243 results on '"Hendriks, Petra"'
Search Results
202. Het gat (aan)gevuld
- Author
-
Hendriks, Petra and de Vries, Marjan
- Published
- 1989
203. Development of voice perception is dissociated across gender cues in school-age children
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
631/378/2649/1594 ,4. Education ,article ,631/378/2649/1723 ,10. No inequality - Abstract
Funder: Center for Language and Cognition Groningen (CLCG), Funder: LabEx CeLyA (“Centre Lyonnais d’Acoustique”, ANR-10-LABX-0060/ANR-11-IDEX-0007), Children’s ability to distinguish speakers’ voices continues to develop throughout childhood, yet it remains unclear how children’s sensitivity to voice cues, such as differences in speakers’ gender, develops over time. This so-called voice gender is primarily characterized by speakers’ mean fundamental frequency (F0), related to glottal pulse rate, and vocal-tract length (VTL), related to speakers’ size. Here we show that children’s acquisition of adult-like performance for discrimination, a lower-order perceptual task, and categorization, a higher-order cognitive task, differs across voice gender cues. Children’s discrimination was adult-like around the age of 8 for VTL but still differed from adults at the age of 12 for F0. Children’s perceptual weight attributed to F0 for gender categorization was adult-like around the age of 6 but around the age of 10 for VTL. Children’s discrimination and weighting of F0 and VTL were only correlated for 4- to 6-year-olds. Hence, children’s development of discrimination and weighting of voice gender cues are dissociated, i.e., adult-like performance for F0 and VTL is acquired at different rates and does not seem to be closely related. The different developmental patterns for auditory discrimination and categorization highlight the complexity of the relationship between perceptual and cognitive mechanisms of voice perception.
204. Development of voice perception is dissociated across gender cues in school-age children
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
Male ,Schools ,4. Education ,Gender Identity ,Psychology, Child ,Child Development ,Cognition ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Child, Preschool ,Auditory Perception ,Voice ,Humans ,Cues ,10. No inequality ,Child - Abstract
Children's ability to distinguish speakers' voices continues to develop throughout childhood, yet it remains unclear how children's sensitivity to voice cues, such as differences in speakers' gender, develops over time. This so-called voice gender is primarily characterized by speakers' mean fundamental frequency (F0), related to glottal pulse rate, and vocal-tract length (VTL), related to speakers' size. Here we show that children's acquisition of adult-like performance for discrimination, a lower-order perceptual task, and categorization, a higher-order cognitive task, differs across voice gender cues. Children's discrimination was adult-like around the age of 8 for VTL but still differed from adults at the age of 12 for F0. Children's perceptual weight attributed to F0 for gender categorization was adult-like around the age of 6 but around the age of 10 for VTL. Children's discrimination and weighting of F0 and VTL were only correlated for 4- to 6-year-olds. Hence, children's development of discrimination and weighting of voice gender cues are dissociated, i.e., adult-like performance for F0 and VTL is acquired at different rates and does not seem to be closely related. The different developmental patterns for auditory discrimination and categorization highlight the complexity of the relationship between perceptual and cognitive mechanisms of voice perception.
205. Development of vocal emotion recognition in school-age children: The EmoHI test for hearing-impaired populations
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Matos Lopes, Marta, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
Emotion ,4. Education ,Cochlear implants ,Cognitive development ,Auditory development ,Perception ,Hearing loss ,Vocal emotion recognition ,Hearing impairment - Abstract
Traditionally, emotion recognition research has primarily used pictures and videos, while audio test materials are not always readily available or are not of good quality, which may be particularly important for studies with hearing-impaired listeners. Here we present a vocal emotion recognition test with pseudospeech productions from multiple speakers expressing three core emotions (happy, angry, and sad): the EmoHI test. The high sound quality recordings make the test suitable for use with populations of children and adults with normal or impaired hearing. Here we present normative data for vocal emotion recognition development in normal-hearing (NH) school-age children using the EmoHI test. Furthermore, we investigated cross-language effects by testing NH Dutch and English children, and the suitability of the EmoHI test for hearing-impaired populations, specifically for prelingually deaf Dutch children with cochlear implants (CIs). Our results show that NH children's performance improved significantly with age from the youngest age group onwards (4-6 years: 48.9%, on average). However, NH children's performance did not reach adult-like values (adults: 94.1%) even for the oldest age group tested (10-12 years: 81.1%). Additionally, the effect of age on NH children's development did not differ across languages. All except one CI child performed at or above chance-level showing the suitability of the EmoHI test. In addition, seven out of 14 CI children performed within the NH age-appropriate range, and nine out of 14 CI children did so when performance was adjusted for hearing age, measured from their age at CI implantation. However, CI children showed great variability in their performance, ranging from ceiling (97.2%) to below chance-level performance (27.8%), which could not be explained by chronological age alone. The strong and consistent development in performance with age, the lack of significant differences across the tested languages for NH children, and the above-chance performance of most CI children affirm the usability and versatility of the EmoHI test.
206. Development of voice perception is dissociated across gender cues in school-age children
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
4. Education ,10. No inequality - Abstract
Children's ability to distinguish speakers' voices continues to develop throughout childhood, yet it remains unclear how children's sensitivity to voice cues, such as differences in speakers' gender, develops over time. This so-called voice gender is primarily characterized by speakers' mean fundamental frequency (F0), related to glottal pulse rate, and vocal-tract length (VTL), related to speakers' size. Here we show that children's acquisition of adult-like performance for discrimination, a lower-order perceptual task, and categorization, a higher-order cognitive task, differs across voice gender cues. Children's discrimination was adult-like around the age of 8 for VTL but still differed from adults at the age of 12 for F0. Children's perceptual weight attributed to F0 for gender categorization was adult-like around the age of 6 but around the age of 10 for VTL. Children's discrimination and weighting of F0 and VTL were only correlated for 4- to 6-year-olds. Hence, children's development of discrimination and weighting of voice gender cues are dissociated, i.e., adult-like performance for F0 and VTL is acquired at different rates and does not seem to be closely related. The different developmental patterns for auditory discrimination and categorization highlight the complexity of the relationship between perceptual and cognitive mechanisms of voice perception.
