120 results on '"Monks, Claire"'
Search Results
102. Peer victimization in multicultural schools in Spain and England
- Author
-
Monks, Claire P., primary, Ortega-Ruiz, Rosario, additional, and Rodríguez-Hidalgo, Antonio J., additional
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
103. Concepts of bullying: Developmental and cultural aspects
- Author
-
Smith, Peter Κ, primary and Monks, Claire Ρ, additional
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
104. Convivencia: The challenge of a cosmopolitan approach to the prevention of school violence
- Author
-
Ortega, Rosario, primary and Monks, Claire, additional
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
105. Bullying and social exclusion in multi-ethnic schools in England and Spain
- Author
-
Monks, Claire P., primary, Ortega, Rosario, additional, and Rodriguez, Antonio Jesus, additional
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
106. What friendships really look like these days.
- Author
-
Van Zalk, Nejra and Monks, Claire
- Subjects
- *
FRIENDSHIP , *ADOLESCENT friendships , *INTERNET friendship , *INTERNET addiction , *ONLINE social networks - Abstract
Last year, Elian Fink and Claire Hughes published an insightful commentary in these pages on the importance of children's friendships. The use of technology-mediated communication (TMC), which includes various modes of communication through texts, social networking technologies and online games, is now a large part of young people's lives practically everywhere. Having conducted research on the development of child and adolescent friendships, we were acutely aware that research into the impact on friendship development has lagged behind. [Extracted from the article]
- Published
- 2020
107. Cyberbullying among primary school pupils
- Author
-
Monks, Claire P., Ortega, Rosario, Robinson, Susanne, and Worlidge, Penny
- Subjects
BF - Abstract
The nature and extent of cyberbullying and traditional bullying were examined among a sample of primary school pupils aged 7 – 11 years in the London area. Pupils were given anonymous self-report questionnaires which asked about their use of mobile phones and the internet, and their experiences of bullying and cyberbullying as either the victims or perpetrators. Most pupils had a mobile phone and almost all had access to the internet at home. It was found that a significant number of pupils reported being involved in both types of bullying, although more children were involved in traditional bullying than cyberbullying. The most commonly reported types of cyberbullying were via instant messenger, email and texts. Children were also likely to consider cyberbullying as upsetting as traditional bullying. Implications for intervention and prevention work are discussed.
108. Cyberbullying among primary school aged pupils in England and Spain
- Author
-
Monks, Claire P., Ortega, Rosario, Robinson, Susanne, Alfaro, Monica, and Worlidge, Penny
- Subjects
BF
109. The group nature of peer-victimisation among young children: An observational study
- Author
-
Monks, Claire
- Subjects
education - Abstract
There is some suggestion that the nature of peer-victimisation differs among younger and older children in some important ways. First, it has been found that, although aggressive behaviour towards peers appears to be relatively stable over time, the role of the victim shows lower temporal stability according to peer and self reports (e.g. Ladd & Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2006), but that there appears to be some stability over time for teacher reports (e.g. Ostrov, 2008). Second, research has found that during middle childhood and adolescence bullying appears to be a group process (Salmivalli et al., 1996; Sutton et al., 1999), with some children taking supporting or reinforcing roles during incidents of peer-victimisation. However, peer-reports with younger children (4-6 years) have failed to identify the roles of Assistant (to the bully) or Reinforcer (who reinforces the bully’s activities) with any reliability (e.g. Monks et al., 2003; Monks & Smith, 2010). It has been suggested that these differences may reflect true differences in the nature of peer-victimisation in early childhood or may reflect limitations in young children’s cognitive capacity to identify victimisation and less impactful behaviours in their peers. To attempt to overcome the potential methodological issues of previous studies, the findings of an observational study of 58 children (girls, N=24, 41.38%) aged 4 and 5 years from 2 Reception Classes in London are presented. Observations were conducted of each child using time-sampling during free-play at school. Each child was observed for 50 minutes across the course of 5 weeks. The focal child’s behaviour during episodes of peer-victimisation was recorded, as well as the behaviour of any peers interacting with the focal child during this period. The study examined 1) whether young aggressive children aggress indiscriminately, 2) whether other children were present when there was an incident of peer-victimisation, and 3) to note what the behaviour of these ‘onlookers’ were (active reinforcement, passive reinforcement, defending or ignoring). The results presented indicate that peer victimisation among younger children appears to be less group-led and that there are some differences in peer-victimisation among younger children and bullying as it is reported among middle childhood and adolescence. The implications of this research for our understanding of the development of peer-victimisation and bullying among young children will be discussed.
