79 results on '"CADINU M"'
Search Results
52. Niscemi
- Author
-
ANTISTA, Giuseppe, Casamento, A, Antista, A, Antista, G, Barbera, P, Cadinu, M, Migliore, V, Miceli,P, Orlando, C, Peghin, G, Pinna, R, Pinzarrone, L, Raia, F, Rigamonti, F, Salamone, L, Sanjust, P, Sardina, P, Schirru, M, Sessa, E, Silvestri, A, Vesco, M, and Zanini, L
- Subjects
città di fondazione ,Niscemi ,Settore ICAR/18 - Storia Dell'Architettura - Abstract
Niscemi, adagiato su un altopiano nella Sicilia centro-meridionale, sorge sui resti di un insediamento di antica origine; la fondazione si deve ai Branciforte, una delle più potenti famiglie feudali dell’isola, che estende il suo controllo feudale su tante altre città (Leonforte, Militello, Mazzarino, Grammichele).
- Published
- 2013
53. Le piazze quadrate ad angoli chiusi nelle città siciliane di fondazione. Alcuni casi di studio
- Author
-
ANTISTA, Giuseppe, Casamento, A, Greco, A, Colletta, T, Villa, G, Orlando, C, Sardina, P, Cadinu, M, Silvestri, A, Pinna, R, Lepri, G, Pinzarrone, L, Rigamonti, F, Antista, G, Finella, A, Antista, A, Migliore, V, Barucci, C, Valensise, F, Genovese, RA, Friello, I, Romano, A, Sanjust, P, Iterar, C, Ciranna, S, Barbera, P, Salamone, L, and Antista, G.
- Subjects
Cattolica Eraclea, Palma di Montechiaro, piazze, città di fondazione ,Settore ICAR/18 - Storia Dell'Architettura - Abstract
La piazza delle città fondate in Sicilia tra Cinquecento e Settecento dalla classe baronale accoglie solitamente la sede del potere politico e religioso, palesata nel palazzo del fondatore e nella chiesa Madre, e con la sua posizione orienta la trama viaria, che è di regola a maglia ortogonale. Un caso particolare, non molto diffuso in Sicilia, è rappresentato dalla piazza ad angoli chiusi, attraversata nella mezzeria dei lati da una croce di strade, i cui esempi più noti possono individuarsi nelle piazze di Cattolica Eraclea (1610) e Palma di Montechiaro (1637), che nel testo vengono analizzate.
- Published
- 2013
54. Gender differences in implicit gender self-categorization lead to stronger gender self-stereotyping by women than by men
- Author
-
Cadinu, MARIA ROSARIA, Galdi, Silvia, Cadinu, M., and Galdi, S.
- Abstract
It was hypothesized that, in natural group contexts, low-status in-group membership would be highly accessible, whereas membership to high-status groups would not. Therefore, gender group membership was predicted to be more accessible for women than for men. It was further hypothesized that the high accessibility of gender group membership would lead to stronger self-stereotyping for women than for men. To measure the accessibility of gender group membership, participants performed a Gender Self-Categorization Implicit Association Test (Studies 1 and 2), measuring the strength of automatic associations between the self and the gender in-group. Participants also performed a Self-Stereotyping Implicit Association Test (Study 2), assessing the strength of automatic associations between the self and the stereotypical traits of the in-group. As expected, implicit gender self-categorization and implicit gender self-stereotyping were stronger for women than for men. Importantly, implicit gender self-categorization mediated the relation between gender and self-stereotyping. Therefore, implicit gender self-categorization was the mechanism underlying stronger implicit self-stereotyping by women.
- Published
- 2012
55. Piazza di Monte Cavallo e la via Pia
- Author
-
MICALIZZI, Paolo, CADINU M, and Micalizzi, Paolo
- Subjects
MONTE ,CAVALLO ,PIAZZA - Abstract
Il saggio prende in esame un affresco tardo-cinquecentesco di Cesare Nebbia e collaboratori che rappresenta la via Pia e il palazzo del Quirinale (in corso di costruzione). Sviluppando alcune considerazioni sul punto di osservazione scelto dai pittori, vengono "scoperte" alcune particolarità dell'assetto di Piazza del Quirinale alla fine del Cinquecento. The essay examines a late-sixteenth-century fresco by Cesare Nebbia and colleagues representing the Via Pia and the Quirinale Palace (under construction. Developing some considerations on the view point chosen by painters, are "discovered" some special characteristics of the Piazza del Quirinale in the late sixteenth century.