207. Development of vocal emotion recognition in school-age children: The EmoHI test for hearing-impaired populations
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Matos Lopes, Marta, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
4. Education - Abstract
Traditionally, emotion recognition research has primarily used pictures and videos, while audio test materials are not always readily available or are not of good quality, which may be particularly important for studies with hearing-impaired listeners. Here we present a vocal emotion recognition test with pseudospeech productions from multiple speakers expressing three core emotions (happy, angry, and sad): the EmoHI test. The high sound quality recordings make the test suitable for use with populations of children and adults with normal or impaired hearing. Here we present normative data for vocal emotion recognition development in normal-hearing (NH) school-age children using the EmoHI test. Furthermore, we investigated cross-language effects by testing NH Dutch and English children, and the suitability of the EmoHI test for hearing-impaired populations, specifically for prelingually deaf Dutch children with cochlear implants (CIs). Our results show that NH children's performance improved significantly with age from the youngest age group onwards (4-6 years: 48.9%, on average). However, NH children's performance did not reach adult-like values (adults: 94.1%) even for the oldest age group tested (10-12 years: 81.1%). Additionally, the effect of age on NH children's development did not differ across languages. All except one CI child performed at or above chance-level showing the suitability of the EmoHI test. In addition, seven out of 14 CI children performed within the NH age-appropriate range, and nine out of 14 CI children did so when performance was adjusted for hearing age, measured from their age at CI implantation. However, CI children showed great variability in their performance, ranging from ceiling (97.2%) to below chance-level performance (27.8%), which could not be explained by chronological age alone. The strong and consistent development in performance with age, the lack of significant differences across the tested languages for NH children, and the above-chance performance of most CI children affirm the usability and versatility of the EmoHI test.
208. A new hypothesis on compositionality
- Author
-
Blutner, R., Hendriks, Petra, de Hoop, Helen, Slezak, P.P., ILLC (FNWI/FGw), and Slezak, P.
- Subjects
Structure and Variation ,Structuur en variatie ,Case Cross-linguistically - Abstract
Contains fulltext : 63739.pdf (author's version ) (Open Access) [Sydney] Joint International Conference on Cognitive Science, 16 juli 2003 Sydney : [s.n.]
209. Partial word order freezing in Dutch
- Author
-
Bouma, Gerlof J. and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
16. Peace & justice - Abstract
Dutch allows for variation as to whether the first position in the sentence is occupied by the subject or by some other constituent, such as the direct object. In particular situations, however, this commonly observed variation in word order is ‘frozen’ and only the subject appears in first position. We hypothesize that this partial freezing of word order in Dutch can be explained from the dependence of the speaker’s choice of word order on the hearer’s interpretation of this word order. A formal model of this interaction between the speaker’s perspective and the hearer’s perspective is presented in terms of bidirectional Optimality Theory. Empirical predictions of this model regarding the interaction between word order and definiteness are confirmed by a quantitative corpus study., Postprints der Universität Potsdam : Humanwissenschaftliche Reihe, 625
210. The discourse structure of free indirect discourse reports.
- Author
-
Bimpikou, Sofia, Maier, Emar, and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
- *
DISCOURSE , *DISCOURSE analysis , *PRONOUNS (Grammar) , *FRAMES (Social sciences) - Abstract
We investigate the discourse structure of Free Indirect Discourse passages in narratives. We argue that Free Indirect Discourse reports consist of two separate propositional discourse units: an (explicit or implicit) frame segment and a reported content. These segments are connected at the level of discourse structure by a non-veridical, subordinating discourse relation of Attribution, familiar from recent SDRT analyses of indirect discourse constructions in natural conversation (Hunter, 2016). We conducted an experiment to detect the covert presence of a subordinating frame segment based on its effects on pronoun resolution. We compared (unframed) Free Indirect Discourse with overtly framed Indirect Discourse and a non-reportative segment. We found that the first two indeed pattern alike in terms of pronoun resolution, which we take as evidence against the pragmatic context split approach of Schlenker (2004) and Eckardt (2014), and in favor of our discourse structural Attribution analysis. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
211. Order Matters! Influences of Linear Order on Linguistic Category Learning.
- Author
-
Hoppe, Dorothée B., Rij, Jacolien, Hendriks, Petra, and Ramscar, Michael
- Subjects
- *
LINEAR orderings , *GRAMMATICAL categories , *FOREIGN language education , *ARTIFICIAL languages , *NATURAL languages , *FACTOR structure - Abstract
Linguistic category learning has been shown to be highly sensitive to linear order, and depending on the task, differentially sensitive to the information provided by preceding category markers (premarkers, e.g., gendered articles) or succeeding category markers (postmarkers, e.g., gendered suffixes). Given that numerous systems for marking grammatical categories exist in natural languages, it follows that a better understanding of these findings can shed light on the factors underlying this diversity. In two discriminative learning simulations and an artificial language learning experiment, we identify two factors that modulate linear order effects in linguistic category learning: category structure and the level of abstraction in a category hierarchy. Regarding category structure, we find that postmarking brings an advantage for learning category diagnostic stimulus dimensions, an effect not present when categories are non‐confusable. Regarding levels of abstraction, we find that premarking of super‐ordinate categories (e.g., noun class) facilitates learning of subordinate categories (e.g., nouns). We present detailed simulations using a plausible candidate mechanism for the observed effects, along with a comprehensive analysis of linear order effects within an expectation‐based account of learning. Our findings indicate that linguistic category learning is differentially guided by pre‐ and postmarking, and that the influence of each is modulated by the specific characteristics of a given category system. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
