To demonstrate utility and reliability of ONCL, a human antibody repertoire was cloned from IgG mRNA extracted from human B-lymphocytes engrafted in Trimera mice. These mice were transplanted with peripheral blood lymphocytes from Candida albicans infected individuals and subsequently immunized with C.albicans glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH). DNA sequencing showed that ONCL resulted in efficient capture of gene repertoires. Indeed, full representation of all V(H) families/segments was observed showing that ONCL did not introduce cloning biases for or against any V(H) family. We validated the efficiency of ONCL by creating a functional Fab phage-display library with a size of 3.3 x 10(10) and by selecting five unique Fabs against GAPDH antigen.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. What change detection tells us about the visual representation of shape.
- Author
-
Cohen EH, Barenholtz E, Singh M, and Feldman J
- Subjects
- Humans, Signal Detection, Psychological, Form Perception physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology
- Abstract
Many recent findings suggest that human observers are surprisingly "blind" to changes in visual displays, failing to notice when substantial scene elements are added, subtracted, or altered in successive presentations of the scene. But observers are far more sensitive to certain visual changes than others, and we suggest that which types of changes enjoy differential sensitivity can reveal a great deal about the underlying visual representations. In this study, we investigate how the human visual system represents the shape of objects by demonstrating a previously unknown influence on detection of changes in shape: the sign of contour curvature. We show that subjects are substantially more sensitive to changes in concave regions of a shape's contour than to changes in convex regions, even when these changes do not alter the number or location of parts. Further, we show that this effect is modulated by figure-ground assignment, so that changes to the same physical contour are more or less detectable, depending on the contour's perceived figural status, which determines whether the change falls in a concave or convex region. The results demonstrate a heightened sensitivity for changes at concavities that is not reducible to a sensitivity to changes in gross part structure.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Generation of high-affinity human antibodies by combining donor-derived and synthetic complementarity-determining-region diversity.
- Author
-
Hoet RM, Cohen EH, Kent RB, Rookey K, Schoonbroodt S, Hogan S, Rem L, Frans N, Daukandt M, Pieters H, van Hegelsom R, Neer NC, Nastri HG, Rondon IJ, Leeds JA, Hufton SE, Huang L, Kashin I, Devlin M, Kuang G, Steukers M, Viswanathan M, Nixon AE, Sexton DJ, Hoogenboom HR, and Ladner RC
- Subjects
- Genetic Variation genetics, Humans, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments genetics, Protein Binding, Recombination, Genetic genetics, Tissue Donors, Antibody Affinity, Antibody Formation, Complementarity Determining Regions genetics, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments biosynthesis, Immunoglobulin Fab Fragments immunology, Peptide Library, Protein Engineering methods
- Abstract
Combinatorial libraries of rearranged hypervariable V(H) and V(L) sequences from nonimmunized human donors contain antigen specificities, including anti-self reactivities, created by random pairing of V(H)s and V(L)s. Somatic hypermutation of immunoglobulin genes, however, is critical in the generation of high-affinity antibodies in vivo and occurs only after immunization. Thus, in combinatorial phage display libraries from nonimmunized donors, high-affinity antibodies are rarely found. Lengthy in vitro affinity maturation is often needed to improve antibodies from such libraries. We report the construction of human Fab libraries having a unique combination of immunoglobulin sequences captured from human donors and synthetic diversity in key antigen contact sites in heavy-chain complementarity-determining regions 1 and 2. The success of this strategy is demonstrated by identifying many monovalent Fabs against multiple therapeutic targets that show higher affinities than approved therapeutic antibodies. This very often circumvents the need for affinity maturation, accelerating discovery of antibody drug candidates.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. An alternatively processed mRNA specific for gamma-glutamyl transpeptidase in human tissues.
