56 results on '"Sophie Wuerger"'
Search Results
2. Are Spatial Chromatic Contrast Sensitivity Band-pass or Lowpass Functions?
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Rafal Mantiuk, Ming Ronnier Luo, Marcel P. Lucassen, Dragan Sekulovski, Qiang Xu, and Stephen Westland
- Subjects
Physics ,Chromatic contrast ,Optics ,Band-pass filter ,business.industry ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,business - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Predicting the colour associated with odours using an electronic nose
- Author
-
Alan G. Marshall, Ryan J. Ward, Sophie Wuerger, and Shammi Rahman
- Subjects
Materials science ,Electronic nose ,genetic structures ,business.industry ,fungi ,food and beverages ,Pattern recognition ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,psychological phenomena and processes - Abstract
Predicting olfactory perception with an electronic nose can aid in the design and evaluation of olfactory-based experiences. We investigate whether the human perception of odours can be predicted outside the bounds of perceived pleasantness and semantic descriptors. We tuned an electronic nose to predict an odour's colour in the CIELAB colour space using human judgements. This revealed that the crossmodal associations people have towards colours could be predicted. Our electronic nose system can predict an odour's colour with a 70 – 81% machine-human similarity rating. These findings suggest a systematic and predictable link exists between the chemical features of odours and the colour associated to them. These findings highlight the possibilities of predicting human olfactory perception using an electronic nose.
- Published
- 2021
4. Decoding of EEG signals reveals non-uniformities in the neural geometry of colour
- Author
-
Lindsay N. Thompson, Ivana Jakovljev, Sophie Wuerger, Jasna Martinovic, and Tushar Chauhan
- Subjects
Computer science ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Pattern recognition ,Luminance ,Unique hues ,Contrast (vision) ,Chromatic scale ,Artificial intelligence ,Neural coding ,Set (psychology) ,business ,Decoding methods ,Hue ,media_common - Abstract
The idea of colour opponency maintains that colour vision arises through the comparison of two chromatic mechanisms, red versus green (RG) and yellow versus blue (YB). The four unique hues, red, green, blue, and yellow, are assumed to appear at the null points of these the two chromatic systems. However, whether unique hues have a distinct signature that can be reliably discerned in neural activity is still an open question. Here we hypothesise that, if unique hues represent a tractable cortical state, they should elicit more robust activity compared to non-unique hues. We use a spatiotemporal decoding approach to reconstruct an activation space for a set of unique and intermediate hues across a range of luminance values. We show that electroencephalographic (EEG) responses carry robust information about isoluminant unique hues within a 100-300 ms window from stimulus onset. Decoding is possible in both passive and active viewing tasks, but is compromised when concurrent high luminance contrast is added to the colour signals. The efficiency of hue decoding is not entirely predicted by their mutual distance in a nominally uniform perceptual colour space. Instead, the encoding space shows pivotal non-uniformities which suggest that anisotropies in neurometric hue-spaces are likely to represent perceptual unique hues. Furthermore, the neural code for hue temporally coincides with the neural code for luminance contrast, thus explaining why potential neural correlates of unique hues have remained so elusive until now.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. 'Fake Tan' or 'Fake News'?
- Author
-
Kinjiro Amano, Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, and Georg Meyer
- Subjects
colour ,genetic structures ,media_common.quotation_subject ,lcsh:BF1-990 ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,perception ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Gamut ,Artificial Intelligence ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Computer vision ,Caucasian population ,skin and connective tissue diseases ,media_common ,integumentary system ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,technology, industry, and agriculture ,Art ,Skin colour ,Sensory Systems ,surfaces/materials ,Short and Sweet ,Ophthalmology ,lcsh:Psychology ,face perception ,Fake news ,Artificial intelligence ,light ,business ,human activities ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
We estimated Trump’s skin colour from 70 internet images and also from the “twitter tan line” image (February 8, 2020; Twitter). We then compared the estimated skin colours with two existing data sets of skin colours: the range of skin tans that occur naturally in the Caucasian population and the range skin colours brought about by a sunless tan. We find that Trump’s skin colour is close to the edge of the natural skin tan gamut and firmly within the gamut of a sunless skin tan. The skin colour above Trump’s tan line is outside of the naturally occurring range of skin colours, even outside the skin tan of nonmelanized albinos. The latter finding is consistent with the hypothesis that part of the image may have been digitally distorted.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Spatio-chromatic contrast sensitivity under mesopic and photopic light levels
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, María Pérez-Ortiz, Jasna Martinovic, Minjung Kim, Maliha Ashraf, Rafal Mantiuk, Kim, Minjung [0000-0002-3388-5947], Mantiuk, Rafal [0000-0003-2353-0349], and Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository
- Subjects
Male ,Light ,Mesopic vision ,Mesopic Vision ,Luminance ,law.invention ,0302 clinical medicine ,law ,Contrast (vision) ,media_common ,Physics ,light adaptation ,luminance ,05 social sciences ,spatial vision ,high light level ,Middle Aged ,Sensory Systems ,Achromatic lens ,Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells ,Female ,Spatial frequency ,Color Perception ,achromatic ,Photopic vision ,Adult ,Color vision ,media_common.quotation_subject ,contrast sensitivity functions ,HDR ,050105 experimental psychology ,Article ,cone adaptation ,Contrast Sensitivity ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,Optics ,Spatio-Temporal Analysis ,mesopic ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Chromatic scale ,photopic ,Color Vision ,business.industry ,isoluminance ,chromatic ,Ophthalmology ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) characterize the sensitivity of the human visual system at different spatial scales, but little is known as to how contrast sensitivity for achromatic and chromatic stimuli changes from a mesopic to a highly photopic range reflecting outdoor illumination levels. The purpose of our study was to further characterize the CSF by measuring both achromatic and chromatic sensitivities for background luminance levels from 0.02 cd/m2 to 7,000 cd/m2. Stimuli consisted of Gabor patches of different spatial frequencies and angular sizes, varying from 0.125 to 6 cpd, which were displayed on a custom high dynamic range (HDR) display with luminance levels up to 15,000 cd/m2. Contrast sensitivity was measured in three directions in color space, an achromatic direction, an isoluminant "red-green" direction, and an S-cone isolating "yellow-violet" direction, selected to isolate the luminance, L/M-cone opponent, and S-cone opponent pathways, respectively, of the early postreceptoral processing stages. Within each session, observers were fully adapted to the fixed background luminance (0.02, 2, 20, 200, 2,000, or 7,000 cd/m2). Our main finding is that the background luminance has a differential effect on achromatic contrast sensitivity compared to chromatic contrast sensitivity. The achromatic contrast sensitivity increases with higher background luminance up to 200 cd/m2 and then shows a sharp decline when background luminance is increased further. In contrast, the chromatic sensitivity curves do not show a significant sensitivity drop at higher luminance levels. We present a computational luminance-dependent model that predicts the CSF for achromatic and chromatic stimuli of arbitrary size.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Spatio-chromatic contrast sensitivity across the life span: interactions between age and light level in high dynamic range
- Author
-
Helen Saunderson, Jasna Martinovic, Minjung Kim, Rafal Mantiuk, Maliha Ashraf, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Physics ,Ophthalmology ,Light level ,Chromatic contrast ,Optics ,Life span ,business.industry ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,business ,Sensory Systems ,High dynamic range - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Neural mechanisms of divided feature-selective attention to colour
- Author
-
Steven A. Hillyard, Sophie Wuerger, Jasna Martinovic, Søren K. Andersen, and Matthias M. Müller
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Computer science ,Color vision ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Space (commercial competition) ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Dimension (vector space) ,Selection (linguistics) ,Humans ,Attention ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Selective attention ,Linear separability ,Hue ,Cerebral Cortex ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Electroencephalography ,Pattern recognition ,Neurology ,Feature (computer vision) ,Evoked Potentials, Visual ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Color Perception ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Feature-based attentional selection of colour is challenging to investigate due to the multidimensional nature of colour-space. When attending concurrently to features from different feature dimensions (e.g. red and horizontal), the attentional selections of the separate dimensions are largely independent. Therefore, if colour constitutes multiple independent feature dimensions for attentional purposes, concurrently attending to two colours should be effective and independent of the specific configuration of target and distractor colours. Here, observers attended concurrently to two out of four fully overlapping random dot kinematograms of different colours, and the allocation of attention to each colour was assessed separately by recordings of steady-state visual evoked potentials. The magnitude of attention effects depended on colour proximity and was well described by a simple model which suggested that colour space is rescaled in an adaptive manner to achieve attentional selection. In conclusion, different spatially overlaid colours can be attended concurrently with an efficiency that is determined by their configuration in colour space, supporting the idea that (at least in terms of hue) colour acts as a single dimension for attentional purposes.