212. Event-Related Potentials Reveal Increased Dependency on Linguistic Context Due to Cognitive Aging.
- Author
-
la Roi, Amélie, Sprenger, Simone A., and Hendriks, Petra
- Abstract
Whereas executive functions are known to be closely tied to successful language processing in children and younger adults, less is known about how age-related decline in these functions affects language processing in elderly adults. Because the abilities to use linguistic context and resolve potential ambiguities such as between an idiom's figurative and literal meaning depend on executive functions, we investigated this issue by examining elderly adults' processing of idioms in context. We recorded event-related potentials of 25 younger (age 18-28) and 25 elderly adults (age 61-74) while they read literal sentences and sentences containing an idiom (e.g., the Dutch idiom to walk against the lamp, meaning "to get caught"), each preceded by a neutral or predictive context sentence. Participants' use of context was hypothesized to relate to working memory capacity, while their ability to disambiguate idioms was hypothesized to depend on inhibition skills. Both groups showed facilitated processing for idioms compared with literal sentences and for sentences preceded by predictive compared with neutral contexts, indexed by a reduced N400. However, only elderly adults showed an increased P600 for literal but not idiomatic sentences preceded by a predictive context, suggesting that they rely on linguistic context when a sentence's meaning needs to be computed word by word, but not when a large part is retrieved from memory (as in idioms). Our findings suggest that in both younger and elderly adults processing literal sentences requires more cognitive effort than processing idiomatic sentences, and that cognitive aging affects language when processing is effortful. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
213. Which Questions Do Children With Cochlear Implants Understand? An Eye-Tracking Study.
- Author
-
Schouwenaars, Atty, Finke, Mareike, Hendriks, Petra, and Ruigendijk, Esther
- Subjects
- *
COGNITIVE processing of language , *LANGUAGE acquisition , *MORPHOSYNTAX , *EYE tracking , *COCHLEAR implants , *AUDITORY perception , *PHONOLOGICAL awareness - Abstract
Purpose: The purpose of this study was to investigate the processing of morphosyntactic cues (case and verb agreement) by children with cochlear implants (CIs) in German which-questions, where interpretation depends on these morphosyntactic cues. The aim was to examine whether children with CIs who perceive the different cues also make use of them in speech comprehension and processing in the same way as children with normal hearing (NH). Method: Thirty-three children with CIs (age 7;01-12;04 years; months, M = 9;07, bilaterally implanted before age 3;3) and 36 children with NH (age 7;05-10;09 years, M = 9;01) received a picture selection task with eye tracking to test their comprehension of subject, object, and passive whichquestions. Two screening tasks tested their auditory discrimination of case morphology and their perception and comprehension of subject-verb agreement. Results: Children with CIs who performed well on the screening tests still showed more difficulty on the comprehension of object questions than children with NH, whereas they comprehended subject questions and passive questions equally well as children with NH. There was large interindividual variability within the CI group. The gaze patterns of children with NH showed reanalysis effects for object questions disambiguated later in the sentence by verb agreement, but not for object questions disambiguated by case at the first noun phrase. The gaze patterns of children with CIs showed reanalysis effects even for object questions disambiguated at the first noun phrase. Conclusions: Even when children with CIs perceive case and subject-verb agreement, their ability to use these cues for offline comprehension and online processing still lags behind normal development, which is reflected in lower performance rates and longer processing times. Individual variability within the CI group can partly be explained by working memory and hearing age. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
214. Children with autism spectrum disorder show pronoun reversals in interpretation.
- Author
-
Overweg, Jessica, Hartman, Catharina A., and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
- *
AUTISM spectrum disorders , *COGNITION , *PHILOSOPHY of mind , *BEHAVIOR , *READABILITY (Literary style) , *SHORT-term memory , *SPEECH , *THOUGHT & thinking - Abstract
Pronoun reversals, saying you when meaning I, in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are generally viewed as manifesting in early development and speech production only. This study investigates pronoun reversals in later development (age 6-12) in interpretation in 48 Dutch-speaking children with ASD and 43 typically developing (TD) peers. We contrasted children's interpretation of I and you in indirect and direct speech reports, with the latter type requiring an additional perspective shift. To examine which cognitive processes are involved in pronoun interpretation, additional tasks were administered to measure Theory of Mind (ToM) understanding, cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. We found that children with ASD showed more problems than TD children interpreting pronouns in direct speech, resulting in pronoun reversals in interpretation. Children with ASD hardly improved with age. Older children with ASD thus showed more pronoun reversals than did their TD peers. ToM understanding, working memory, IQ, and verbal ability, but not inhibition and flexibility, were associated with pronoun interpretation. ToM understanding in particular was associated with correct pronoun interpretation in older TD children relative to younger TD children, but this improvement was not found in children with ASD. These findings indicate that pronoun reversals most likely result from perspective-shifting difficulties. We conclude that pronoun reversals are more pronounced in individuals with ASD, occur beyond early development, and require sufficient cognitive resources. The relation with ToM understanding, but not inhibition and flexibility, suggests that pronoun reversals are best classified as a social communication problem in the diagnosis of ASD. (PsycINFO Database Record [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
215. Language in autism: domains, profiles and co-occurring conditions.
- Author
-
Schaeffer, Jeannette, Abd El-Raziq, Muna, Castroviejo, Elena, Durrleman, Stephanie, Ferré, Sandrine, Grama, Ileana, Hendriks, Petra, Kissine, Mikhail, Manenti, Marta, Marinis, Theodoros, Meir, Natalia, Novogrodsky, Rama, Perovic, Alexandra, Panzeri, Francesca, Silleresi, Silvia, Sukenik, Nufar, Vicente, Agustín, Zebib, Racha, Prévost, Philippe, and Tuller, Laurice
- Subjects
- *
AUTISM , *PRAGMATICS , *LANGUAGE ability , *COGNITIVE ability , *LANGUAGE & languages - Abstract
This article reviews the current knowledge state on pragmatic and structural language abilities in autism and their potential relation to extralinguistic abilities and autistic traits. The focus is on questions regarding autism language profiles with varying degrees of (selective) impairment and with respect to potential comorbidity of autism and language impairment: Is language impairment in autism the co-occurrence of two distinct conditions (comorbidity), a consequence of autism itself (no comorbidity), or one possible combination from a series of neurodevelopmental properties (dimensional approach)? As for language profiles in autism, three main groups are identified, namely, (i) verbal autistic individuals without structural language impairment, (ii) verbal autistic individuals with structural language impairment, and (iii) minimally verbal autistic individuals. However, this tripartite distinction hides enormous linguistic heterogeneity. Regarding the nature of language impairment in autism, there is currently no model of how language difficulties may interact with autism characteristics and with various extralinguistic cognitive abilities. Building such a model requires carefully designed explorations that address specific aspects of language and extralinguistic cognition. This should lead to a fundamental increase in our understanding of language impairment in autism, thereby paving the way for a substantial contribution to the question of how to best characterize neurodevelopmental disorders. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