110. Investigating conscious, psychophysiological, and behavioural measures of covert surveillance detection via nonconventional means
- Author
-
Friday, Ross, Monks, Claire, and Thompson, Trevor
- Subjects
BF Psychology - Abstract
Many people have turned to see someone behind them due to a 'sense' they were being watched. Others have 'inexplicably' felt as though they were the focus of others' attention, despite there being no conventional means via which this could be detected (Sheldrake, 2003). The most popular and enduring of the theoretical explanations for these events is that extrasensory awareness was evolutionarily advantageous, and therefore may have developed during an era in which danger was ever-present with survival depending on such capabilities (Sheldrake, 2005). The term 'extrasensory perception' is often abbreviated to ESP and was adopted by Rhine (1934), a Duke University psychologist who employed it to refer to the claimed reception of information gained via the mind, rather than the recognized physical senses. Such abilities include telepathy, intuition, psychometry, clairvoyance, clairaudience, and their associated trans- temporal operations as retrocognition or precognition. Such phenomena are also often referred to as a 'sixth sense' or 'second sight'. Evidence supporting the existence of extrasensory surveillance detection would have implications beyond purely scientific interest, yet the phenomena remains under-researched and may benefit from a fresh approach. The research conducted as part of the current thesis aimed to examine not only the possible existence of covert surveillance detection - but also which psychosocial and neurological factors may predict this ability. Research concerning an individual‟s ability to detect attention which they could not be aware of via conventional senses has previously been restricted to the psychic staring effect, also known as scopaesthesia - a phenomenon in which people respond via non-conventional means to being the subject of another person‟s gaze (Sheldrake 2003). However, this new investigation furthered the research by incorporating the previously uninvestigated sense of being listened to as well as seen. The existence of these abilities was gauged during a series of experiments and was measured via a) the accuracy of participants‟ self-reports of being watched or listened to, b) psychophysiological reactions determined by electrodermal activity (EDA) which measures the electrical conductance of the participant‟s skin to indicate a response, and c) differences in their behaviour during surveillance. Self-reports (Colwell et al., 2000; Peterson, 1978; Sheldrake, 2000), behavioural differences (Chen, 1937; Cottrell et al., 1968; Dashiell, 1935; Platt et al., 1967; Travis, 1925; Triplett, 1898), and EDA (Colwell et al,. 2000; Peterson, 1978; Sheldrake, 2000; Williams, 1983) have all provided significant evidence of extrasensory detection in previous research, however they had never been combined in a single study; doing so provided the opportunity to cross reference the results, and to directly compare these methodologies. This original and unique fusion of neuroscientific, parapsychological, forensic, anomalistic, and psychosocial factors represented an essential and progressive step in understanding possible covert surveillance detection, and its psychosocial and neurological predictors such as schizotypy and temporal lobe lability - and it produced findings both expected and surprising. Through a series of studies which were adapted and improved upon based on the results of the experiments which preceded them, the researcher was able to uncover not only which methods of covert surveillance detection were the most effective, but also the circumstances under which they were most sensitive. Analysis of the resulting data revealed individuals to be able to self-report surveillance they could not be aware of via conventional senses as the literature would suggest (Sheldrake, 2003), however the importance of addressing participant expectation and the reporting bias associated with this was made clear. A major finding of the research however was the discovery that peoples‟ behaviour could be significantly altered by covert surveillance, as results demonstrated that participants‟ decision-making ability was affected by whether they were being watched and/or listened to during a cognitive task. Perhaps the most surprising element of the collection of experiments though was that through the evolving methodology, it was revealed that the stress involved in being tested may be a necessary element for effective research to be conducted in this area. Indeed, by creating and adding stress to experiments in which it was previously absent, the researcher was able to capture positive results via participants‟ EDA even though this physiological measurement had been shown to be an ineffective measurement of surveillance detection when the participants were relaxed. When similar results were found following a field experiment based on the laboratory research, the researcher developed a theory that stress or threat is an essential element which should be included in future research related to this topic, as well as considered in real-world environments.