- Published
- 2018
9. Safety and acceptability of an organic light-emitting diode sleep mask as a potential therapy for retinal disease
- Author
-
Kate M. Bennett, Jayashree Sahni, Ian Grierson, C Murray-Dunning, S A Taylor, Sophie Wuerger, T Gutu, Simon P. Harding, M N Holland, and Gabriela Czanner
- Subjects
Male ,Visual acuity ,genetic structures ,Visual Acuity ,Glaucoma ,Neuro-ophthalmology ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,0302 clinical medicine ,Contrast (vision) ,Prospective Studies ,media_common ,Masks ,Middle Aged ,Patient Satisfaction ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Color Perception ,Tomography, Optical Coherence ,Adult ,Sleep Wake Disorders ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Macular Edema ,Retina ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,Ophthalmology ,Reaction Time ,medicine ,Humans ,Aged ,Diabetic Retinopathy ,business.industry ,Psychomotor vigilance task ,Retinal ,Phototherapy ,medicine.disease ,Alertness ,chemistry ,Clinical Study ,030221 ophthalmology & optometry ,Visual Fields ,Sleep ,business ,Microperimetry ,Psychomotor Performance ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Purpose The purpose of the study was to study the effect of an organic light-emitting diode sleep mask on daytime alertness, wellbeing, and retinal structure/function in healthy volunteers and in diabetic macular oedema (DMO). Patients and methods Healthy volunteers in two groups, 18–30 yrs (A), 50–70 yrs (B) and people with DMO (C) wore masks (504 nm wavelength; 80 cd/m2 luminance; ≤8 h) nightly for 3 months followed by a 1-month recovery period. Changes from baseline were measured for (means): psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) (number of lapses (NL), response time (RT)), sleep, depression, psychological wellbeing (PW), visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, colour, electrophysiology, microperimetry, and retinal thickness on OCT. Results Of 60 participants, 16 (27%) withdrew, 8 (13%) before month 1, due to sleep disturbances and mask intolerance. About 36/55 (65%) who continued beyond month 1 reported ≥1 adverse event. At month 3 mean PVT worsened in Group A (RT (7.65%, P
- Published
- 2017
10. Limitations of visual gamma corrections in LCD displays
- Author
-
Jordi Roca-Vila, Sophie Wuerger, Dimosthenis Karatzas, and C. Alejandro Parraga
- Subjects
Liquid-crystal display ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Display calibration ,Observer (special relativity) ,Luminance ,Transfer function ,Visual gamma correction ,law.invention ,Human-Computer Interaction ,Perceptual ,Optics ,Measurement device ,Hardware and Architecture ,law ,Observer-based calibration ,Luminance matching ,Psychophysics ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,Electrical and Electronic Engineering ,business - Abstract
A method for estimating the non-linear gamma transfer function of liquid–crystal displays (LCDs) without the need of a photometric measurement device was described by Xiao et al. (2011) [1]. It relies on observer’s judgments of visual luminance by presenting eight half-tone patterns with luminances from 1/9 to 8/9 of the maximum value of each colour channel. These half-tone patterns were distributed over the screen both over the vertical and horizontal viewing axes. We conducted a series of photometric and psychophysical measurements (consisting in the simultaneous presentation of half-tone patterns in each trial) to evaluate whether the angular dependency of the light generated by three different LCD technologies would bias the results of these gamma transfer function estimations. Our results show that there are significant differences between the gamma transfer functions measured and produced by observers at different viewing angles. We suggest appropriate modifications to the Xiao et al. paradigm to counterbalance these artefacts which also have the advantage of shortening the amount of time spent in collecting the psychophysical measurements.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity at high light levels
- Author
-
María Pérez-Ortiz, Jasna Martinovic, Rafal Mantiuk, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Physics ,Ophthalmology ,Chromatic contrast ,Optics ,business.industry ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,business ,Luminance ,Sensory Systems - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. The time course of auditory–visual processing of speech and body actions: Evidence for the simultaneous activation of an extended neural network for semantic processing
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger, and Neil Harrison
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Speech perception ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Electroencephalography ,Brain mapping ,Young Adult ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Phonetics ,medicine ,Humans ,Semantic memory ,Motion perception ,Brain Mapping ,Communication ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,business.industry ,Brain ,Semantics ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Speech Perception ,Female ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,business ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,Biological motion - Abstract
An extensive network of cortical areas is involved in multisensory object and action recognition. This network draws on inferior frontal, posterior temporal, and parietal areas; activity is modulated by familiarity and the semantic congruency of auditory and visual component signals even if semantic incongruences are created by combining visual and auditory signals representing very different signal categories, such as speech and whole body actions. Here we present results from a high-density ERP study designed to examine the time-course and source location of responses to semantically congruent and incongruent audiovisual speech and body actions to explore whether the network involved in action recognition consists of a hierarchy of sequentially activated processing modules or a network of simultaneously active processing sites. We report two main results:1) There are no significant early differences in the processing of congruent and incongruent audiovisual action sequences. The earliest difference between congruent and incongruent audiovisual stimuli occurs between 240 and 280 ms after stimulus onset in the left temporal region. Between 340 and 420 ms, semantic congruence modulates responses in central and right frontal areas. Late differences (after 460 ms) occur bilaterally in frontal areas.2) Source localisation (dipole modelling and LORETA) reveals that an extended network encompassing inferior frontal, temporal, parasaggital, and superior parietal sites are simultaneously active between 180 and 420 ms to process auditory–visual action sequences. Early activation (before 120 ms) can be explained by activity in mainly sensory cortices. . The simultaneous activation of an extended network between 180 and 420 ms is consistent with models that posit parallel processing of complex action sequences in frontal, temporal and parietal areas rather than models that postulate hierarchical processing in a sequence of brain regions.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. A colour image reproduction framework for 3D colour printing
- Author
-
Julian Yates, Changjun Li, Ali Sohiab, Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, and Pei-li Sun
- Subjects
Colour image ,3d printed ,business.industry ,Reproduction (economics) ,Color reproduction ,Color printing ,Biology ,Color management ,law.invention ,law ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Focus (optics) - Abstract
In this paper, the current technologies in full colour 3D printing technology were introduced. A framework of colour image reproduction process for 3D colour printing is proposed. A special focus was put on colour management for 3D printed objects. Two approaches, colorimetric colour reproduction and spectral based colour reproduction are proposed in order to faithfully reproduce colours in 3D objects. Two key studies, colour reproduction for soft tissue prostheses and colour uniformity correction across different orientations are described subsequently. Results are clear shown that applying proposed colour image reproduction framework, performance of colour reproduction can be significantly enhanced. With post colour corrections, a further improvement in colour process are achieved for 3D printed objects. © (2016) COPYRIGHT Society of Photo-Optical Instrumentation Engineers (SPIE). Downloading of the abstract is permitted for personal use only.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Colour Image Reproduction for 3D Printing Facial Prostheses
- Author
-
Faraedon Mostafa, Julian Yates, Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, Ali Sohaib, and Shishkovsky, IV
- Subjects
Protocol (science) ,Engineering ,Colour image ,business.industry ,Objective measurement ,3d image processing ,ComputingMethodologies_IMAGEPROCESSINGANDCOMPUTERVISION ,3D printing ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,Skin colour ,business - Abstract
In this chapter, using colour 3D printing technology, a 3D colour image reproduction system is detailed for the semi-automated and accurate additive manufacturing of facial soft tissue prostheses. A protocol for 3D colour image reproduction was designed based on the six steps of processing. For this specific application, protocols for each sub‐process required development and details of each technique applied are discussed. The quality of facial prostheses was evaluated through objective measurement and subjective assessment. The results demonstrated that the proposed colour reproduction system can be effectively used to produce accurate skin colour with fine textures over a 3D shape, with significant savings in both time and cost when compared to traditional techniques.