216. Pragmatics is not a monolithic phenomenon, and neither is theory of mind: Response to Kissine.
- Author
-
MOGNON, IRENE, HUKKER, VERA, SCHOLTEN, IRIS, and HENDRIKS, PETRA
- Subjects
- *
PRAGMATICS , *GENERAL semantics , *LANGUAGE acquisition , *PSYCHOLINGUISTICS , *COGNITION - Abstract
In this commentary, we emphasize the importance of the observations presented by Kissine (2021) in his target article for our understanding of the nonmonolithic nature of pragmatics. Our first aim is to complement Kissine’s argument, discussing some critical cases of linguistic processes that demonstrate the need for a finer-grained characterization of pragmatic phenomena. In addition, we report some findings that suggest that perspective taking may emerge as atypical even in autistic individuals who appear to be able to pass the standard theory-of-mind tasks. Our second aim is thus to argue that, albeit difficult to spot in experimental settings, the atypical theory-of-mind profile of low- and high-functioning autistic individuals is mirrored in their difficulties in everyday sociocommunicative interactions. Moreover, we claim that subtle differences in perspective-taking abilities may explain the highly heterogeneous linguistic profile of autistic individuals. Ultimately, with this commentary we wish to highlight the need for an increased appreciation of the role of perspective taking in typical and atypical language acquisition. This is crucial to our understanding of the nature of language acquisition, and can shed more light on the interaction between language and other aspects of human cognition. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
217. School-age children benefit from voice gender cue differences for the perception of speech in competing speech.
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
- *
GENDER differences (Psychology) , *SPEECH perception , *GENDER , *ADULTS - Abstract
Differences in speakers' voice characteristics, such as mean fundamental frequency (F0) and vocal-tract length (VTL), that primarily define speakers' so-called perceived voice gender facilitate the perception of speech in competing speech. Perceiving speech in competing speech is particularly challenging for children, which may relate to their lower sensitivity to differences in voice characteristics than adults. This study investigated the development of the benefit from F0 and VTL differences in school-age children (4–12 years) for separating two competing speakers while tasked with comprehending one of them and also the relationship between this benefit and their corresponding voice discrimination thresholds. Children benefited from differences in F0, VTL, or both cues at all ages tested. This benefit proportionally remained the same across age, although overall accuracy continued to differ from that of adults. Additionally, children's benefit from F0 and VTL differences and their overall accuracy were not related to their discrimination thresholds. Hence, although children's voice discrimination thresholds and speech in competing speech perception abilities develop throughout the school-age years, children already show a benefit from voice gender cue differences early on. Factors other than children's discrimination thresholds seem to relate more closely to their developing speech in competing speech perception abilities. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
218. How Children Process Reduced Forms: A Computational Cognitive Modeling Approach to Pronoun Processing in Discourse.
- Author
-
Vogelzang, Margreet, Guasti, Maria Teresa, Rijn, Hedderik, and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
- *
PRONOUNS (Grammar) , *PRAGMATICS , *COGNITIVE development , *SHORT-term memory , *DIVISION of labor , *HUMAN behavior models - Abstract
Reduced forms such as the pronoun he provide little information about their intended meaning compared to more elaborate descriptions such as the lead singer of Coldplay. Listeners must therefore use contextual information to recover their meaning. Across languages, there appears to be a trade‐off between the informativity of a form and the prominence of its referent. For example, Italian adults generally interpret informationally empty null pronouns as in the sentence Corre (meaning "He/She/It runs") as referring to the most prominent referent in the discourse, and more informative overt pronouns (e.g., lui in Lui corre, "He runs") as referring to less prominent referents. Although children acquiring Italian are known to experience difficulties interpreting pronouns, it is unclear how they acquire this division of pragmatic labor between null and overt subject pronouns, and how this relates to the development of their cognitive capacities. Here we show that cognitive development can account for the general interpretation patterns displayed by Italian‐speaking children and adults. Using experimental studies and computational simulations in a framework modeling bounded‐rational behavior, we argue that null pronoun interpretation is influenced by working memory capacity and thus appears to depend on discourse context, whereas overt pronoun interpretation is influenced by processing speed, suggesting that listeners must reason about the speaker's choices. Our results demonstrate that cognitive capacities may constrain the acquisition of linguistic forms and their meanings in various ways. The novel predictions generated by the computational simulations point out several directions for future research. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
219. Development of voice perception is dissociated across gender cues in school-age children.
- Author
-
Nagels, Leanne, Gaudrain, Etienne, Vickers, Deborah, Hendriks, Petra, and Başkent, Deniz
- Subjects
- *
SCHOOL children , *GENDER , *DISCRIMINATION (Sociology) , *CHILD development , *CATEGORIZATION (Psychology) - Abstract
Children's ability to distinguish speakers' voices continues to develop throughout childhood, yet it remains unclear how children's sensitivity to voice cues, such as differences in speakers' gender, develops over time. This so-called voice gender is primarily characterized by speakers' mean fundamental frequency (F0), related to glottal pulse rate, and vocal-tract length (VTL), related to speakers' size. Here we show that children's acquisition of adult-like performance for discrimination, a lower-order perceptual task, and categorization, a higher-order cognitive task, differs across voice gender cues. Children's discrimination was adult-like around the age of 8 for VTL but still differed from adults at the age of 12 for F0. Children's perceptual weight attributed to F0 for gender categorization was adult-like around the age of 6 but around the age of 10 for VTL. Children's discrimination and weighting of F0 and VTL were only correlated for 4- to 6-year-olds. Hence, children's development of discrimination and weighting of voice gender cues are dissociated, i.e., adult-like performance for F0 and VTL is acquired at different rates and does not seem to be closely related. The different developmental patterns for auditory discrimination and categorization highlight the complexity of the relationship between perceptual and cognitive mechanisms of voice perception. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
220. Reasoning about alternative forms is costly: The processing of null and overt pronouns in Italian using pupillary responses.