- Published
- 2012
56. Girls' math performance under stereotype threat: the moderating role of mothers' gender stereotypes
- Author
-
Carlo Tomasetto, Mara Cadinu, Francesca Romana Alparone, Tomasetto C., Alparone F.R., and Cadinu M.
- Subjects
Adult ,media_common.quotation_subject ,education ,Vulnerability ,Mothers ,Stereotype ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Developmental psychology ,Random Allocation ,PARENTS ,children ,mental disorders ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Life-span and Life-course Studies ,Child ,Demography ,media_common ,Preschool child ,Sexual identity ,Stereotyping ,School age child ,Gender identity ,math gender stereotype ,stereotype threat ,mathematics ,Gender Identity ,Middle Aged ,Social relation ,Mother-Child Relations ,Stereotype threat ,Child, Preschool ,Educational Status ,Regression Analysis ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Mathematics - Abstract
Previous research on stereotype threat in children suggests that making gender identity salient disrupts girls’ math performance at as early as 5 to 7 years of age. The present study (n = 124) tested the hypothesis that parents’ endorsement of gender stereotypes about math moderates girls’ susceptibility to stereotype threat. Results confirmed that stereotype threat impaired girls’ performance on math tasks among students from kindergarten through 2nd grade. Moreover, mothers’ but not fathers’ endorsement of gender stereotypes about math moderated girls’ vulnerability to stereotype threat: Performance of girls whose mothers strongly rejected the gender stereotype about math did not decrease under stereotype threat. These findings are important because they point to the role of mothers’ beliefs in the development of girls’ vulnerability to the negative effects of gender stereotypes about math.
- Published
- 2011
57. The role of implicit and explicit body dissatisfaction in the prediction of diet behavior
- Author
-
DI RISO, Daniela, Chessa, Daphne, Verzeletti, Chiara, Galdi, Silvia, Carraro, Luciana, Cadinu, M., Galdi, S., Maass, A., Di Riso, Daniela, Chessa, Daphne, Verzeletti, Chiara, Galdi, Silvia, and Carraro, Luciana
- Published
- 2011
58. Relations between implicit attitudes and dietary behaviors among Italian adolescents
- Author
-
Verzeletti, Chiara, DI RISO, Daniela, Chessa, Daphne, Galdi, Silvia, Carraro, Luciana, Loredana, Laghezza, Cadinu, M., Galdi, S., Maass, A., Verzeletti, Chiara, Di Riso, Daniela, Chessa, Daphne, Galdi, Silvia, Carraro, Luciana, and Laghezza, Loredana
- Published
- 2011
59. Consequences of implicit-explicit attitude inconsistency
- Author
-
ZOGMAISTER, CRISTINA, Cadinu, M, Galdi, S, Maass, A, and Zogmaister, C
- Subjects
implicit attitude ,attitude inconsistency ,explicit attitude ,social cognition - Published
- 2011
60. The Cognitive Representation of Self-Stereotyping
- Author
-
Andrea Carnaghi, Maria Rosaria Cadinu, Jeroen Vaes, Marcella Latrofa, Latrofa, M., Vaes, J., Cadinu, M., and Carnaghi, Andrea
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Social Psychology ,Adolescent ,Self-stereotyping ,Cognitive structure ,Hierarchy, Social ,Models, Psychological ,Social group ,Young Adult ,Cognition ,Sex Factors ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,"ingroup stereotyping" ,Humans ,Self anchoring ,Projection ,"self-stereotyping" ,"majority and minority context" ,Stereotyping ,Social Identification ,Self ,Representation (systemics) ,Ingroups and outgroups ,Self Concept ,Social Isolation ,Social Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Prejudice - Abstract
The present work looks at the self-stereotyping process and reveals its underlying cognitive structure. When this process occurs, it is necessarily the result of an overlap between the representation of the ingroup and that of the self. Two studies measured this overlap and showed that it was higher on stereotype-relevant than on stereotype-irrelevant traits, it involved both positive and negative stereotypical traits, and it implied a deduction-to-the-self process of ingroup stereotypical dimensions. Moreover, the status of one’s social group was found to be a key variable in this process, showing that self-stereotyping is limited to low-status group members. Indeed, results of Study 2 showed that the overlap between the self and the ingroup for high-status group members was the result of an induction-to-the-ingroup process of personal characteristics. Implications for research on people’s self-construal are discussed.