- Published
- 2019
111. Resource control and social dominance in early childhood : behavioural, cognitive and affective factors in the first year at school
- Author
-
Roberts, Alan, Monks, Claire, and Tsermentseli, Stella
- Subjects
LB1501 Primary Education - Abstract
Social dominance in early childhood has traditionally been viewed as a social status based on aggression. However, through the lens of resource control theory, social dominance is attained via acquisition of resources (e.g. toys, friendship), which can be achieved via any behavioural means - prosocial or coercive. This research used this theoretical approach as a basis to investigate the associations between resource control strategy use, resource control success and social dominance, along with associated cognitive and affective factors - theory of mind, empathy, emotion comprehension and contextual moral application. Ninety-two children aged between 4 and 5 years old were recruited, along with their class teachers from 3 schools in the south east of England. At three time points across their first year at school, the children were assessed using a battery of verbal, theory of mind, empathy, emotion comprehension and selective moral disengagement assessments, as well as being asked to respond to vignettes in which their avatar was required to acquire a resource from an opponent. At each timepoint, class teachers reported on the children's general behaviour, resource control strategy use, resource control success and social dominance. Results found children's resource control strategy use explained near half the variance in resource control success near the beginning of the school year. However, this reduced by the end of the school year, with affective empathy associated with more frequent coercive and prosocial strategy use. Resource control success predicted social dominance at all timepoints. Social dominance was also directly predicted by both prosocial and coercive strategy use at T1, but only coercive strategy use at T2 and T3. Regardless of social preference, ToM, empathy, emotion comprehension and selective moral disengagement, as well as overall changes in class prosocial and coercive resource control strategy over the year, both social dominance and resource control success at the start of the year predicted themselves at the end of the year. Whilst resource control success was found to be directly predictive of social dominance in the immediate-short term, it did not predict social dominance across the year, suggesting that teacher-ratings of social dominance may be influenced by the social prominence of the child when rating their social dominance. The lack of significant associations between resource control strategy and theory of mind suggest future measures should attempt to capture instances of theory of mind usage within real-world resource contest situations, via direct observation. Overall this work highlights the need for direct observational data to more accurately ascertain the interplay between cognition and strategy selection in early childhood, allowing greater elucidation of the establishment of social dominance in young children's peer groups.
- Published
- 2019
112. A neuroscientific and cognitive examination of individual differences in face recognition ability
- Author
-
Belanova, Elena, Davis, Josh P., Thompson, Trevor, and Monks, Claire
- Subjects
153.7 ,BF Psychology - Abstract
There are large individual differences in face processing ability, with Super-Recognisers (SRs) being exceptionally superior to individuals with average face recognition ability. This thesis describes seven experiments examining SRs' cognitive performance as well as neural/electrical activity in order to explore potential quantitative and qualitative contributions to their face processing superiority. Chapter 4 examined whether SRs rely on holistic and parts-based processing to the same extent as controls, and whether their face recognition superiority can be observed at the face perception stage as well. SRs outperformed controls at face recognition and face matching, inverted face recognition, object recognition and feature matching. SRs also demonstrated normal (Part-Whole Effect), greater (Inversion Effect), or reduced (Composite Face Effect) holistic processing, implying a more effective use of holistic and parts-based processing. Chapter 5 explored whether SRs' face processing superiority transcends to faces they have less experience with (infant faces), and whether this Other Age Effect could be observed on a neural/electrical level. SRs outperformed controls on adult and infant faces despite limited experience with the latter. Furthermore, EEG analysis indicated enhanced P1 (pictorial processing) and P600 (explicit recognition) in SRs during face recognition, suggesting they may benefit from a more effective pictorial processing of faces. Chapter 6 employed the Remember/Know paradigm with EEG recording to explore SRs' recollection and familiarity of faces and objects. SRs' recognition was often accompanied by contextual information, suggesting they remembered more than just the stimuli's identity. Furthermore, SRs' visual recognition was reflected in neural/electrical activity in central and right brain sites, while controls only demonstrated central site activation. Applicability of this thesis' findings, as well as the design's limitations and new potential directions for future research are discussed in the final chapter.