- Author
-
Vogelzang, Margreet, Foppolo, Francesca, Guasti, Maria Teresa, van Rijn, Hedderik, and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
- *
VOCABULARY , *DISCOURSE - Abstract
Different words generally have different meanings. However, some words seemingly share similar meanings. An example are null and overt pronouns in Italian, which both refer to an individual in the discourse. Is the interpretation and processing of a form affected by the existence of another form with a similar meaning? With a pupillary response study, we show that null and overt pronouns are processed differently. Specifically, null pronouns are found to be less costly to process than overt pronouns. We argue that this difference is caused by an additional reasoning step that is needed to process marked overt pronouns but not unmarked null pronouns. A comparison with data from Dutch, a language with overt but no null pronouns, demonstrates that Italian pronouns are processed differently from Dutch pronouns. These findings suggest that the processing of a marked form is influenced by alternative forms within the same language, making its processing costly. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
221. Narrative production in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Similarities and differences.
- Author
-
Kuijper, Sanne J. M., Hartman, Catharina A., Bogaerds-Hazenberg, Suzanne T. M., and Hendriks, Petra
- Subjects
- *
ATTENTION-deficit hyperactivity disorder , *BEHAVIOR , *LANGUAGE disorders , *SHORT-term memory , *THOUGHT & thinking , *NARRATIVES , *DISEASE complications - Abstract
The present study focuses on the similarities and differences in language production between children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In addition, we investigated whether Theory of Mind (ToM), working memory, and response inhibition are associated with language production. Narratives, produced by 106 Dutch-speaking children (36 with ASD, 34 with ADHD, and 36 typically developing) aged 6 to 12 during ADOS assessment, were examined on several linguistic measures: verbal productivity, speech fluency, syntactic complexity, lexical semantics, and discourse pragmatics. Children were tested on ToM, working memory, and response inhibition and parents filled in the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC-2). Gold-standard diagnostic measures (Autism Diagnostic Observation Schema [ADOS], Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised [ADI-R], and the Parent Interview for Child Symptoms [PICS]) were administered to all children to confirm diagnosis. Regarding similarities, both clinical groups showed impairments in narrative performance relative to typically developing children. These were confirmed by the CCC-2. These impairments were not only present on pragmatic measures, such as the inability to produce a narrative in a coherent and cohesive way, but also on syntactic complexity and their production of repetitions. As for differences, children with ADHD but not children with ASD showed problems in their choice of referring expressions and speech fluency. ToM and working memory performance but not response inhibition were associated with many narrative skills, suggesting that these cognitive mechanisms explain some of the impairments in language production. We conclude that children with ASD and children with ADHD manifest multiple and diverse language production problems, which may partly relate to their problems in ToM and working memory. (PsycINFO Database Record [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
222. From Voice to Speech
- Author
-
Leanne Nagels, Hendriks, Petra, and Baskent, Deniz
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,medicine ,otorhinolaryngologic diseases ,Audiology ,Psychology ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,psychological phenomena and processes ,media_common - Abstract
Cochlear implants (CIs) are auditory prostheses for deaf individuals. The CI speech signal is degraded compared to normal acoustic hearing. This causes difficulties with the perception of speech and voice characteristics, such as emotions, pitch, or vocal-tract length, related to speakers’ height. Speakers’ mean pitch and vocal-tract length together lead to perceived differences between male and female voices. Voice characteristics help listeners to focus on one speaker to perceive speech while multiple people talk at the same time. The consequences of the degraded CI speech signal on the development of voice and speech perception in CI children and how this development differs from normal-hearing children are unclear. This dissertation investigated how the perception of differences in speakers’ pitch, vocal-tract length, and emotions and the perception of speech in background speech develops in normal-hearing and CI children.My research shows that there is a strong development in the perception of speech in background speech and differences in voice characteristics in normal-hearing children as well as CI children during the school-age years and beyond. This development does not only seem to relate to CI children’s hearing abilities, but also to their cognitive abilities that are not adult-like yet. Furthermore, CI children seem to make better use of the information provided by the degraded CI speech signal than CI adults. This could be caused by differences in brain plasticity and early exposure to the CI signal. Thus, better cognitive abilities lead to better voice and speech perception in both normal-hearing and CI children.
- Published
- 2021
223. Idioms in the Aging Brain
- Author
-
Amélie la Roi, Hendriks, Petra, and Sprenger, Simone
- Subjects
business.industry ,Medicine ,Aging brain ,business ,Neuroscience - Abstract
With the world’s population getting older and older, gaining more insight into the cognitive consequences of aging is an urgent matter. Many studies have investigated the effects of aging on general cognitive abilities, such as memory. However, much less is known about the effects of aging on language abilities. This thesis investigates how cognitive aging affects healthy elderly adults’ processing of Dutch expressions that can have multiple interpretations, such as the idiom tegen de lamp lopen (literally: ‘to walk against the lamp’, figuratively: ‘to get caught’). Studying idioms is interesting, as idiom knowledge has been shown to keep increasing until old age. Idiom processing, in contrast, partly depends on cognitive functions that become increasingly vulnerable with age. Comparing young and elderly adults, we found that both use contextual information to facilitate the processing of idioms and that this ability remains stable over several years. However, older adults, but not younger adults, need additional contextual information to process sentences word by word. Also, we found that the suppression of an idiom’s literal meaning, which is necessary to select its figurative meaning, slows down from the age of 40. Additionally, elderly adults were found to be slower than young adults to activate an idiom’s figurative meaning. Taken together, we have demonstrated that linguistic processing changes slowly across the lifespan, but stays remarkably efficient until old age. These findings on language abilities in healthy elderly adults form an important baseline for research on cognitive decline due to neurodegenerative diseases such as dementia.