- Published
- 2016
15. Unique hue data for colour appearance models. Part II: Chromatic adaptation transform
- Author
-
Dimosthenis Karatzas, Dimitris Mylonas, Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, and Chenyang Fu
- Subjects
business.industry ,Computer science ,General Chemical Engineering ,Chromatic adaptation ,Computer graphics (images) ,Benchmark (computing) ,CIECAM02 ,Human Factors and Ergonomics ,Pattern recognition ,General Chemistry ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Hue - Abstract
Unique hue settings of 185 observers under three room-lighting conditions were used to evaluate the accuracy of full and mixed chromatic adaptation transform models of CIECAM02 in terms of unique hue reproduction. Perceptual hue shifts in CIECAM02 were evaluated for both models with no clear difference using the current Commission Internationale de l'Eclairage (CIE) recommendation for mixed chromatic adaptation ratio. Using our large dataset of unique hue data as a benchmark, an optimised parameter is proposed for chromatic adaptation under mixed illumination conditions that produces more accurate results in unique hue reproduction. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2013
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Visual gamma correction for LCD displays
- Author
-
Kaida Xiao, Chenyang Fu, Dimosthenis Karatzas, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Liquid-crystal display ,genetic structures ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Linear interpolation ,Luminance ,law.invention ,Human-Computer Interaction ,Color rendering index ,Relative luminance ,Hardware and Architecture ,law ,Gamma correction ,Psychophysics ,Digital signal ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,Electrical and Electronic Engineering ,business - Abstract
An improved method for visual gamma correction is developed for LCD displays to increase the accuracy of digital colour reproduction. Rather than utilising a photometric measurement device, we use observers’ visual luminance judgements for gamma correction. Eight half tone patterns were designed to generate relative luminances from 1/9 to 8/9 for each colour channel. A psychophysical experiment was conducted on an LCD display to find the digital signals corresponding to each relative luminance by visually matching the half-tone background to a uniform colour patch. Both inter- and intra-observer variability for the eight luminance matches in each channel were assessed and the luminance matches proved to be consistent across observers (Δ E 00 E 00 E 00 . We conclude that this observer-based method of visual gamma correction is useful to estimate the OETF for LCD displays. Its major advantage is that no particular functional relationship between digital inputs and luminance outputs has to be assumed.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Colour-opponent mechanisms are not affected by age-related chromatic sensitivity changes
- Author
-
Dimosthenis Karatzas, Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, and Chenyang Fu
- Subjects
genetic structures ,business.industry ,Sensory Systems ,Unique hues ,Large sample ,Ophthalmology ,Optics ,Age related ,Statistics ,Chromatic scale ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,business ,Optometry ,Mathematics ,Hue - Abstract
The purpose of this study was to assess whether age-related chromatic sensitivity changes are associated with corresponding changes in hue perception in a large sample of colour-normal observers over a wide age range (n = 185; age range: 18―75 years). In these observers we determined both the sensitivity along the protan, deutan and tritan line; and settings for the four unique hues, from which the characteristics of the higher-order colour mechanisms can be derived. We found a significant decrease in chromatic sensitivity due to ageing, in particular along the tritan line. From the unique hue settings we derived the cone weightings associated with the colour mechanisms that are at equilibrium for the four unique hues. We found that the relative cone weightings (w L /w M and w L /w S ) associated with the unique hues were independent of age. Our results are consistent with previous findings that the unique hues are rather constant with age while chromatic sensitivity declines. They also provide evidence in favour of the hypothesis that higher-order colour mechanisms are equipped with flexible cone weightings, as opposed to fixed weights. The mechanism underlying this compensation is still poorly understood.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Motion extrapolation of auditory–visual targets
- Author
-
Kerstin Schill, Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger, Christoph Zetzsche, and Markus Hofbauer
- Subjects
Modality (human–computer interaction) ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Extrapolation ,Object (computer science) ,Signal ,Motion (physics) ,Task (computing) ,Hardware and Architecture ,Position (vector) ,Signal Processing ,Structure from motion ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Software ,Information Systems - Abstract
Many tasks involve the precise estimation of speed and position of moving objects, for instance to catch or avoid objects that cohabit in our environment. Many of these objects are characterised by signal representations in more than one modality, such as hearing and vision. The aim of this study was to investigate the extent to which the simultaneous presentation of auditory and visual signals enhances the estimation of motion speed and instantaneous position. Observers are asked to estimate the instant when a moving object arrives at a target spatial position by pressing a response button. This task requires observers to estimate the speed of the moving object and to calibrate the timing of their manual response such that it coincides with the true arrival time of the moving object. When both visual and auditory motion signals are available, the variability in estimating the arrival time of the moving object is significantly reduced compared to the variability in the unimodal conditions. This reduction in variability is consistent with optimal integration of the auditory and visual speed signals. The average bias in the estimated arrival times depends on the motion speed: for medium speeds (17deg/s) observers' subjective arrival times are earlier than the true arrival times; for high speeds (47deg/s) observers exhibit a (much smaller) bias in the other direction. This speed-dependency suggests that the bias is due to an error in estimating the motion speeds rather than an error in calibrating the timing of the motor response. Finally, in this temporal localization task, the bias and variability show similar patterns for motion defined by vision, audition or both.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. S-cone signals invisible to the motion system can improve motion extraction via grouping by color
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Jasna Martinovic, Matthias M. Müller, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Physiology ,Color vision ,Computer science ,Motion Perception ,Color ,Motion (physics) ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Motion ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Motion perception ,Brownian motion ,Euclidean vector ,Communication ,Color Vision ,Color difference ,business.industry ,Electroencephalography ,Sensory Systems ,Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells ,Noise (video) ,Artificial intelligence ,Cues ,business ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation ,Motion system - Abstract
The purpose of this study was to test whether color–motion correlations carried by a pure color difference (S-cone component only) can be used to improve global motion extraction. We also examined the neural markers of color–motion correlation processing in event-related potentials. Color and motion information was dissociated using a two-colored random dot kinematogram, wherein coherent motion and motion noise differed from each other only in their S-cone component, with spatial and temporal parameters set so that global motion processing relied solely on a constant L-M component. Hence, when color and the local motion direction are correlated, more efficient segregation of coherent motion can only be brought about by the S-cone difference, and crucially, this S-cone component does not provide any effective input to a global motion mechanism but only changes the color appearance of the moving dots. The color contrasts (vector length in the S vs. L-M plane) of both the dots carrying coherent motion and the dots moving randomly were fixed at motion discrimination threshold to ensure equal effectiveness for motion extraction. In the behavioral experiment, participants were asked to discriminate between coherent and random motion, and d′ was determined for three different conditions: uncorrelated, uncued correlated, and cued correlated. In the electroencephalographic experiment, participants discriminated direction of motion for uncued correlated and cued correlated conditions. Color–motion correlations were found to improve performance. Cueing a specific color also modulated the N1 component of the event-related potential, with sources in visual area middle temporal. We conclude that S-cone signals “invisible” to the motion system can influence the analysis by direction-selective motion mechanisms through grouping of local motion signals by color. This grouping mechanism must precede motion processing and is likely to be under attentional control.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. When S-cones contribute to chromatic global motion processing
- Author
-
Marco Bertamini, Alexa I. Ruppertsberg, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Male ,Fovea Centralis ,Current (mathematics) ,Physiology ,Computer science ,Motion Perception ,Motion processing ,Displacement (vector) ,Motion (physics) ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Chromatic scale ,Color contrast ,Size Perception ,Communication ,business.industry ,Sensory Systems ,Koniocellular cell ,Space Perception ,Human visual system model ,Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Color Perception - Abstract
There is common consensus now that color-defined motion can be perceived by the human visual system. For global motion integration tasks based on isoluminant random dot kinematograms conflicting evidence exists, whether observers can (Ruppertsberg et al., 2003) or cannot (Bilodeau & Faubert, 1999) extract a common motion direction for stimuli modulated along the isoluminant red-green axis. Here we report conditions, in which S-cones contribute to chromatic global motion processing. When the display included extra-foveal regions, the individual elements were large ( approximately 0.3 degrees ) and the displacement was large ( approximately 1 degrees ), stimuli modulated along the yellowish-violet axis proved to be effective in a global motion task. The color contrast thresholds for detection for both color axes were well below the contrasts required for global motion integration, and therefore the discrimination-to-detection ratio was >1. We conclude that there is significant S-cone input to chromatic global motion processing and the extraction of global motion is not mediated by the same mechanism as simple detection. Whether the koniocellular or the magnocellular pathway is involved in transmitting S-cone signals is a topic of current debate (Chatterjee & Callaway, 2002).
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Unique Hue Data for Colour Appearance Models. Part III: Comparison with NCS Unique Hues
- Author
-
Michael R. Pointer, Tushar Chauhan, Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, and Guihua Cui
- Subjects
business.industry ,General Chemical Engineering ,Diagram ,CIECAM02 ,Human Factors and Ergonomics ,Pattern recognition ,General Chemistry ,Unique hues ,Active appearance model ,Optics ,Natural Color System ,Artificial intelligence ,Monochromatic color ,Chromatic scale ,business ,Hue ,Mathematics - Abstract
In this study, Swedish Natural Color System (NCS) unique hue data were used to evaluate the performance of unique hue predictions by the CIECAM02 colour appearance model. The colour appearance of 108 NCS unique hue stimuli was predicted using CIECAM02, and their distributions were represented in a CIECAM02 ac–bc chromatic diagram. The best-fitting line for each of the four unique hues was found using orthogonal distance regression in the ac–bc chromatic diagram. Comparison of these predicted unique hue lines (based on the NCS data) with the default unique hue loci in CIECAM02 showed that there were significant differences in both unique yellow (UY) and unique blue (UB). The same tendency was found for hue uniformity: hue uniformity is worse for UY and UB stimuli in comparison with unique red (UR) and unique green (UG). A comparison between NCS unique hue stimuli and another set of unique hue stimuli (obtained on a calibrated cathode ray tube) was conducted in CIECAM02 to investigate possible media differences that might affect unique hue predictions. Data for UY and UB are in very good agreement; largest deviations were found for UR. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 40, 256–263, 2015
- Published
- 2015
22. The effects of stereo disparity on the behavioural and electrophysiological correlates of perception of audio-visual motion in depth
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Sian Witheridge, Alan J. Pegna, Neil Harrison, Sophie Wuerger, and Alexis D. J. Makin
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Auditory perception ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Vision Disparity ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Audiology ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Looming ,ddc:150 ,Perception ,Psychophysics ,medicine ,Humans ,Motion perception ,Evoked Potentials ,media_common ,Communication ,business.industry ,Brain ,Electroencephalography ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Auditory Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,business ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Motion is represented by low-level signals, such as size-expansion in vision or loudness changes in the auditory modality. The visual and auditory signals from the same object or event may be integrated and facilitate detection. We explored behavioural and electrophysiological correlates of congruent and incongruent audio–visual depth motion in conditions where auditory level changes, visual expansion, and visual disparity cues were manipulated. In Experiment 1 participants discriminated auditory motion direction whilst viewing looming or receding, 2D or 3D, visual stimuli. Responses were faster and more accurate for congruent than for incongruent audio–visual cues, and the congruency effect (i.e., difference between incongruent and congruent conditions) was larger for visual 3D cues compared to 2D cues. In Experiment 2, event-related potentials (ERPs) were collected during presentation of the 2D and 3D, looming and receding, audio–visual stimuli, while participants detected an infrequent deviant sound. Our main finding was that audio–visual congruity was affected by retinal disparity at an early processing stage (135–160 ms) over occipito-parietal scalp. Topographic analyses suggested that similar brain networks were activated for the 2D and 3D congruity effects, but that cortical responses were stronger in the 3D condition. Differences between congruent and incongruent conditions were observed between 140–200 ms, 220–280 ms, and 350–500 ms after stimulus onset.