- Published
- 2010
61. Nomina sunt omina: On the inductive potential of nouns vs. adjectives in person perception
- Author
-
Mara Cadinu, Sara Gresta, Andrea Carnaghi, Anne Maass, Mauro Bianchi, Luciano Arcuri, Carnaghi, Andrea, Maass, A, Gresta, S, Bianchi, M, Cadinu, M, and Arcuri, L.
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Culture ,Stereotype ,linguaggio ,Personality Assessment ,German ,Social cognition ,Noun ,Similarity (psychology) ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,media_common ,Stereotyping ,Psycholinguistics ,Social Identification ,Social perception ,Verbal Behavior ,essenzialismo ,stereotipi ,percezione sociale ,categorizzazione sociale ,language.human_language ,Semantics ,Categorization ,language ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Adjective - Abstract
Six studies (N = 491) investigated the inductive potential of nouns versus adjectives in person perception. In the first 5 studies, targets were either described by an adjective (e.g., Mark is homosexual) or by the corresponding noun (e.g., Mark is a homosexual) or by both (Study 3). The authors predicted and found that nouns, more so than adjectives, (a) facilitate descriptor-congruent inferences but inhibit incongruent inferences (Studies 1-3), (b) inhibit alternative classifications (Study 4), and (c) imply essentialism of congruent but not of incongruent preferences (Study 5). This was supported for different group memberships and inclinations (athletics, arts, religion, sexual preference, drinking behavior, etc.), languages (Italian and German), and response formats, suggesting that despite the surface similarity of nouns and adjectives, nouns have a more powerful impact on person perception. Study 6 investigated the inverse relationship, showing that more essentialist beliefs (in terms of a genetic predisposition rather than training) lead speakers to use more nouns and fewer adjectives. Possible extensions of G. R. Semin and K. Fiedler's (1988) linguistic category model and potential applications for language use in intergroup contexts are discussed.
- Published
- 2008
62. Napoli metropoli medievale del Trecento La costruzione della città portuale e mercantile nel Trecento
- Author
-
COLLETTA, TERESA, CADINU M., and Colletta, Teresa
- Published
- 2007
63. The best way to tell you to use a condom: The interplay between message format and individuals’ level of need for cognition
- Author
-
Andrea Carnaghi, Luigi Castelli, Jeff Kiesner, C. Bragantini, Maria Rosaria Cadinu, Carnaghi, Andrea, Cadinu, M., Castelli, L., Kiesner, J., and Bragantini, C.
- Subjects
Male ,Safe Sex ,Need for cognition ,Persuasion ,Health (social science) ,AIDS ,Condom ,Prevention ,Adolescent ,Social Psychology ,Message format ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Persuasive Communication ,education ,HIV Infections ,"prevention" ,Condoms ,Theory of reasoned action ,Cognition ,Humans ,"Need for cognition" ,media_common ,Motivation ,Public Health, Environmental and Occupational Health ,Social environment ,Mental health ,"Message format" ,Female ,Norm (social) ,Psychology ,Attitude to Health ,Social psychology - Abstract
The present study addressed how individuals in high versus low need for cognition react to a persuasive message (concerning safer sexual conduct) presented either in a written format or in a comic-strip format. A control group that did not receive any persuasive message was also included. With reference to the Theory of Reasoned Action, we analyzed participants' instrumental attitude and instrumental norm toward the use of condoms. Results indicated that, compared to participants in the control group, providing participants with the persuasive message bolstered their instrumental attitude and norm. More importantly, participants high in need for cognition displayed higher levels of both instrumental attitude and norm when the message was in a written, as compared to a comic-strip, format. In contrast, participants low in need for cognition reported a stronger level of both instrumental attitude and norm in reaction to a comic-strip than a written message. Results are discussed with respect to their theoretical and practical implications.