- Author
-
Pawlak A, Cohen EH, Octave JN, Schweickhardt R, Wu SJ, Bulle F, Chikhi N, Baik JH, Siegrist S, and Guellaën G
- Subjects
- Adult, Amino Acid Sequence, Base Sequence, Carcinoma, Hepatocellular, Cell Line, Cloning, Molecular, DNA genetics, Female, Fetus, Gene Library, Genes, Humans, Liver enzymology, Liver Neoplasms, Microsomes enzymology, Molecular Sequence Data, Placenta enzymology, Polymerase Chain Reaction, Polyribosomes metabolism, Pregnancy, Sequence Homology, Nucleic Acid, RNA Splicing, RNA, Messenger genetics, gamma-Glutamyltransferase genetics
- Abstract
Human gamma-glutamyl transpeptidase (GGT)1 is composed of two subunits derived from a single precursor (Nash, B., and Tate, S.S. (1984) J. Biol. Chem. 259, 678-685; Finidori, J., Laperche, Y., Tsapis, R., Barouki, R., Guellaën, G., and Hanoune, J. (1984) J. Biol. Chem. 259, 4687-4690) consisting of 569 amino acids (Laperche, Y., Bulle, F., Aissani, T., Chobert, M.N., Aggerbeck, M., Hanoune, J., and Guellaën, G. (1986) Proc Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 83, 937-941). In the present study we report the cloning of an altered form of this precursor from human liver. We have isolated two clones, one 2,632 base pairs (bp) long from a fetal liver cDNA library and one 926 bp long from an adult liver cDNA library, each containing a 22-bp insertion that introduces a premature stop codon and shortens the open reading frame to 1,098 bp when compared with known human cDNA sequences specific for GGT. Sequence analysis of a human genomic GGT clone shows that this insertion of 22 bp is generated by a splicing event involving an alternative 3'-acceptor site. By polymerase chain reaction experiments we demonstrate that the alternatively spliced mRNA is present in polysomes from the microsomal fraction of a human hepatoma cell line (Hep G2) and thus could encode an altered GGT molecule of 39,300 Da (366 amino acids) encompassing most of the heavy subunit which is normally 41,500 Da (380 amino acids). The altered mRNA is detected in various human tissues including liver, kidney, brain, intestine, stomach, placenta, and mammary gland. This report is the first demonstration of an alternative primary sequence in the mRNA coding for GGT, a finding that could be related to the presence of some inactive forms of GGT detected in human tissues.
- Published
- 1990
20. Gamma-glutamyl transferase locus (GGT) displays a PvuII polymorphism.
- Author
-
Rouleau GA, Bazanowski A, Cohen EH, Guellaen G, and Gusella JF
- Subjects
- Chromosomes, Human, Pair 22, Humans, Deoxyribonucleases, Type II Site-Specific, Polymorphism, Genetic, Polymorphism, Restriction Fragment Length, gamma-Glutamyltransferase genetics
- Published
- 1988
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Sequences responsible for transcription termination on a gene segment in Saccharomyces cerevisiae.
- Author
-
Henikoff S and Cohen EH
- Subjects
- Animals, Base Sequence, Chromosome Deletion, DNA genetics, Drosophila melanogaster genetics, Mutation, Nucleic Acid Hybridization, Plasmids, RNA, Messenger genetics, Genes, Fungal, Saccharomyces cerevisiae genetics, Transcription, Genetic
- Abstract
We have mapped a signal sequence for mRNA 3'-end formation in Saccharomyces cerevisiae by using a Drosophila melanogaster DNA segment that complements a yeast adenine-8 mutation. That the 3' end of the transcript in S. cerevisiae nearly coincides with that in D. melanogaster is consistent with the possibility that mRNA termini are similarly determined in both organisms. Deletion analysis reveals that the complete signal is no more than 21 base pairs long. Part of the signal is the sequence TTTTTATA, which is seen in the termination region of several yeast genes. TTTTTATA appears to be able to act autonomously as a partial termination signal. The efficiency of the complete signal is affected by substitution of sequences downstream from it. This modulation of the effect of a signal is consistent with termination in S. cerevisiae, resembling rho-dependent termination in bacteria.