- Published
- 2015
23. Low-level integration of auditory and visual motion signals requires spatial co-localisation
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Florian Röhrbein, Christoph Zetzsche, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Auditory perception ,Sound localization ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Fixation, Ocular ,Sensory threshold ,Perception ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Sound Localization ,Motion perception ,media_common ,Communication ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Auditory Threshold ,Motion detection ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Sensory Thresholds ,Space Perception ,Fixation (visual) ,Auditory Perception ,Visual Perception ,Artificial intelligence ,Visual Fields ,business ,Algorithms ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
It is well known that the detection thresholds for stationary auditory and visual signals are lower if the signals are presented bimodally rather than unimodally, provided the signals coincide in time and space. Recent work on auditory-visual motion detection suggests that the facilitation seen for stationary signals is not seen for motion signals. We investigate the conditions under which motion perception also benefits from the integration of auditory and visual signals. We show that the integration of cross-modal local motion signals that are matched in position and speed is consistent with thresholds predicted by a neural summation model. If the signals are presented in different hemi-fields, move in different directions, or both, then behavioural thresholds are predicted by a probability-summation model. We conclude that cross-modal signals have to be co-localised and co-incident for effective motion integration. We also argue that facilitation is only seen if the signals contain all localisation cues that would be produced by physical objects.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Catching audiovisual mice: Predicting the arrival time of auditory-visual motion signals
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Christoph Zetzsche, Kerstin Schill, Sophie Wuerger, Markus Hofbauer, and F. Roehrbein
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Signal Detection, Psychological ,Time Factors ,Adolescent ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Speech recognition ,Auditory visual ,Motion Perception ,Sensory system ,Fixation, Ocular ,Arrival time ,Motion (physics) ,Task (project management) ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Perception ,Humans ,media_common ,Communication ,business.industry ,Middle Aged ,Object (computer science) ,Space Perception ,Auditory Perception ,Visual Perception ,Facilitation ,Female ,Psychology ,business - Abstract
We investigated the extent to which auditory and visual motion signals are combined when observers are asked to predict the location of a virtually moving target. In Condition 1, the unimodal and bimodal signals were noisy, but the target object was continuously visible and audible; in Condition 2, the virtually moving object was hidden (invisible and inaudible) for a short period prior to its arrival at the target location. Our main finding was that the facilitation due to simultaneous visual and auditory input is very different for the two conditions. When the target is continuously visible and audible (Condition 1), the bimodal performance is twice as good as the unimodal performances, thus suggesting a very effective integration mechanism. On the other hand, if the object is hidden for a short period (Condition 2) and the task therefore requires the extrapolation of motion speed over a temporal and spatial period, the facilitation due to both sensory inputs is almost absent, and the bimodal performance is limited by the visual performance.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Color quality assessments of 3D facial prostheses in varying illuminations
- Author
-
Kinjiro Amano, Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, Ali Sohaib, Julian Yates, and Charles Whitford
- Subjects
0209 industrial biotechnology ,Ophthalmology ,Communication ,020901 industrial engineering & automation ,Color quality ,business.industry ,Computer science ,Computer vision ,02 engineering and technology ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Sensory Systems - Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. The effect of stereoscopic acquisition parameters on both distortion and comfort
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Robert H. Black, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Computer science ,business.industry ,Stereoscopy ,Vergence ,law.invention ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,law ,medicine ,Image translation ,Computer vision ,Human eye ,Ray tracing (graphics) ,Artificial intelligence ,Perceptual Distortion ,business - Abstract
The purpose of our experiments was to investigate the effect of interaxial camera separation on the perceived shape and viewing comfort of 3D images. Horizontal Image Translation (HIT) and interaxial distance were altered together. Following Banks et al (2009), our stimuli were simple stereoscopic hinges and we measured the perceived angle as a function of camera separation. We compared the predictions based on ray tracing with the perceived 3D shape obtained psychophysically. 40 participants were asked to judge the angles of 250 hinges at different camera separations (interaxial and HIT linked a 20-100mm; angle range: 50°-130°). Comfort data was obtained using a five point Likert scale. Stimuli were presented in orthoscopic conditions with screen and observer Field of View (FOVO) matched at 45°. Our main results are: (1) For the 60mm camera separation, observers perceived a right angle correctly, but at other camera separations right angles were perceived as larger than 90° (camera separations > 60mm) or smaller than 90° (camera separations < 60 mm). (2) The observed perceptual deviations from a right angle were smaller than predicted based on disparity information (ray tracing model) alone. (3) We found an interaction between comfort and camera separation: only at the 60mm camera separation (e.g. at typical human eye separation) do we find a significant negative correlation between angle and comfort. All other camera separations, the disparity (angle) has no systematic effect on comfort. This research is set out to provide a foundation for tolerance limits for comfort and perceptual distortions brought about by various virtual camera separations.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. The achromatic locus: effect of navigation direction in color space
- Author
-
Esther Perales, Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, Emily Hird, Dimosthenis Karatzas, Tushar Chauhan, Visión y Color, and Universidad de Alicante. Departamento de Óptica, Farmacología y Anatomía
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Light ,Color vision ,Movement ,Color ,Color space ,Luminance ,Unique hues ,law.invention ,Young Adult ,Optics ,law ,Humans ,Chromaticity ,Lighting ,Vision, Ocular ,Óptica ,Hue ,Mathematics ,Color constancy ,business.industry ,Articles ,Middle Aged ,Sensory Systems ,Achromatic locus ,Ophthalmology ,Achromatic lens ,Female ,business ,Color Perception - Abstract
An achromatic stimulus is defined as a patch of light that is devoid of any hue. This is usually achieved by asking observers to adjust the stimulus such that it looks neither red nor green and at the same time neither yellow nor blue. Despite the theoretical and practical importance of the achromatic locus, little is known about the variability in these settings. The main purpose of the current study was to evaluate whether achromatic settings were dependent on the task of the observers, namely the navigation direction in color space. Observers could either adjust the test patch along the two chromatic axes in the CIE u*v* diagram or, alternatively, navigate along the unique-hue lines. Our main result is that the navigation method affects the reliability of these achromatic settings. Observers are able to make more reliable achromatic settings when adjusting the test patch along the directions defined by the four unique hues as opposed to navigating along the main axes in the commonly used CIE u*v* chromaticity plane. This result holds across different ambient viewing conditions (Dark, Daylight, Cool White Fluorescent) and different test luminance levels (5, 20, and 50 cd/m2). The reduced variability in the achromatic settings is consistent with the idea that internal color representations are more aligned with the unique-hue lines than the u* and v* axes. EP and EH were supported by TSB Project No. 720014; KX is supported by the EPSRC (EP/K040057/1).
- Published
- 2014
28. Cross-modal integration of auditory and visual motion signals
- Author
-
Georg Meyer and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Auditory perception ,Psychometrics ,genetic structures ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,ComputingMethodologies_IMAGEPROCESSINGANDCOMPUTERVISION ,Motion capture ,Motion (physics) ,Stimulus modality ,Perception ,Humans ,Structure from motion ,Computer vision ,Motion perception ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,media_common ,Communication ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Auditory Threshold ,eye diseases ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Auditory Perception ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,Psychology ,business ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Real-world moving objects are usually defined by correlated information in multiple sensory modalities such as vision and hearing. The aim of our study was to assess whether simultaneous auditory supra-threshold motion introduces a bias or affects the sensitivity in a visual motion detection task. We demonstrate a bias in the perceived direction of visual motion that is consistent with the direction of the auditory motion (audio-visual motion capture). This bias effect is robust and occurs even if the auditory and visual motion signals come from different locations or move at different speeds. We also show that visual motion detection thresholds are higher for consistent auditory motion than for inconsistent motion, provided the stimuli move at the same speed and are co-localised.