- Published
- 2017
113. Impacto de la actividad lúdica en el desarrollo de la competencia social.
- Author
-
Romero, Eva M., Ortega, Rosario, and Monks, Claire
- Subjects
- *
PLAY & psychology , *SOCIAL skills , *INTERPERSONAL relations , *AGGRESSION (Psychology) in children , *EDUCATION - Abstract
Impact of the play activity in the development of social competence. Among the bad relations that may arise in social interactions is unjustified aggression and harassment. The unjustified aggressiveness can be considered an antecedent of the bullying phenomenon, which could be prevented through appropriate educational programs. The objective of this study is to check if the intervention aimed at improving interpersonal relationships in play activities promote both competence for learning and social development as the quality of interpersonal relationships, under the assumption that both skills are essential for the prevention of unjustified aggression in preschool. The results seem to reflect some success in the quality of interpersonal relations that encourages and promotes this educational model. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2008
114. Cognitive predictors of aggression, prosocial behaviour and peer acceptance across early childhood : the role of cool and hot executive function and theory of mind
- Author
-
O'Toole, Sarah Elizabeth, Monks, Claire, and Tsermentseli, Stella
- Subjects
155.4 ,BF Psychology ,LB Theory and practice of education - Abstract
Executive function (EF) and theory of mind (ToM) have been linked to children's social outcomes, including aggression, prosocial behaviour and peer acceptance. However, understanding of these relations is limited by the focus of research on broad behaviour categories and on cool-cognitive EF skills, to the neglect of hot-affective EF, and the lack of longitudinal studies. This research examined the links between EF, ToM and social outcomes across early childhood. 106 children (initially 3- to 6-years-old) were assessed at three time points, approximately 6 months apart, across 12 months. At each time point children completed tasks assessing their cool EF (inhibition, working memory, planning), hot EF (affective decision making, delay of gratification), ToM (first- and second-order false belief understanding) and verbal ability. Teacher reports of children's proactive and reactive, physical and relational aggression, prosocial behaviour and peer acceptance were obtained at each time point and Peer reports were gathered at Time 1. EF, including cool and hot skills, and ToM were more strongly associated with physical, rather than relational, aggression. However, the role of individual EFs varied across subtypes of aggression, supporting a multi-dimensional model of aggression that differentiates between functions and forms. Although initial EF and ToM did not predict change in social outcomes across early childhood, the role of these cognitive skills changed across time points. EF and ToM were more strongly associated with social outcomes towards the beginning of early childhood than towards the end. This may reflect the significant advances in EF and ToM that were evident across early childhood. Cool EF skills were consistently correlated across time points, but hot EF skills were not related at any of the time points, suggesting that separable cool and hot domains of EF may not be apparent in early childhood. The present research emphasised the importance of examining the link between cognition and behaviour within the context of development.
- Published
- 2016
115. An exploration of four to seven year olds' perceptions of their own behaviour with comparisons to peers, class teachers and teaching assistants
- Author
-
Rix, Katie Rose, Maras, Pamela, and Monks, Claire
- Subjects
370.15 ,BF Psychology - Abstract
Children aged four to seven years, experience social and cognitive changes and may exhibit a variety of aggressive (Monks, Smith, & Swettenham, 2003), prosocial (Sebanc, 2003) and solitary behaviours (Coplan & Ooi, 2014), which are related to positive and negative outcomes. Research in this area has tended to adopt methods using reports from peers and adults, and hypothetical scenarios presented to children. This thesis is original in showing that young children can also offer unique insights into their own behaviour by providing self-reports and explanations. These insights vary across different forms of behaviour and over the course of a school year. This research also employed an original method. Stick figure animations were developed to collect behaviour reports from children on a three-point scale. Furthermore, a cohort sequential design with 273 participants was used to assess how children’s reported perceptions changed longitudinally over three time points, and cross-sectionally across two year groups. Reports of children’s behaviour were also collected from peers, Class Teachers, and Teaching Assistants. Children’s self-reports of solitary and prosocial behaviour tended to be higher than other reporters’. Self-reports of aggressive behaviour tended to be lower than other reporters. Agreement between self-reports and reports from others mostly increased over time. Children’s self-reports were categorised into one of five clusters at each time point: prosocial / social, solitary, low behavioural levels, excluders, and antisocial / solitary. There were no consistent patterns in cluster and age group, time point, or sex. Children’s behaviour explanations were mostly focused on causes, rather than consequences, Agency was mostly external. Outcome focus was more varied across forms of behaviour. Explanations became more consequential over time and there was some variation in explanations by children’s self-reported ratings. Findings are discussed in relation to literature and research, developmental theories, implications for future research, and work in schools.