- Published
- 2021
224. Development of voice perception is dissociated across gender cues in school-age children
- Author
-
Deborah Vickers, Deniz Başkent, Petra Hendriks, Leanne Nagels, Etienne Gaudrain, Nagels, Leanne [0000-0003-4853-969X], Gaudrain, Etienne [0000-0003-0490-0295], Vickers, Deborah [0000-0002-7498-5637], Hendriks, Petra [0000-0002-7584-4078], Başkent, Deniz [0000-0002-6560-1451], Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, University of Groningen [Groningen], Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon (CRNL), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet [Saint-Étienne] (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University Medical Center Groningen [Groningen] (UMCG), Department of Clinical Neurosciences [Cambridge], University of Cambridge [UK] (CAM), Gaudrain, Etienne, Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon - Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet - Saint-Étienne (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Perceptual and Cognitive Neuroscience (PCN), and Robotics and image-guided minimally-invasive surgery (ROBOTICS)
- Subjects
Male ,[SDV.NEU.PC] Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Psychology and behavior ,lcsh:Medicine ,631/378/2649/1723 ,Psychology, Child ,Audiology ,01 natural sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Child Development ,Cognition ,Discrimination, Psychological ,lcsh:Science ,10. No inequality ,Child ,010301 acoustics ,media_common ,Language ,Multidisciplinary ,School age child ,Schools ,[SDV.NEU.PC]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Psychology and behavior ,4. Education ,article ,Gender Identity ,[PHYS.MECA.ACOU]Physics [physics]/Mechanics [physics]/Acoustics [physics.class-ph] ,Categorization ,Child, Preschool ,[SCCO.PSYC] Cognitive science/Psychology ,[SCCO.PSYC]Cognitive science/Psychology ,Auditory Perception ,Cues ,Psychology ,[PHYS.MECA.ACOU] Physics [physics]/Mechanics [physics]/Acoustics [physics.class-ph] ,medicine.medical_specialty ,media_common.quotation_subject ,03 medical and health sciences ,Perception ,0103 physical sciences ,medicine ,Glottal pulse ,Humans ,[SCCO.NEUR]Cognitive science/Neuroscience ,lcsh:R ,[SCCO.NEUR] Cognitive science/Neuroscience ,631/378/2649/1594 ,Voice ,lcsh:Q ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Children’s ability to distinguish speakers’ voices continues to develop throughout childhood, yetit remains unclear how children’s sensitivity to voice cues, such as differences in speakers’ gender,develops over time. This so-called voice gender is primarily characterized by speakers’ meanfundamental frequency (F0), related to glottal pulse rate, and vocal-tract length (VTL), related tospeakers’ size. Here we show that children’s acquisition of adult-like performance for discrimination,a lower-order perceptual task, and categorization, a higher-order cognitive task, differs across voicegender cues. Children’s discrimination was adult-like around the age of 8 for VTL but still differed fromadults at the age of 12 for F0. Children’s perceptual weight attributed to F0 for gender categorizationwas adult-like around the age of 6 but around the age of 10 for VTL. Children’s discrimination andweighting of F0 and VTL were only correlated for 4- to 6-year-olds. Hence, children’s developmentof discrimination and weighting of voice gender cues are dissociated, i.e., adult-like performancefor F0 and VTL is acquired at different rates and does not seem to be closely related. The differentdevelopmental patterns for auditory discrimination and categorization highlight the complexity of therelationship between perceptual and cognitive mechanisms of voice perception.
- Published
- 2020
225. Taking an alternative perspective on language in autism
- Author
-
Overweg, Jennigje, Hendriks, Petra, and Hartman, Catharina
- Abstract
Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have problems with social communication and interaction. These problems are possibly partly due to a perspective-taking deficit, which makes it difficult to understand that others may think, know or see things differently. Jessica Overweg tested whether children with ASD also show language comprehension problems as a result of a perspective-taking deficit. The ability as a hearer to take the speaker’s perspective into account is important in language comprehension. For example, perspective-taking skills are needed to interpret personal pronouns, such as I and you. Suppose a colleague says: James said: “You won the price”. To select the correct referent of you, a shift is needed from the perspective of the actual speaker (the colleague) to the perspective of the reported speaker (James). Without this perspective shift, the hearer will think that he won the price, even though the colleague used the pronoun you to refer to himself. To make this perspective shift, a hearer needs perspective-taking skills. Overweg tested whether children with ASD have difficulties interpreting linguistic expressions that involve perspective taking, such as personal pronouns. She found that children with ASD show language comprehension problems partly due to a perspective-taking deficit. This means that the perspective-taking deficit in children with ASD not only influence their social communication and interaction skills, but also influence their language comprehension skills. Therefore, it is important that in the future, in a clinical setting and at schools, more attention is paid to language comprehension in children with ASD.