- Published
- 2001
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Low-level and high-level modulations of fixational saccades and high frequency oscillatory brain activity in a visual object classification task
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Jasna Martinovic, Amelia R. Hunt, Ben J. Jennings, Maciej Kosilo, and Matt Craddock
- Subjects
fixational saccades ,C850 Cognitive Psychology ,gamma-band activity ,colour ,genetic structures ,Brain activity and meditation ,lcsh:BF1-990 ,Color ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Electroencephalography ,050105 experimental psychology ,parallel visual pathways ,law.invention ,03 medical and health sciences ,visual object representation, parallel visual pathways, color, luminance, fixational saccades, microsaccades, EEG, gamma-band activity ,0302 clinical medicine ,ddc:150 ,law ,medicine ,Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Computer vision ,Original Research Article ,EEG ,Parallel visual pathways ,General Psychology ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Lumiance ,business.industry ,luminance ,05 social sciences ,Eye movement ,C830 Experimental Psychology ,C800 Psychology ,color ,microsaccades ,lcsh:Psychology ,Achromatic lens ,Saccade ,Eye tracking ,visual object representation ,Visual object representation ,Artificial intelligence ,Microsaccade ,business ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Until recently induced gamma-band activity (GBA) was considered a neural marker of cortical object representation. However, induced GBA in the electroencephalogram (EEG) is susceptible to artifacts caused by miniature fixational saccades. Recent studies have demonstrated that fixational saccades also reflect high-level representational processes. Do high-level as opposed to low-level factors influence fixational saccades? What is the effect of these factors on artifact-free GBA? To investigate this, we conducted separate eye tracking and EEG experiments using identical designs. Participants classified line drawings as objects or non-objects. To introduce low-level differences, contours were defined along different directions in cardinal color space: S-cone-isolating, intermediate isoluminant, or a full-color stimulus, the latter containing an additional achromatic component. Prior to the classification task, object discrimination thresholds were measured and stimuli were scaled to matching suprathreshold levels for each participant. In both experiments, behavioral performance was best for full-color stimuli and worst for S-cone isolating stimuli. Saccade rates 200-700 ms after stimulus onset were modulated independently by low and high-level factors, being higher for full-color stimuli than for S-cone isolating stimuli and higher for objects. Low-amplitude evoked GBA and total GBA were observed in very few conditions, showing that paradigms with isoluminant stimuli may not be ideal for eliciting such responses. We conclude that cortical loops involved in the processing of objects are preferentially excited by stimuli that contain achromatic information. Their activation can lead to relatively early exploratory eye movements even for foveally-presented stimuli. © 2013 Kosilo, Wuerger, Craddock, Jennings, Hunt and Martinovic.
- Published
- 2013
30. Dynamic cues to binocular depth
- Author
-
Laurence P. Tidbury, Anna R. O'Connor, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Stereoscopy ,Luminance ,050105 experimental psychology ,law.invention ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,lcsh:Ophthalmology ,law ,Dynamic stereo-acuity ,Contrast (vision) ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Computer vision ,Stereopsis ,media_common ,Mathematics ,business.industry ,Stimulus pattern ,05 social sciences ,Stereoscopic acuity ,Ophthalmology ,Monocular depth ,lcsh:RE1-994 ,030221 ophthalmology & optometry ,Binocular disparity ,sense organs ,Artificial intelligence ,Binocular vision ,business ,Optometry - Abstract
Aim: Subjects with no clinically measurable stereoacuity report compelling ‘pop-out’ depth effects when viewing a 3D stereoscopic video. The purpose of this study was to systematically investigate the effectiveness of static and dynamic stereoscopic stimuli, by isolating cues to depth present in stereoscopic 3D entertainment media. Methods: Stereoscopic stimuli were developed that either featured or lacked changes of disparity and/or of stimulus pattern. A PC-controlled 4-alternativeforced-choice (4AFC) task was used to assess the depth detection thresholds of visually normal subjects, with stimuli presented on a passive polarised stereoscopic monitor at 3 m. Thresholds were determined in four conditions: baseline STATIC (fixed disparity level), STATIC PATTERN CHANGE (fixed disparity level with a change in stimulus pattern), Z-LOCATION CHANGE (disparity increase towards target level with a fixed pattern) and CDOT (disparity increase with pattern change). Results: In total 32 subjects aged 18–41 years were recruited from the University of Liverpool. The mean(SD) thresholds were: STATIC 183@(101), Z-LOCATION CHANGE 120@(60), CDOT 167@(111) and STATIC PATTERN CHANGE 241@(129). The conditions which contained a change in z-location yielded a significantly lower threshold than those with fixed disparity ( 'p >' 0.01), whereas the presence of a pattern change resulted in a statistically significant reduction in threshold (' p >' 0.05). There was no significant interaction between the factors. Conclusion: By directly comparing thresholds for static and dynamic conditions using stimuli presented on the same device with the same settings (such as display duration, size, contrast, colour, display method, luminance, testing protocol), we can conclude that it is the dynamic nature of the disparity information that confers a benefit on individuals’ depth detection. The dynamic facet of stereopsis may contribute to the compelling ‘pop-out’ effect described when viewing 3D entertainment media.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Evidence for auditory-visual processing specific to biological motion
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger, and Alexander Crocker-Buque
- Subjects
Adult ,genetic structures ,Adolescent ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Auditory visual ,Motion Perception ,Auditory signal ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Visual modality ,Walking ,Scrambling ,Young Adult ,Looming ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Communication ,business.industry ,Observer (special relativity) ,Middle Aged ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Auditory Perception ,Visual Perception ,Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Psychology ,Biological motion - Abstract
Biological motion is usually associated with highly correlated sensory signals from more than one modality: an approaching human walker will not only have a visual representation, namely an increase in the retinal size of the walker's image, but also a synchronous auditory signal since the walker's footsteps will grow louder. We investigated whether the multisensorial processing of biological motion is subject to different constraints than ecologically invalid motion. Observers were presented with a visual point-light walker and/or synchronised auditory footsteps; the walker was either approaching the observer (looming motion) or walking away (receding motion). A scrambled point-light walker served as a control. Observers were asked to detect the walker's motion as quickly and as accurately as possible. In Experiment 1 we tested whether the reaction time advantage due to redundant information in the auditory and visual modality is specific for biological motion. We found no evidence for such an effect: the reaction time reduction was accounted for by statistical facilitation for both biological and scrambled motion. In Experiment 2, we dissociated the auditory and visual information and tested whether inconsistent motion directions across the auditory and visual modality yield longer reaction times in comparison to consistent motion directions. Here we find an effect specific to biological motion: motion incongruency leads to longer reaction times only when the visual walker is intact and recognisable as a human figure. If the figure of the walker is abolished by scrambling, motion incongruency has no effect on the speed of the observers' judgments. In conjunction with Experiment 1 this suggests that conflicting auditory-visual motion information of an intact human walker leads to interference and thereby delaying the response.