- Published
- 2007
64. National disparities favoring males are reflected in girls' implicit associations about gender and academic subjects.
- Author
-
Cvencek D, Sanders EA, Del Río MF, Susperreguy MI, Strasser K, Brečić R, Gaćeša D, Skala D, Tomasetto C, Galdi S, Cadinu M, Kapur M, Passolunghi MC, Rueda Ferreira TI, Mirisola A, Mariani B, and Meltzoff AN
- Abstract
Based on data for N = 2,756 children (1,410 girls; M
age = 8.10 years) from 16 data sets spanning five nations, this study investigated relations between national gender disparities and children's beliefs about gender and academic subjects. One national-level gender disparity involved inequalities in socioeconomic standing favoring adult males over females (U.N. Human Development Index). The other involved national-level gaps in standardized math achievement, favoring boys over girls (Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study Grade 4). Three novel findings emerged. First, girls' results from a Child Implicit Association Test showed that implicit associations linking boys with math and girls with reading were positively related to both national male advantages in socioeconomic standing and national boy advantages in Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study. Second, these relations were obtained for implicit but not explicit measures of children's beliefs linking gender and academic subjects. Third, implicit associations linking gender to academic subjects increased significantly as a function of children's age. We propose a psychological account of why national gender disparities are likely to influence children's developing implicit associations about gender and academic subjects, especially for girls. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
65. #SexyBodyPositive: When Sexualization Does Not Undermine Young Women's Body Image.
- Author
-
Di Michele D, Guizzo F, Canale N, Fasoli F, Carotta F, Pollini A, and Cadinu M
- Subjects
- Humans, Female, Affect, Personal Satisfaction, Beauty, Social Networking, Body Image, Social Media
- Abstract
Research suggests that exposure to social networking sites portraying a thin and often sexualized beauty ideal reduces young women's body satisfaction, while exposure to body-positive content improves it. However, it is unclear whether sexualization could impair the beneficial effects of body-positivity messages. Young Italian women were exposed to one of three experimental conditions showing sexualized beauty ideals, sexualized body positivity, or non-sexualized body positivity that appeared either on Instagram (Study 1, N = 356) or TikTok (Study 2, N = 316). Across the two studies, results showed that, regardless of sexualization, exposure to body positivity increased body satisfaction and positive mood compared with pre-exposure measures, while exposure to sexualized beauty ideals reduced it. Participants in the sexualized beauty ideal condition also engaged in upward appearance social comparison whereas body positivity elicited downward comparison. Problematic social networking sites' use moderated the effects of condition on body satisfaction, appearance social comparison, and positive mood, while downward comparison mediated the relation between condition and body satisfaction and positive mood. Our results highlight both beneficial and critical aspects of body positivity that should be taken into consideration when designing body image interventions and policymaking.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
66. Sexualization of Sexual Harassment Victims Reduces Bystanders' Help: The Mediating Role of Attribution of Immorality and Blame.