- Published
- 2015
116. The development and validation of a teacher-reported low-level classroom disruption scale (LLCD-S).
- Author
-
Cogswell, Suzanne, Carr, Amanda, Abbott, Nicola, and Monks, Claire P.
- Subjects
- *
STATISTICAL correlation , *EXPERIMENTAL design , *FACTOR analysis , *RESEARCH methodology , *QUESTIONNAIRES , *SCHOOL environment , *BEHAVIOR disorders , *RESEARCH methodology evaluation , *COLLEGE teacher attitudes , *DESCRIPTIVE statistics ,RESEARCH evaluation - Abstract
Low-level classroom disruption (LLCD) is characterised by pupils swinging on chairs, whispering or fidgeting in class. This paper provides initial data on the development and validation of the teacher-rated Low-Level Classroom Disruption Scale (LLCD-S), with two samples of primary school pupils. Exploratory factor analysis in Study 1 (N= 120) revealed one factor accounting for 61% of the variance; supported by confirmatory factor analysis in Study 2 (N= 274), with one factor accounting for 63% of the variance. Both studies reported high Cronbach's alpha values of.82 and.93 respectively. The evidence supports LLCD being a unidimensional construct, measured by the eight item LLCD-S. Weak convergence validity was found between the LLCD-S and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire's (SDQ) externalising behaviours: conduct problems and hyperactivity. This preliminary evidence indicates that LLCD-S is a valid and reliable measure of low-level classroom disruption. Further research is needed to test the utility of the LLCD-S across different levels of education, cultures and as a pupil-reported measure. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
117. What makes for effectiveness when starting early – Learning from an integrated school-based violence and abuse prevention programme for children under 12.
- Author
-
Stanley, Nicky, Devaney, John, Kurdi, Zain, Ozdemir, Ugur, Barter, Christine, Monks, Claire, Edwards, Rhiannon Tudor, Batool, Farwa, Charles, Joanna, Farrelly, Nicola, Hayes, David, Millar, Annemarie, Thompson, Trevor, Winrow, Eira, and Radford, Lorraine
- Subjects
- *
VIOLENCE prevention , *CHILD sexual abuse , *READINESS for school , *SCHOOL environment , *GIRLS , *ADULT child abuse victims , *SEX crimes , *HELP-seeking behavior - Abstract
Integrated programmes addressing varying forms of violence and abuse are increasingly delivered to children under 12 but uncertainty remains about what should be delivered to whom, when and in what dose. To examine the impact of Speak Out Stay Safe (SOSS) - an integrated prevention programme for children under 12 - and whether impact varied by age, gender and context. A representative UK sample of primary schools in receipt of SOSS was matched with comparison schools not receiving SOSS. At 6 months follow-up, 1553 children from 36 schools completed the survey. The matched control study incorporated economic and process evaluations. Survey measures included: children's knowledge and understanding of different forms of violence and abuse, readiness to seek help, knowledge of sexual abuse, perceptions of school culture and health and wellbeing. Perceptions of children, teachers, and facilitators were captured. At 6 months, children aged 9–10 who received SOSS retained their improved knowledge of neglect and their ability to identify a trusted adult who they would tell about violence or abuse. Children aged 6–7 receiving a shorter version of the programme were less likely to benefit and boys made fewer gains than girls. SOSS improved the knowledge of children with low knowledge of abuse. School culture was closely associated with programme impact. School-based prevention programmes deliver benefits at low cost but should acknowledge and engage with the specific school context to achieve school readiness and embed programme messages. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
118. Bullying in Different Contexts
- Author
-
Monks, Claire P., editor and Coyne, Iain, editor
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
119. Violencia escolar en sociedades pluriculturales: bullying y victimización entre escolares de carácter étnico-cultural
- Author
-
Rodríguez Hidalgo, Antonio Jesús, Monks, Claire P., and Ortega Ruiz, Rosario
- Subjects
Violencia escolar ,Xenofobia ,Bullying ,Racismo - Abstract