- Published
- 2012
32. Blue–green color categorization in Mandarin–English speakers
- Author
-
Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, Dimosthenis Karatzas, Dimitris Mylonas, Galina V. Paramei, Emily Hird, and Qingmei Huang
- Subjects
Male ,Color vision ,Speech recognition ,First language ,Color ,Multilingualism ,Color space ,Mandarin Chinese ,Young Adult ,Optics ,Reaction Time ,Psychophysics ,Humans ,Child ,business.industry ,Atomic and Molecular Physics, and Optics ,language.human_language ,Electronic, Optical and Magnetic Materials ,Visual field ,Categorization ,Language Arts ,language ,Female ,Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition ,Psychology ,business ,Color Perception - Abstract
Observers are faster to detect a target among a set of distracters if the targets and distracters come from different color categories. This cross-boundary advantage seems to be limited to the right visual field, which is consistent with the dominance of the left hemisphere for language processing [Gilbert et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 489 (2006)]. Here we study whether a similar visual field advantage is found in the color identification task in speakers of Mandarin, a language that uses a logographic system. Forty late Mandarin-English bilinguals performed a blue-green color categorization task, in a blocked design, in their first language (L1: Mandarin) or second language (L2: English). Eleven color singletons ranging from blue to green were presented for 160 ms, randomly in the left visual field (LVF) or right visual field (RVF). Color boundary and reaction times (RTs) at the color boundary were estimated in L1 and L2, for both visual fields. We found that the color boundary did not differ between the languages; RTs at the color boundary, however, were on average more than 100 ms shorter in the English compared to the Mandarin sessions, but only when the stimuli were presented in the RVF. The finding may be explained by the script nature of the two languages: Mandarin logographic characters are analyzed visuospatially in the right hemisphere, which conceivably facilitates identification of color presented to the LVF.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Multivoxel Pattern Analysis Using Information-Preserving EMD
- Author
-
Laura M. Parkes, Sophie Wuerger, Hujun Yin, and Zareen Mehboob
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Pattern recognition ,Cognition ,Mutual information ,Lateral geniculate nucleus ,Unique hues ,Hilbert–Huang transform ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,medicine ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Multivoxel pattern analysis - Abstract
This paper presents a quantitative analysis on fMRI data using the information-preserving mode decomposition. Multivoxel patterns in fMRI responses in a cognitive experiment were analyzed for spatial selectivity to color perceptions of neurons in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) and the primary visual cortex (V1). The performance of the new method is tested and evaluated in a case study and the results are compared with the previous findings on the same dataset. While conforming to the previous study, the new results have shown improved classification of patterns for unique hues in V1.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Event-related potentials reveal an early advantage for luminance contours in the processing of objects
- Author
-
Alan Johnston, Marty Woldorff, Aurelio Bruno, Paul Hibbard, Pascal Mamassian, Frank Sengpiel, Sophie Wuerger, Bruno Richard, Jeroen J.A. Van Boxtel, Michael Webster, Eli Brenner, Pablo De Gracia, Jeroen Smeets, CHIEN-TE WU, Lucie Sawides, Sieu Khuu, Bruce Hansen, Almut Kelber, Jasna Martinovic, and Vebjørn Ekroll
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Light ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Color ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Electroencephalography ,Luminance ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Young Adult ,Optics ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Event-related potential ,Perception ,medicine ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Chromatic scale ,Evoked Potentials ,media_common ,Visual Cortex ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,business.industry ,Sensory Systems ,Form Perception ,Ophthalmology ,Sensory Thresholds ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,Spatial frequency ,business ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Detection and identification of objects are the most crucial goals of visual perception. We studied the role of luminance and chromatic information for object processing by comparing performance of familiar, meaningful object contours with those of novel, non-object contours. Comparisons were made between full-color and reduced-color object (or non-object) contours. Full-color stimuli contained both chromatic and luminance information, whereas luminance information was absent in the reduced-color stimuli. All stimuli were made equally salient by fixing them at multiples of discrimination threshold contrast. In a subsequent electroencephalographic experiment observers were asked to classify contours as objects or non-objects. An advantage in accuracy was found for full-color stimuli over the reduced-color stimuli but only if the contours depicted objects as opposed to non-objects. Event-related potentials revealed the neural correlate of this object-specific luminance advantage. The amplitude of the centro-occipital N1 component was modulated by stimulus class with the effect being driven by the presence of luminance information. We conclude that high-level discrimination processes in the cortex start relatively early and exhibit object-selective effects only in the presence of luminance information. This is consistent with the superiority of luminance in subserving object identification processes.
- Published
- 2011
35. Induced Gamma-Band Activity and Fixational Eye Movements are Differentially Influenced by Low-and High-Level Factors in a Visual Object Classification Task
- Author
-
Amelia R. Hunt, Maciej Kosilo, Jasna Martinovic, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
medicine.diagnostic_test ,genetic structures ,business.industry ,Line drawings ,lcsh:BF1-990 ,Eye movement ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Electroencephalography ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Sensory Systems ,Fixational eye movements ,Ophthalmology ,lcsh:Psychology ,Artificial Intelligence ,Fixation (visual) ,medicine ,Eye tracking ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Psychology ,Gamma band ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Until recently induced high frequency oscillatory activity (gamma-band activity; >30 Hz) was considered a neural marker of cortical object representation. However, Yuval-Greenberg et al (2008; Neuron) demonstrated that induced gamma-band activity (GBA) in the elecetroencephalogram (EEG) is susceptible to artifacts caused by miniature eye movements, which account for the major part of the signal in the crucial time window of 200-400 ms after stimulus onset. Is there an underlying cortical-induced gamma-band response that is obscured by ocular artifacts but can still be recorded with EEG? Furthermore, if object-specific modulations of induced GBA in previous studies were caused by ocular artifacts, should we instead study fixational eye movements as a response that can reflect higher-level representational processes in vision? In order to investigate this, we conducted an eye tracking experiment and an EEG experiment using the same design. Participants were asked to classify line drawings of objects or non-objects. To introduce low-level differences, their contours were defined along different directions in cardinal colour space: 1) S-cone-isolating (S), or 2) intermediate isoluminant (S and L-M), or 3) a full-colour stimulus, containing an additional achromatic component (S; L-M; L+M+S). In both experiments, behavioural performance was optimal for full-colour stimuli. In the eye tracking experiment, fixational eye movement rates 200-400 ms after stimulus onset depended on low-level factors, with no difference between objects and non-objects. In the EEG experiment, miniature eye movements were identified and removed using the saccadic filter approach. The artifact-free induced GBA exhibited a lateralised distribution, with enhancements at left and right posterior sites. Activity was higher for full-colour objects on the left, with the opposite effect observed on the right. We conclude that induced GBA can be observed in the EEG. While it showed high-level object-specific modulations, miniature eye movements were driven solely by low-level information.
- Published
- 2011
36. The integration of local chromatic motion signals is sensitive to contrast polarity
- Author
-
Stephanie Malek, Alexa I. Ruppertsberg, Jasna Martinovic, Marco Bertamini, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
colour ,Polarity (physics) ,Physiology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Color ,Luminance ,Motion (physics) ,global motion ,Motion Integration ,ON/OFF channels ,Quick model ,Sensory Systems ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Contrast (vision) ,Chromatic scale ,Brownian motion ,media_common ,Physics ,Communication ,business.industry ,Uncorrelated ,Biomechanical Phenomena ,Sensory Thresholds ,Noise (video) ,Biological system ,business ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Global motion integration mechanisms can utilize signals defined by purely chromatic information. Is global motion integration sensitive to the polarity of such color signals? To answer this question, we employed isoluminant random dot kinematograms (RDKs) that contain a single chromatic contrast polarity or two different polarities. Single-polarity RDKs consisted of local motion signals with either a positive or a negative S or L–M component, while in the different-polarity RDKs, half the dots had a positive S or L–M component, and the other half had a negative S or L–M component. In all RDKs, the polarity and the motion direction of the local signals were uncorrelated. Observers discriminated between 50% coherent motion and random motion, and contrast thresholds were obtained for 81% correct responses. Contrast thresholds were obtained for three different dot densities (50, 100, and 200 dots). We report two main findings: (1) dependence on dot density is similar for both contrast polarities (+S vs. −S, +LM vs. −LM) but slightly steeper for S in comparison to LM and (2) thresholds for different-polarity RDKs are significantly higher than for single-polarity RDKs, which is inconsistent with a polarity-blind integration mechanism. We conclude that early motion integration mechanisms are sensitive to the polarity of the local motion signals and do not automatically integrate information across different polarities.
- Published
- 2011
37. The effect of blur adaptation on accommodative response and pupil size during reading
- Author
-
Alexander Robert Wade, Antigona Martinez, Roger Carpenter, GIOVANNA CITTI, Kevin Brooks, Neil Harrison, Sophie Wuerger, Justin Ales, Sowmya Ravikumar, Andreas Sprenger, Alan Pegna, Richard Van Wezel, Albert Victor Van den Berg, Georg Meyer, Konrad Kording, Joram Van Rheede, Bart Krekelberg, Liqiang Huang, Torsten Schubert, Pablo Artal, Sieu Khuu, Yoram Bonneh, Ian Stevenson, Andrew Anderson, Sven Haller, Susan Wardle, Tobias H. Donner, and Holger Rambold
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,genetic structures ,Emmetropia ,Adaptation (eye) ,Pupil ,Young Adult ,Pupillary response ,Myopia ,Humans ,Monocular ,business.industry ,Adaptation, Ocular ,Accommodation, Ocular ,eye diseases ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Rapid serial visual presentation ,Reading ,Autorefractor ,Optometry ,Female ,sense organs ,Psychology ,business ,Accommodation - Abstract
To study the effect of blur adaptation on accommodative variability, accommodative responses and pupil diameters in myopes (n = 22) and emmetropes (n = 19) were continuously measured before, during, and after exposure to defocus blur. Accommodative and pupillary response measurements were made by an autorefractor during a monocular reading exercise. The text was presented on a computer screen at 33 cm viewing distance with a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm. After baseline testing and a 5-min rest, blur was induced by wearing either an optimally refractive lens, or a +1.0 DS or a +3.0 DS defocus lens. Responses were continuously measured during a 5-min period of adaptation. The lens was then removed, and measurements were again made during a 5-min post-adaptation period. After a second 5-min rest, a final post-adaptation period was measured. No significant change of baseline accommodative responses was found after the 5-min period of adaptation to the blurring lenses (p > 0.05). Compared to the pre-adaptation level, both refractive groups had similar and significant increases in accommodative variability right after blur adaptation to both defocus lenses. After the second rest period, the accommodative variability in both groups returned to the pre-adaptation level. The results indicate that blur adaptation has a short-term effect on the accommodative system to elevate instability of the accommodative response. Mechanisms underlying the increase in accommodative variability by blur adaptation and possible influences of the accommodation stability on myopia development were discussed.