- Author
-
Gramazio S, Cadinu M, Pagliaro S, and Pacilli MG
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Morals, Social Perception, Workplace, Crime Victims, Sexual Harassment
- Abstract
Women's representation in social media is becoming increasingly sexualized, even when they are victims of sexual harassment (SH). In the present research, we adopted a bystander approach to investigate the role of victims' sexualization on bystanders' reactions to an episode of SH. In Study 1, female participants read a fictitious newspaper article that described a workplace SH episode: According to condition, the article included a picture of the victim who was wearing either sexualized or nonsexualized clothing. In Study 2, which also included male participants, we used a similar procedure and measured a series of traditional beliefs against women equality. As predicted, participants showed lower willingness to help the sexualized than nonsexualized victim: This effect occurred because they attributed lower morality to the victim and blamed her more for the SH event. Study 2 very well replicated Study 1 results and also showed that higher levels of endorsement of traditional masculine norms further enhanced biased perception of the sexualized (vs. nonsexualized) victim. Together, findings suggest that biased evaluations of workplace SH episodes associated with sexualized victims' appearance, consistent with traditional masculine norms, may have detrimental consequences by increasing legitimization and tolerance toward SH.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
67. Group meaningfulness and the causal direction of influence between the ingroup and the self or another individual: Evidence from the Induction-Deduction Paradigm.
- Author
-
Cadinu M, Carnaghi A, and Guizzo F
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Social Identification, Social Perception, Sociometric Techniques, Stereotyping, Peer Influence, Self Concept
- Abstract
The goal of the present study was to investigate the causal direction of influence between the ingroup as a whole and the self or another ingroup member considering a key feature of groups, i.e., their perceived meaningfulness. To this goal, in Study 1, 2, and 3 we predicted a preference for self-stereotyping and ingroup-stereotyping in the meaningful social categories of sorority women, left-handed people and psychology students. In Study 4 we further expect that the meaningfulness attributed to a group moderates the direction of causality between individual and ingroup perception. Thus, we used one's Zodiac sign as the ingroup whose degree of meaningfulness varies across participants and we hypothesized higher levels of meaningfulness attributed to the ingroup to be associated with higher self- and ingroup-stereotyping. Using the methodologically stringent Induction Deduction Paradigm, participants were given information on unfamiliar dimensions, about either the ingroup or an individual (self or other ingroup member) and asked to make inferences on those same attributes about the ingroup (induction condition) or the individual (deduction condition). As predicted, a preference for deduction to the self (i.e., self-stereotyping) and deduction to another ingroup member (i.e., ingroup-stereotyping) were found for the meaningful groups of sorority women, left-handed people, and Psychology students (Studies 1, 2, and 3). In Study 4, consistent with predictions, the higher the level of attributed meaningfulness to the Zodiac system the higher the degree of deduction both to the self (self-stereotyping) and to another Zodiac ingroup member (ingroup-stereotyping). Several implications of these results are discussed, for example in relation to the possibility of educational interventions aimed at invalidating intergroup differences., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
68. The role of implicit gender spatial stereotyping in mental rotation performance.
- Author
-
Guizzo F, Moè A, Cadinu M, and Bertolli C
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Rotation, Sex Factors, Young Adult, Imaging, Three-Dimensional methods, Photic Stimulation methods, Sexism psychology, Stereotyping, Thinking physiology
- Abstract
Men outscore women in mental rotation. Among the possible explanations for this result are gender stereotypes. Research has shown that instructions confirming or disconfirming the gender stereotype that men are more talented than women may affect performance in some spatial tasks, such as mental rotation, but research so far has shown inconsistent or null results. However, no research to date has assessed whether participants' implicit associations linking men to spatial abilities may modulate these effects. Thus, the goal of this study was to assess the moderating role of the implicit gender spatial stereotyping, that is the automatic associations between men vs. women and space, in male and female participants receiving either stereotypical (stating that men outscore women) or stereotype-nullifying (stating that there is no gender difference) explicit instructions. Results confirmed that men performed better than women in mental rotation, but also showed that in the stereotype-nullifying condition, the higher the automatic associations between space and men the lower men's performance. The discussion focuses on the importance of considering implicit gender spatial stereotyping as a factor that can modulate mental rotation performance., (Copyright © 2019. Published by Elsevier B.V.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
69. Effects of objectifying gaze on female cognitive performance: The role of flow experience and internalization of beauty ideals.