- Published
- 1984
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Detection and location of three simple sequence DNAs in polytene chromosomes from virilis group species of Drosophila.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Bowman SC
- Subjects
- Animals, Base Sequence, Chromosomes, DNA, Satellite metabolism, Mitosis, Nucleic Acid Hybridization, RNA chemical synthesis, RNA metabolism, DNA, Satellite analysis, Drosophila genetics
- Abstract
In vitro synthesized RNAs complementary to the three satellite DNAs of Drosophila virilis have been used in a series of in situ hybridization experiments with polytene chromosomes from virilis group species. Gall and Atherton (1974) demonstrated that each of the satellites of D. virilis is comprised of many repeats of a distinct, seven base pair long, simple sequence. With few exceptions, copies of each of these simple sequences are detected in the chromocenters of all virilis group species. This is true even in species which do not possess satellite DNAs at buoyant densities corresponding to those of the satellite DNAs of D. virilis. Small quantities of the three simple sequences are also detected in euchromatic arms of several different species. The same euchromatic location may contain detectable copies of one, two, or all three simple sequence DNAs. The amounts of simple sequences at each location in the euchromatin may vary between species, between different stocks of the same species, and even between individuals of the same stock. The simple sequences located in the euchromatin appear to undergo DNA replication during formation of polytene chromosomes unlike those in heterochromatin. The locations of the euchromatic sequences are not the results of single chromosomal inversion events involving heterochromatic and euchromatic breakpoints.
- Published
- 1979
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Analysis of DNAs from two species of the virilis group of Drosophila and implications for satellite DNA evolution.
- Author
-
Cohen EH and Kaplan GC
- Subjects
- Animals, Chromosome Banding, Nucleic Acid Denaturation, Nucleic Acid Hybridization, Species Specificity, Biological Evolution, DNA genetics, DNA, Satellite genetics, Drosophila genetics
- Abstract
The DNAs from two virilis group species of Drosophila, D. lummei and D. kanekoi, have been analyzed. D. lummei DNA has a major satellite which, on the basis of CsCl equilibrium centrifugation, thermal denaturation, renaturation and in situ hybridization is identical to D. virilis satellite I. D. kanekoi DNA has a major satellite at the same buoyant density in neutral CsCl gradients as satellite III of D. virilis. However, on the basis of alkaline CsCl gradients, the satellite contains a major and a minor component, neither one of which is identical to D. virilis satellite III. By in situ hybridization experiments, sequences complementary to the major component of the D. kanekoi satellite are detected in only some species and in a way not consistent with the phylogeny of the group. However, by filter hybridization experiments using nick-translated D. kanekoi satellite as well as D. lummei satellite I and D. virilis satellite III DNAs as probes, homologous sequences are detected in the DNAs of all virilis group species. Surprisingly, sequences homologous to these satellite DNAs are detected in DNAs from non-virilis group Drosophila species as well as from yeast, sea urchin, Xenopus and mouse.
- Published
- 1982
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Two anticipations of Henley's "Invictus.".
- Author
-
Cohen EH
- Subjects
- History, Modern 1601-, United Kingdom, Bone Diseases history, Literature history
- Published
- 1974
25. Effect of testosterone propionate upon the incorporation of labelled glycine into homogenates of mouse kidney.
- Author
-
FRIEDEN EH and COHEN EH
- Subjects
- Biochemical Phenomena, Fabaceae, Glycine metabolism, Kidney metabolism, Testosterone pharmacology, Testosterone Propionate
- Published
- 1958
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Reptitive DNA sequences in drosophila.
- Author
-
Gall JG, Cohen EH, and Polan ML
- Subjects
- Animals, Cell Nucleus, Centrifugation, Density Gradient, Chromosomes, Diploidy, Drosophila, Hybridization, Genetic, Mitosis, RNA, Salivary Glands cytology, Sex Chromosomes, Species Specificity, Chromosome Mapping, DNA
- Published
- 1971
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.