- Published
- 2018
226. Reference and cognition: Experimental and computational cognitive modeling studies on reference processing in Dutch and Italian
- Author
-
Vogelzang, Margreet, Hendriks, Petra, and van Rijn, Hedderik
- Abstract
Veel van de uitdrukkingen die we in een gesprek gebruiken verwijzen naar iets of iemand om ons heen, zoals 'de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen' of 'de leadzanger van Coldplay'. Sommige uitdrukkingen kunnen naar meerdere dingen of personen verwijzen, zoals de voornaamwoorden 'hij' en 'zij'. De uitdaging voor de luisteraar is om te begrijpen wat de spreker bedoelde met zo’n uitdrukking. Margreet Vogelzang onderzocht hoe het proces van het begrijpen van verwijzende uitdrukkingen in zijn werk gaat, en of hierin verschillen zijn tussen talen. In het Nederlands kun je namelijk naar iemand verwijzen met een voornaamwoord (zoals 'hij' of 'zij'), maar in het Italiaans kun je ofwel zo'n voornaamwoord gebruiken, ofwel het onderwerp van een zin helemaal weglaten. Je zegt dan bijvoorbeeld 'corre' (d.w.z. ‘rent’) in plaats van 'hij rent'. Door het meten van de pupilgrootte van volwassenen terwijl ze naar zinnen in hun moedertaal luisterden, ontdekte Vogelzang dat niet alleen kinderen, maar zelfs volwassenen soms veel moeite hebben met het begrijpen van verwijzende uitdrukkingen die naar meerdere dingen kunnen verwijzen. Daarnaast ontdekte ze dat het gebruik van een explicietere uitdrukking (bijvoorbeeld 'de egel' in plaats van 'hij') het proces van begrijpen makkelijker maakt, en dat dit gemak terug te zien is in de grootte van je pupil. Vogelzang vond verder dat voornaamwoorden in verschillende talen een net iets andere betekenis kunnen hebben, wat belangrijk is wanneer je als Nederlander Italiaans wilt leren of omgekeerd, en ook voor de ontwikkeling van computerprogramma’s die teksten vertalen. Door het proces van taalbegrip na te bootsen met computermodellen concludeerde Vogelzang dat de moeite die kinderen hebben met het begrijpen van verwijzende uitdrukkingen veroorzaakt wordt door een lagere snelheid van zinsverwerking en minder werkgeheugencapaciteit in vergelijking met volwassenen. Interessant is dat Italiaanse kinderen van 8 jaar nog problemen hebben met het begrijpen van een een weggelaten onderwerp, een vorm die in het Nederlands niet bestaat. Daarnaast laten de computermodellen zien dat het in het Italiaans nodig is om te redeneren over wat de spreker bedoelde om een voornaamwoord te goed te kunnen begrijpen. Samen laten deze studies zien dat dat je voor een goed begrip van verwijzende uitdrukkingen zowel voldoende kennis van de taal als voldoende verwerkingssnelheid en werkgeheugencapaciteit nodig hebt.
- Published
- 2017
227. Between direct and indirect speech: The acquisition of pronouns in reported speech
- Author
-
Köder, Franziska Maria, Romeijn, Jan-Willem, Hendriks, Petra, Maier, Emar, and Graduate School
- Abstract
Een fundamentele eigenschap van het menselijk taalvermogen is het kunnen weergeven van wat iemand anders gezegd heeft. De meeste talen, inclusief het Nederlands, maken een onderscheid tussen twee soorten redeweergave: directe rede (Anna zei: "Ik ben blij") en indirecte rede (Anna zei dat ze blij was). In mijn proefschrift onderzoek ik hoe en wanneer kinderen in staat zijn om directe en indirecte rede uit elkaar te houden. Ik heb hiervoor een tablet app ontworpen, waarin kinderen op grond van gesproken zinnetjes als “Olifant zei dat ik de voetbal krijg” moeten bepalen welk dier de voetbal krijgt. De resultaten laten zien dat Nederlandse kinderen vanaf vier jaar de indirecte rede al onder de knie hebben, maar dat zelfs elfjarigen nog worstelen met de directe rede. Dit is verrassend omdat kinderboeken juist relatief veel directe rede bevatten. Om deze paradox op te lossen, hebben we het tablet spel omgebouwd tot een soort interactief plaatjesboek. Het blijkt dat kinderen vanaf zes jaar geen moeite hebben met de directe rede als die gepresenteerd wordt in de context van een verhaal. We concluderen dat kinderen een minder strict onderscheid maken tussen directe en indirete rede dan volwassenen, en hiervoor meer afhankelijk zijn van de context.
- Published
- 2016
228. Communication abilities of children with ASD and ADHD: Production, comprehension, and cognitive mechanisms
- Author
-
Kuijper, Sanne Joanna Maria, Hendriks, Petra, and Hartman, Catharina
- Abstract
Kinderen met een autisme spectrum stoornis (ASS) hebben vaak communicatieproblemen. Bij kinderen met ADHD lijkt er soms sprake te zijn van communicatieproblemen, maar hierover is nog maar weinig bekend. Voor een geslaagde communicatie is het van cruciaal belang dat de ander begrijpt wat je bedoelt. Daarom moet je als spreker rekening houden met je luisteraar, en omgekeerd. Je moet bijvoorbeeld als spreker voortdurend bepalen hoe specifiek je moet zijn als je naar iemand verwijst. Je kunt het hebben over ‘de zanger’ (specifiek), of ‘hij’ gebruiken (minder specifiek). Ik heb onderzocht hoe kinderen met ASS en kinderen met ADHD presteerden op taaltaken die taalproductie en taalbegrip testen. Ik keek daarbij of ze in hun taal rekening hielden met hun gesprekspartner. Bovendien onderzocht ik of hun inlevingsvermogen, werkgeheugen en impulscontrole bijdroegen aan succesvolle communicatie. In gestructureerde taaltaken bleken kinderen met ASS en kinderen met ADHD even goed rekening te houden met hun gesprekspartner als kinderen zonder ontwikkelingsproblemen. Hoe groter hun inlevingsvermogen en impulscontrole, hoe beter ze rekening hielden met hun gesprekspartner. De capaciteit van hun werkgeheugen was vooral belangrijk in complexe situaties waarbij veel informatie onthouden moest worden. Ondanks hun vermogen om rekening houden met de ander in taal vond ik meerdere taalproblemen bij kinderen met ASS en kinderen met ADHD. Zo hadden beide groepen in minder gestructureerde taaltaken moeite om verbanden te leggen in hun verhalen en om grammaticaal complexe zinnen te maken. Dit laat zien dat taal soms een uitdaging vormt voor zowel kinderen met ASS als kinderen met ADHD.