- Author
-
Guizzo F and Cadinu M
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Beauty, Defense Mechanisms, Female, Humans, Male, Neuropsychological Tests, Young Adult, Body Image psychology, Cognition physiology, Dehumanization, Interpersonal Relations, Self Concept
- Abstract
Although previous research has demonstrated that objectification impairs female cognitive performance, no research to date has investigated the mechanisms underlying such decrement. Therefore, we tested the role of flow experience as one mechanism leading to performance decrement under sexual objectification. Gaze gender was manipulated by having male versus female experimenters take body pictures of female participants (N = 107) who then performed a Sustained Attention to Response Task. As predicted, a moderated mediation model showed that under male versus female gaze, higher internalization of beauty ideals was associated with lower flow, which in turn decreased performance. The implications of these results are discussed in relation to objectification theory and strategies to prevent sexually objectifying experiences., (© 2016 The British Psychological Society.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
70. An Integrative Model of Social Identification: Self-Stereotyping and Self-Anchoring as Two Cognitive Pathways.
- Author
-
van Veelen R, Otten S, Cadinu M, and Hansen N
- Subjects
- Humans, Models, Psychological, Cognition, Self Concept, Social Identification, Stereotyping
- Abstract
Social identification denotes individuals' psychological bond with their ingroup. It is an indispensable construct in research on intragroup and intergroup dynamics. Today's understanding of social identification is firmly grounded in self-stereotyping principles (i.e., assimilation to the ingroup prototype). However, we argue for a more integrative approach to understand social identification, including a more prominent role for the personal self. We present the Integrative Model of Social Identification (IMSI) and postulate that there are two cognitive pathways to self-group overlap that can simultaneously yet distinctly explain social identification: self-stereotyping and self-anchoring (i.e., projection of personal self onto ingroup). We review different theoretical and methodological approaches to both processes and integrate them into one model. Subsequently, we empirically demonstrate the positive relationship between self-stereotyping, self-anchoring, and identification in various group contexts and individuals. In sum, our model highlights the dynamic interplay of personal and social self as cornerstones of social identification., (© 2015 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.)
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
71. Women drive better if not stereotyped.
- Author
-
Moè A, Cadinu M, and Maass A
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Italy, Male, Sex Factors, Task Performance and Analysis, Young Adult, Automobile Driving psychology, Automobile Driving statistics & numerical data, Safety statistics & numerical data, Stereotyping
- Abstract
A commonly held stereotype is that women are poor drivers. This stereotype is recognized and endorsed by women and girls very early on, long before taking their driving licence, nevertheless they are less involved in accidents and drive safer and less fast than men. In line with the stereotype threat theory, the present study tests the hypothesis that making the driving stereotype salient will lead women to underperform in a driving simulation task. In Experiment 1women in the stereotype threat condition were told that the aim of the study was to detect gender differences in driving whereas in a control condition no study aim was provided. In Experiment 2, two conditions were compared: stereotype threat (same instructions as in Experiment 1), and stereotype boost (the alleged goal was to compare driving ability of young vs. old people). As predicted, the results of both experiments showed that women under stereotype threat, as compared to either control or stereotype boost participants, doubled the number of mistakes. Nevertheless, they overall expected/self-reported to drive/have driven poorly. Importantly, their level of expectation was a significant predictor of their actual driving performance only in the stereotype threat condition. Implications of these effects of stereotype threat on women's driving performance and self-assessment are discussed., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
72. The roots of stereotype threat: when automatic associations disrupt girls' math performance.
- Author
-
Galdi S, Cadinu M, and Tomasetto C
- Subjects
- Child, Female, Humans, Male, Sex Factors, Association, Mathematics education, Stereotyping, Task Performance and Analysis
- Abstract
Although stereotype awareness is a prerequisite for stereotype threat effects (Steele & Aronson, 1995), research showed girls' deficit under stereotype threat before the emergence of math-gender stereotype awareness, and in the absence of stereotype endorsement. In a study including 240 six-year-old children, this paradox was addressed by testing whether automatic associations trigger stereotype threat in young girls. Whereas no indicators were found that children endorsed the math-gender stereotype, girls, but not boys, showed automatic associations consistent with the stereotype. Moreover, results showed that girls' automatic associations varied as a function of a manipulation regarding the stereotype content. Importantly, girls' math performance decreased in a stereotype-consistent, relative to a stereotype-inconsistent, condition and automatic associations mediated the relation between stereotype threat and performance., (© 2013 The Authors. Child Development © 2013 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.)