- Published
- 2010
38. A FUNCTIONAL TRANSCRANIAL DOPPLER ULTRASOUND STUDY OF BRAIN LATERALISATION IN STONE TOOL MAKING AND LANGUAGE
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger, and Natalie Uomini
- Subjects
Stone tool ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,business.industry ,engineering ,medicine ,engineering.material ,business ,Neuroscience ,Transcranial Doppler - Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Integration of ordinal and metric cues in depth processing
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Marco Bertamini, and Jasna Martinovic
- Subjects
Communication ,Depth Perception ,Vision, Binocular ,Vision Disparity ,business.industry ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Figure–ground ,Luminance ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Random dot stereogram ,Metric (mathematics) ,Binocular disparity ,Contrast (vision) ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,Kinetic depth effect ,Cues ,Depth perception ,business ,Photic Stimulation ,media_common - Abstract
J. Burge, M. A. Peterson, and S. E. Palmer (2005) reported that ordinal, configural cues of familiarity and convexityinfluence perceived depth even when unambiguous metric information in the form of binocular disparity is available. In theirstudy, a shape that was both convex and familiar (i.e., a face) increased perceived depth in random dot stereograms if theshape was shown in the foreground and decreased perceived depth if it was shown in the background. It is generallyassumed that luminance cues are necessary for pre-figural shape representation to influence figure-ground computations inthis way (M. A. Peterson & B. S. Gibson, 1993); thus, Burge et al. (2005) had used a luminance edge. In this research, weasked whether configural cues need to be defined by luminance, contrast, or neither. For a sufficiently large disparitypedestal (about 2.5 arcmin), configural cues influenced perceived depth both for second-order contours and for contoursdefined only by disparity. The integration of ordinal and metric cues seems to be driven by the general saliency of thecontours and not only by luminance information. This challenges the notion that the integration of such cues always needsto arise during figure-ground organization through early combinations of luminance-defined shape and binocular disparity.Keywords: depth perception, figure-ground, disparity, configural cues, cue combination, occlusion, familiarityCitation: Bertamini, M., Martinovic, J., & Wuerger, S. M. (2008). Integration of ordinal and metric cues in depth processing.Journal of Vision, 8(2):10, 1–12, http://journalofvision.org/8/2/10/, doi:10.1167/8.2.10.
- Published
- 2007
40. Estimating discrimination ellipsoids for skin images
- Author
-
Julian Yates, Tushar Chauhan, Sophie Wuerger, and Kaida Xiao
- Subjects
Ophthalmology ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Pattern recognition ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Ellipsoid ,Sensory Systems - Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. The perception of motion in chromatic stimuli
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger and Simon J. Cropper
- Subjects
Parallel processing (psychology) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Luminance ,050105 experimental psychology ,Motion (physics) ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Perception ,Psychophysics ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Visual Pathways ,Chromatic scale ,media_common ,Communication ,Mechanism (biology) ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Computational Biology ,business ,Psychology ,Perceptual Masking ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Color Perception ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The issue of whether there is a motion mechanism sensitive to purely chromatic stimuli has been pertinent for the past 30 or more years. The aim of this review is to examine why such different conclusions have been drawn in the literature and to reach some reconciliation. The review critically examines the behavioral evidence and concludes that there is a purely chromatic motion mechanism but that it is limited to the fovea. Examination of motion performance for chromatic and luminance stimuli provides convincing evidence that there are at least two different mechanisms for the two kinds of stimuli. The authors further argue that the chromatic mechanism may be at a particular disadvantage when the integration of multiple local motion signals is required. Finally, the authors present a descriptive model that may go some way toward explaining the reasons for the differences in collected data outlined in this article.
- Published
- 2006
42. The integration of auditory and visual motion signals at threshold
- Author
-
Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger, and Markus Hofbauer
- Subjects
Auditory perception ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Psychometrics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Visual system ,Motion (physics) ,Discrimination Learning ,Perception ,Sensory threshold ,Orientation ,Psychophysics ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Attention ,Motion perception ,General Psychology ,media_common ,Probability ,Communication ,Models, Statistical ,business.industry ,Sensory Systems ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Sensory Thresholds ,Auditory Perception ,Artificial intelligence ,Psychology ,business - Abstract
To interpret our environment, we integrate information from all our senses. For moving objects, auditory and visual motion signals are correlated and provide information about the speed and the direction of the moving object. We investigated at what level the auditory and the visual modalities interact and whether the human brain integrates only motion signals that are ecologically valid. We found that the sensitivity for identifying motion was improved when motion signals were provided in both modalities. This improvement in sensitivity can be explained by probability summation. That is, auditory and visual stimuli are combined at a decision level, after the stimuli have been processed independently in the auditory and the visual pathways. Furthermore, this integration is direction blind and is not restricted to ecologically valid motion signals.
- Published
- 2004
43. The chromatic input to global motion perception
- Author
-
Marco Bertamini, Sophie Wuerger, and Alexa I. Ruppertsberg
- Subjects
Parvocellular ,Light ,Color vision ,Physiology ,Color ,Cone-opponent mechanisms ,Isoluminance ,Koniocellular ,Motion ,Random-dot kinematogram ,Sensory Systems ,Motion Perception ,Luminance ,Motion (physics) ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Psychophysics ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Motion perception ,Chromatic scale ,Communication ,business.industry ,Koniocellular cell ,Sensory Thresholds ,Human visual system model ,Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells ,Visual Perception ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Psychology ,Artifacts ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
For over 30 years there has been a controversy over whether color-defined motion can be perceived by the human visual system. Some results suggest that there is no chromatic motion mechanism at all, whereas others do find evidence for a purely chromatic motion mechanism. Here we examine the chromatic input to global motion processing for a range of color directions in the photopic luminance range. We measure contrast thresholds for global motion identification and simple detection using sparse random-dot kinematograms. The results show a discrepancy between the two chromatic axes: whereas it is possible for observers to perform the global motion task for stimuli modulated along the red–green axis, we could not assess the contrast threshold required for stimuli modulated along the yellowish-violet axis. The contrast required for detection for both axes, however, are well below the contrasts required for global motion identification. We conclude that there is a significant red–green input to global motion processing providing further evidence for the involvement of the parvocellular pathway. The lack of S-cone input to global motion processing suggests that the koniocellular pathway mediates the detection but not the processing of complex motion for our parameter range.
- Published
- 2003
44. Towards a spatio-chromatic standard observer for detection
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Andrew B. Watson, and Albert J. Ahumada
- Subjects
business.industry ,Ellipsoid ,law.invention ,Linear map ,Achromatic lens ,law ,Line (geometry) ,Diagonal matrix ,Computer vision ,Spatial frequency ,Chromatic scale ,Artificial intelligence ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,business ,Algorithm ,Mathematics - Abstract
The aim of the ColorFest is to extend the original ModelFest (http://vision.arc.nasa.gov/modelfest/) experiments to build a spatio-chromatic standard observer for the detection of static coloured images. The two major issues that need to be addressed are (1) the contrast sensitivity functions for the three chromatic mechanisms and (2) how the output of these channels is combined. We measured detection thresholds for stimuli modulated along different colour directions and for a wide range of spatial frequencies. The three main directions (an achromatic direction, a nominally isoluminant red-green direction, and the tritanopic confusion line) and four intermediate colour directions were used. These intermediate directions were the vector sums of the thresholds along the main directions. We evaluate two models. Detection performance is described by a linear transformation C defining the chromatic tuning and a diagonal matrix S reflecting the sensitivity of the chromatic mechanisms for a particular spatial frequency. The output of the three chromatic mechanisms is combined according to a Minkowski metric (General Separable Model), or according to a Euclidean Distance measure (Ellipsoidal Separable Model). For all three observers the ellipsoidal model fits as well as the general separable model. Estimating the chromatic tuning improves the model fit for one observer.© (2002) COPYRIGHT SPIE--The International Society for Optical Engineering. Downloading of the abstract is permitted for personal use only.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Blur tolerance for luminance and chromatic stimuli
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Huw Owens, and Steve Westland
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Light ,Color vision ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Visual Acuity ,Color ,Color space ,Luminance ,Models, Biological ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Optics ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Psychophysics ,Contrast (vision) ,Humans ,Chromatic scale ,Vision, Ocular ,media_common ,Mathematics ,business.industry ,eye diseases ,Atomic and Molecular Physics, and Optics ,Electronic, Optical and Magnetic Materials ,Sensory Thresholds ,sense organs ,Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition ,Spatial frequency ,Visual angle ,business ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
We investigated the blur tolerance of human observers for stimuli modulated along the isoluminant red-green, the isoluminant yellow-blue, and the luminance (black-white) direction in color space. We report the following results: (i) Blur difference thresholds for red-green and luminance stimuli (of equal cone contrast) are very similar and as low as 0.5 min of visual angle; for yellow-blue the lowest blur thresholds are much higher (1.5 min of visual angle). (ii) The smallest blur thresholds are found for slightly blurred square waves (reference blur of 1 arc min) and not for sharp edges. (iii) Blur thresholds for red-green and black-white follow a Weber law for reference (pedestal) blurs greater than the optimum blur. (iv) Using the model proposed by Watt and Morgan [Vision Res. 24, 1387 (1984)] we estimated the internal blur of the visual system for the black-white and the red-green color directions and arrived at the following estimates: 1.2 arc min for black-white stimuli at 10% contrast and 0.9 arc min for red-green stimuli at 10% cone contrast. Blur tolerance for yellow-blue is independent of external blur and cannot be predicted by the model. (v) The contrast dependence of blur sensitivity is similar for red-green and luminance modulations (slopes of -0.15 and -0.16 in log-log coordinates, respectively) and slightly stronger for yellow-blue (slope = -0.75). Blur discrimination thresholds are not predicted by the contrast sensitivity function of the visual system. Our findings are useful for predicting blur tolerance for complex images and provide a spatial frequency cutoff point when Gaussian low-pass filters are used for noise removal in colored images. They are also useful as a baseline for the study of visual disorders such as amblyopia.