Las investigaciones europeas sobre violencia escolar y bullying apuntan a que no existen diferencias en la prevalencia de la victimización entre escolares en función del grupo étnico-cultural. Algunos estudios han contemplado dentro de la victimización étnico-cultural dos subtipos: el insulto o nominación racista; y la exclusión social racista o xenófoba. Este trabajo focaliza en el estudio paralelo de tipos de victimización personal (física, verbal, relacional directa y relacional indirecta) y tipos de victimización étnico-cultural (verbal, relacional directa y relacional indirecta), analizando algunos factores asociados y controlando la variable étnico-cultural. Una muestra trasnacional de 1.185 escolares (España, n¹=863; Inglaterra, n² = 322) étnico-culturalmente diversos de Educación Primaria y Secundaria, participaron cumplimentando una batería auto-informe. Esta batería se compuso de varios cuestionarios sobre victimización personal y étnico-cultural, autoestima personal, autoestima cultural y sociometría de las relaciones entre compañeros/as. Los resultados muestran como la victimización personal afecta de igual modo a escolares con independencia de su grupo cultural, mientras que la victimización étnico-cultural afecta significativamente más al alumnado de minorías culturales que al de la mayoría cultural. En la muestra española los más victimizados culturalmente fueron las y los escolares de origen inmigrante y en segundo lugar las y los escolares gitanos, mientras que las y los escolares de la mayoría cultural apenas se mostraban afectados. El alumnado de minorías culturales era más sensible a la perniciosidad de algunos tipos de victimización étnico-cultural que sus compañeros/as del grupo mayoritario. Sobre la muestra trasnacional se ha observado que: la autoestima personal baja predice la victimización personal, pero no la victimización cultural; y que la autoestima cultural baja predice la victimización cultural, pero no la victimización personal. Las conclusiones y discusión se orientan hacia la propuesta de un modelo teórico de la relación entre victimización personal y victimización étnico-cultural, así como al planteamiento de algunas iniciativas educativas preventivas y paliativas.
- Published
- 2010
120. Prevalence of violence against children in the United Kingdom: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
- Author
-
Nation A, Pacella R, Monks C, Mathews B, and Meinck F
- Subjects
- Humans, Child, Prevalence, United Kingdom epidemiology, Domestic Violence psychology, Child Abuse psychology, Intimate Partner Violence psychology
- Abstract
Background: Violence against children can have extensive, long-term, and far-reaching adverse impacts on survivors and society. There is currently little consensus in the United Kingdom around the prevalence of violence against children: maltreatment, intimate partner violence, sexual violence, bullying, and community violence, and most existing studies focus on only a single or a few forms of violence. This study aims to produce data to highlight the current magnitude of the problem in the UK, to inform policy, drive action and allow for monitoring of progress over time., Objective: To produce weighted prevalence estimates by violence type, as well as gender and age sub-categories, to give as full a picture as possible of the current prevalence of violence against children in the UK., Participants and Setting: The prevalence of violence against children from 23 self-report studies conducted in the United Kingdom was gathered through a systematic review., Methods: Databases were searched from inception to 24th June 2022. Studies were reviewed systematically for appropriate data and meta-analyses were conducted to give pooled prevalence data based on a quality effects model., Results: The most prevalent self-reported experience of childhood violence was community violence at 27.33 % (95 % CI [9.84, 48.97]). Prevalence of bullying was also high at 22.75 % (95 % CI [13.25, 33.86]). The most prevalent forms of child maltreatment were domestic violence exposure at 11.9 % (95 % CI [6.34, 18.84]) and emotional maltreatment at 11.84 % (95 % CI [5.58, 19.89])., Conclusion: National child maltreatment surveys are needed in the UK, using a comprehensive and conceptually robust approach, and valid and reliable instruments, to provide data for researchers and policymakers on the prevalence of all types of violence against children including exposure to multiple types. This allows monitoring of trends over time, can inform strategies for prevention, and can enable monitoring of future progress in reducing violence against children and its associated health and economic burden., (Copyright © 2023 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.