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
73. Self-stereotyping: the central role of an ingroup threatening identity.
- Author
-
Latrofa M, Vaes J, and Cadinu M
- Subjects
- Achievement, Adolescent, Adult, Female, Gender Identity, Hierarchy, Social, Humans, Male, Motivation, Social Desirability, Surveys and Questionnaires, Young Adult, Self Concept, Social Identification, Social Stigma, Stereotyping
- Abstract
Self-stereotyping is a process by which people belonging to a stigmatized social group tend to describe themselves more with stereotypical traits as compared with traits irrelevant to the ingroup stereotype. The present work analyzes why especially members of low-status groups are more inclined to self-stereotype compared to members of high-status groups. We tested the hypothesis that belonging to a low-, rather than a high-status group, makes low-status members feel more threatened and motivates them to protect their self-perception by increasing their similarity with the ingroup. Specifically, we investigated the effects of an experimental manipulation that was conceived to either threaten or protect the natural group membership of participants from either a low- or a high-status group on the level of self-stereotyping. The findings supported the idea that only low-status group members protected themselves when their group identity was threatened through increased self-stereotyping.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
74. Girls' math performance under stereotype threat: the moderating role of mothers' gender stereotypes.
- Author
-
Tomasetto C, Alparone FR, and Cadinu M
- Subjects
- Adult, Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Humans, Middle Aged, Random Allocation, Regression Analysis, Educational Status, Gender Identity, Mathematics, Mother-Child Relations, Mothers psychology, Stereotyping
- Abstract
Previous research on stereotype threat in children suggests that making gender identity salient disrupts girls' math performance at as early as 5 to 7 years of age. The present study (n = 124) tested the hypothesis that parents' endorsement of gender stereotypes about math moderates girls' susceptibility to stereotype threat. Results confirmed that stereotype threat impaired girls' performance on math tasks among students from kindergarten through 2nd grade. Moreover, mothers' but not fathers' endorsement of gender stereotypes about math moderated girls' vulnerability to stereotype threat: performance of girls whose mothers strongly rejected the gender stereotype about math did not decrease under stereotype threat. These findings are important because they point to the role of mothers' beliefs in the development of girls' vulnerability to the negative effects of gender stereotypes about math., (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
75. Nomina sunt omina: on the inductive potential of nouns and adjectives in person perception.
- Author
-
Carnaghi A, Maass A, Gresta S, Bianchi M, Cadinu M, and Arcuri L
- Subjects
- Adult, Culture, Female, Humans, Male, Personality Assessment, Psycholinguistics, Social Identification, Verbal Behavior, Interpersonal Relations, Semantics, Stereotyping
- Abstract
Six studies (N = 491) investigated the inductive potential of nouns versus adjectives in person perception. In the first 5 studies, targets were either described by an adjective (e.g., Mark is homosexual) or by the corresponding noun (e.g., Mark is a homosexual) or by both (Study 3). The authors predicted and found that nouns, more so than adjectives, (a) facilitate descriptor-congruent inferences but inhibit incongruent inferences (Studies 1-3), (b) inhibit alternative classifications (Study 4), and (c) imply essentialism of congruent but not of incongruent preferences (Study 5). This was supported for different group memberships and inclinations (athletics, arts, religion, sexual preference, drinking behavior, etc.), languages (Italian and German), and response formats, suggesting that despite the surface similarity of nouns and adjectives, nouns have a more powerful impact on person perception. Study 6 investigated the inverse relationship, showing that more essentialist beliefs (in terms of a genetic predisposition rather than training) lead speakers to use more nouns and fewer adjectives. Possible extensions of G. R. Semin and K. Fiedler's (1988) linguistic category model and potential applications for language use in intergroup contexts are discussed., ((c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved)
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
76. Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking.