- Published
- 2001
46. The spatio-chromatic sensitivity of the human visual system
- Author
-
Sophie Wuerger, Michael J. Morgan, Steve Westland, and Huw Owens
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Physiology ,business.industry ,Computer science ,Vision Tests ,Biomedical Engineering ,Biophysics ,Visual Acuity ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Visual sensitivity ,Visual orientation ,eye diseases ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Parvocellular cell ,Physiology (medical) ,Space Perception ,Human visual system model ,Humans ,Computer vision ,Chromatic scale ,Spatial frequency ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Color Perception ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
The response of the human visual system depends on a multitude of image features, such as the wavelength (colour) of the visual stimulus and its spatial frequency content. Hence we need to take into account the spatial and chromatic sensitivity as well as spatio-chromatic interactions to properly characterize visual sensitivity. In this paper we report two experiments that further characterize the spatio-chromatic sensitivity of the human visual system for stationary stimuli, namely the detection of small visual orientation differences and the detection of blur. In both cases we find that the visual system is equally sensitive to red-green and to black-white modulations for a wide range of spatial parameters. Furthermore, the contrast dependence for red-green and black-white modulations is identical, suggesting that the same mechanism mediates both types of stimulus. Our results are in accordance with the hypothesis that both tasks are mediated by the parvocellular as opposed to the magnocellular pathway.
- Published
- 2000
47. Perception of moving lines: interactions between local perpendicular signals and 2D motion signals
- Author
-
Eric Castet and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Male ,Time Factors ,Rotation ,Aperture ,Acoustics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Integration ,Motion Perception ,Motion (geometry) ,Signal ,Optics ,Orientation (geometry) ,Perception ,Perpendicular ,Humans ,Aperture problem ,Features ,media_common ,2D motion ,Physics ,business.industry ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Feature (computer vision) ,Line (geometry) ,Female ,business - Abstract
An oblique line translating vertically behind a horizontal rectangular aperture is perceived as moving in the horizontal direction, i.e., in the line-ending's direction. When a feature is added on the line, and thus provides a vertical unambiguous motion signal, the line's perceived direction is still horizontal. In parallel, the feature appears to slide obliquely along the line ( Wallach, 1935 ). We first show that this finding which we call “the sliding effect” is robust and easy to replicate for different orientations of the rectangular aperture (up to about 20 deg from vertical). This effect also occurs with an invisible circular aperture. In this case, using an adjustment task, observers have great difficulty extracting the actual direction of the feature (a gap or a dot on the line). Instead, a systematic bias towards the direction of “sliding” is observed. This misperception of the feature's velocity is markedly reduced or even suppressed when the circular aperture is outlined or when a visible circle is drawn around this invisible aperture. The line's perceived direction is always roughly orthogonal to the line's orientation regardless of the presence of a visible circular outline. This latter result is important because it shows that the feature on the line does not disambiguate the perpendicular signals extracted along the contour, even in conditions where this feature's motion is almost correctly perceived. Altogether, these results suggest that line-endings are not used by the visual system in the same way as a feature on the line when it comes to determining the line's perceived direction. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
- Published
- 1997
48. Proximity judgments in color space: tests of a Euclidean color geometry
- Author
-
Laurence T. Maloney, John Krauskopf, and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Male ,Similarity (geometry) ,Psychometrics ,Color vision ,Color similarity ,Salience of colors ,Color space ,Measure (mathematics) ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Judgment ,0302 clinical medicine ,Optics ,11. Sustainability ,Euclidean geometry ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Mathematics ,Color difference ,Plane (geometry) ,business.industry ,Distance Perception ,05 social sciences ,Pattern recognition ,Color geometry ,Sensory Systems ,Ophthalmology ,Space Perception ,Metric (mathematics) ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Color Perception - Abstract
We describe two tests of the hypothesis that human judgments of the proximity of colors are consistent with a Euclidean geometry on color matching space. The first test uses proximity judgments to measure the angle between any two intersecting lines in color space. Pairwise estimates of the angles between three lines in a plane were made in order to test the additivity of angles. Three different color proximity tasks were considered. Additivity failed for each of the three proximity tasks. Secondly, we tested a prediction concerning the growth of the variability of judgments of similarity with the distance between the test and reference stimuli. The Euclidean hypothesis was also rejected by this test. The results concerning the growth of variability are consistent with the assumption that observers use a city-block metric when judging the proximity of colored lights.
- Published
- 1995
49. 独特色调设置随年龄变化的研究
- Author
-
Kaida Xiao, Sophie Wuerger, Dimosthenis Karatzas, and Chenyang Fu
- Subjects
genetic structures ,business.industry ,Color vision ,Atomic and Molecular Physics, and Optics ,Unique hues ,Electronic, Optical and Magnetic Materials ,Large sample ,Range finding ,Optics ,Ageing ,Chromatic scale ,Electrical and Electronic Engineering ,business ,Mathematics ,Hue - Abstract
Clromatic sensitivity along the protan, deutan, and tritan lines and the loci of the unique hues (red, green, yellow, blue) for a very large sample (n = 185) of colour-normal observers ranging from 18 to 75 years of age are assessed. Visual judgments are obtained under normal viewing conditions using colour patches on self-luminous display under controlled adaptation conditions. Trivector discrimination thresholds show an increase as a function of age along the protan, deutan, and tritan axes, with the largest increase present along the tritan line, less pronounced shifts in unique hue settings are also observed. Based on the chromatic (protan, deutan, tritan) thresholds and using scaled cone signals, we predict the unique hue changes with ageing. A dependency on age for unique red and unique yellow for predicted hue angle is found. We conclude that the chromatic sensitivity deteriorates significantly with age, whereas the appearance of unique hues is much less affected, remaining almost constant despite the known changes in the ocular media.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Role of chromatic and luminance contrast in inferring structure from motion
- Author
-
Michael S. Landy and Sophie Wuerger
- Subjects
Light ,Color vision ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Motion detection ,Luminance ,Atomic and Molecular Physics, and Optics ,Electronic, Optical and Magnetic Materials ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Form Perception ,Optics ,Sensory Thresholds ,Psychophysics ,Structure from motion ,Contrast (vision) ,Humans ,Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition ,Spatial frequency ,Chromatic scale ,business ,Color Perception ,Mathematics ,media_common - Abstract
We measured the ability to infer structure from motion (SFM) in several directions in three-dimensional color space. Only motion cues are useful to subjects in performing this three-dimensional shape-identification task. We report the following results: (1) SFM performance is at chance for equiluminant stimuli that isolate short-wavelength-sensitive cones. Hence the short-wavelength-sensitive-cone input to SFM is negligible. (2) SFM performance increases with the magnitude of delta L - delta M signal when delta L + delta M = 0 (i.e., only chromatic and no luminance contrast is available). We reject the hypothesis that SFM obtains input from a single chromatic mechanism combining the long- and medium-wavelength-sensitive cones linearly. Our data are compatible with SFM that uses the output of two mechanisms, one taking the difference between the long- and medium-wavelength-sensitive-cone signals and the other taking the respective sum. We reject the particular hypothesis that SFM utilizes only the magnitude and not the sign of the long- and medium-wavelength-sensitive-cone signal. (3) We compare SFM performance with threshold performance for velocity and motion discrimination. Stimuli with luminance contrast yield SFM performance that is superior to stimuli without luminance contrast when they are expressed as multiples of velocity discrimination threshold. This superiority is even greater when SFM performance is compared with motion-direction discrimination thresholds.
- Published
- 1993
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.