- Author
-
Cadinu M, Maass A, Rosabianca A, and Kiesner J
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Affect, Mathematics, Stereotyping, Thinking
- Abstract
This study investigated the role of negative thinking as a potential mediator of performance deficits under stereotype threat. After being assigned to a stereotype-threat or a no-threat condition, 60 female participants were asked to complete a difficult math task. Using the thought-listing technique, women under stereotype threat reported a higher number of negative thoughts specifically related to the test and to mathematics compared with women in the no-threat condition. Moreover, women under stereotype threat also showed a sharp decrease in performance that (a) was most pronounced in the second half of the test and (b) was mediated by the increase in negative thinking.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
77. Sexual harassment under social identity threat: the computer harassment paradigm.
- Author
-
Maass A, Cadinu M, Guarnieri G, and Grasselli A
- Subjects
- Adult, Defense Mechanisms, Erotica, Feminism, Humans, Individuality, Male, Personality Inventory, Risk Factors, Stereotyping, Gender Identity, Sexual Harassment psychology, Social Identification, User-Computer Interface
- Abstract
Two laboratory experiments investigated the hypothesis that threat to male identity would increase the likelihood of gender harassment. In both experiments, using the computer harassment paradigm, male university students (N=80 in Experiment 1, N=90 in Experiment 2) were exposed to different types of identity threat (legitimacy threat and threat to group value in Experiment 1 and distinctiveness threat and prototypicality threat in Experiment 2) or to no threat and were then given the opportunity to send pornographic material to a virtual female interaction partner. Results show that (a) participants harassed the female interaction partner more when they were exposed to a legitimacy, distinctiveness, or prototypicality threat than to no threat; (b) this was mainly true for highly identified males; and (c) harassment enhanced postexperimental gender identification. Results are interpreted as supporting a social identity account of gender harassment.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
78. Group identification in early adolescence: its relation with peer adjustment and its moderator effect on peer influence.
- Author
-
Kiesner J, Cadinu M, Poulin F, and Bucci M
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Child, Dominance-Subordination, Female, Humans, Individuality, Longitudinal Studies, Male, Risk Factors, Sociometric Techniques, Antisocial Personality Disorder psychology, Juvenile Delinquency psychology, Peer Group, Psychology, Adolescent, Social Adjustment, Social Facilitation, Social Identification
- Abstract
This study was conducted to test whether group identification (importance of the group to the individual) covaried with individual-group similarity on problem behavior; and whether group identification moderated peer group influence on the individual's development of delinquent behavior across a 1-year period. The level of reciprocated nominations within the individual's self-nominated group was controlled for in all analyses. Participants were 190 sixth and seventh graders (during the first year of the study) from the north of Italy. Level of reciprocated nominations within the group, but not identification, was found to covary with individual-group behavioral similarity (group behavior interacted with reciprocity of group nominations in predicting individual behavior). Group identification, but not reciprocated nominations, was found to moderate peer group influence on the individual's change in delinquent behavior, across 1 year. The individual's peer status within the classroom, level of reciprocated nominations, and gender all were related to the individual's level of group identification. Results are discussed in terms of understanding peer group influence on the individual.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
79. Self-anchoring and differentiation processes in the minimal group setting.
- Author
-
Cadinu MR and Rothbart M
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Generalization, Psychological, Humans, Individuality, Male, Social Facilitation, Individuation, Self Concept, Social Conformity, Social Identification
- Abstract
In-group favoritism in the minimal group setting was hypothesized to be a function of 2 processes: a tendency to base in-group judgments on the self (self-anchoring) and a tendency to assume 1 group to be opposite of the other (differentiation). In the first 3 experiments, in which the order of rating the self and target group was varied, was categorized and uncategorized participants were given trait information about 1 group and were asked to estimate the level of those traits in the other group. In-group judges tended to base group ratings on the self, whereas out-group and uncategorized judges inferred the 2 groups to be opposite of one another. Experiment 4 attempted to directly assess the direction of inference between self and in-group by giving feedback about self or in-group on unfamiliar dimensions and found that participants were more willing to generalize from self to in-group than from in-group to self.
- Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.