- Published
- 2016
229. Projection in discourse: A data-driven formal semantic analysis
- Author
-
Venhuizen, Noortje Joost, Bos, Johan, and Hendriks, Petra
- Abstract
A sentence like "Bertrand, a famous linguist, wrote a book" contains different contributions: there is a person named "Bertrand", he is a famous linguist, and he wrote a book. These contributions convey different types of information; while the existence of Bertrand is presented as given information---it is presupposed---the other contributions signal new information. Moreover, the contributions are affected differently by linguistic constructions. The inference that Bertrand wrote a book disappears when the sentence is negated or turned into interrogative form, while the other contributions survive; this is called 'projection'. In this thesis, I investigate the relation between different types of contributions in a sentence from a theoretical and empirical perspective. I focus on projection phenomena, which include presuppositions ('Bertrand exists' in the aforementioned example) and conventional implicatures ('Bertrand is a famous linguist'). I argue that the differences between the contributions can be explained in terms of information status, which describes how content relates to the unfolding discourse context. Based on this analysis, I extend the widely used formal representational system Discourse Representation Theory (DRT) with an explicit representation of the different contributions made by projection phenomena; this extension is called 'Projective Discourse Representation Theory' (PDRT). I present a data-driven computational analysis based on data from the Groningen Meaning Bank, a corpus of semantically annotated texts. This analysis shows how PDRT can be used to learn more about different kinds of projection behaviour. These results can be used to improve linguistically oriented computational applications such as automatic translation systems.
- Published
- 2015
230. Early word order and animacy
- Author
-
Cannizzaro, Courtney Leigh, Hendriks, Petra, and Faculty of Arts
- Subjects
taalverwerving ,Woordvolgorde ,Proefschriften (vorm) ,Kinderen - Abstract
Kinderen begrijpen woorden en zinnen meestal eerder dan dat ze diezelfde woorden en zinnen zelf kunnen produceren. Maar is dat altijd zo? In dit proefschrift wordt onderzocht hoe Nederlandstalige en Engelstalige kinderen van 2 en 3 jaar oud zinnen in een bepaalde woordvolgorde kunnen begrijpen en produceren. Binnen het onderzoek zijn verschillende taken gebruikt om begrip en productie te testen. Bij een aantal van deze taken is gebruik gemaakt van eyetracking. Simpele transitieve zinnen met levende en niet-levende objecten werden gebruikt, zoals de auto duwt de koe of de koe duwt de auto. Uit het onderzoek blijkt dat jonge kinderen kunnen woorden in de goede volgorde zetten om de juiste betekenis uit te drukken. Echter, ze zijn niet zo goed in staat om de woordvolgorde juist te interpreteren als ze bepaalde zinnen horen. Jonge kinderen hebben vooral moeite met het interpreteren van zinnen met een niet-levend onderwerp in combinatie met een levend lijdend voorwerp. Ze interpreteren een zin als de auto duwt de koe eerder als een situatie waarin de koe de auto duwt, omdat de koe een levend object is (en de auto niet). Hoewel volwassenen woordvolgorde gebruiken bij het interpreteren van zinnen, werd wel gevonden dat zij langzamer waren bij het interpreteren van zinnen met een niet-levend onderwerp. Op basis van deze resultaten kan er geconcludeerd worden dat de productie van woordvolgorde eerder geleerd wordt dan het begrijpen hiervan. Verder lijkt levendheid een rol te spelen bij het interpreteren van woordvolgorde, zowel bij kinderen als bij volwassenen. De opvallende en ongewone volgorde van taalontwikkeling die dit onderzoek laat zien, is moeilijk te verklaren binnen de traditionele kijk dat begrip altijd voorloopt op productie. Deze asymmetrie heeft gevolgen voor zowel taalontwikkelingonderzoek als voor theorieën van grammatica.
- Published
- 2012
231. Pronoun processing: computational, behavioral, and psychophysiological studies in children and adults
- Author
-
van Rij, Jacolien, Hendriks, Petra, and van Rijn, Hedderik
- Abstract
In de frase ‘Gisteren sprak James met Rob. Hij bekende de diefstal.’ kan zowel James als Rob de bekennende dief zijn. De oorzaak van deze ambiguïteit is dat persoonlijke voornaamwoorden zoals hij en hem geen vaste betekenis hebben, maar dat hun betekenis afhankelijk is van de context. Jacolien van Rij onderzocht waarom persoonlijke voornaamwoorden eenvoudig lijken voor volwassenen, maar moeilijk zijn voor kinderen. Verschillende factoren spelen een rol bij de interpretatie van voornaamwoorden, waaronder de structuur van de voorafgaande zin. Volwassenen weten meestal meteen naar wie het voornaamwoord verwijst, maar kinderen interpreteren een voornaamwoord vaak anders dan bedoeld door de spreker. Jacolien van Rij onderzocht waarom persoonlijke voornaamwoorden eenvoudig lijken voor volwassenen, maar moeilijk voor kinderen. Om deze vraag te beantwoorden ontwikkelde Van Rij computermodellen die het produceren en interpreteren van persoonlijke voornaamwoorden nabootsen. Deze modellen testte ze in verschillende experimenten met kinderen en volwassenen. Van Rij ontdekte dat kinderen zich meer gedroegen als volwassenen en minder fouten maakten met voornaamwoorden wanneer ze vertraagde spraak hoorden. Dit bevestigt het idee dat de verwerkingssnelheid van kinderen onvoldoende is om alle relevante informatie te kunnen gebruiken. Volwassenen gedroegen zich daarentegen meer als kinderen wanneer ze tijdens het interpreteren van een voornaamwoord een tweede taak moesten uitvoeren, zoals het onthouden van cijfers. Aanvullende studies, waarin hersenactiviteit werd gemeten met EEG en pupilgrootte met eyetracking, suggereren dat het volwassen gebruik van de context afhankelijk is van voldoende werkgeheugencapaciteit. Kortom, de schijnbaar eenvoudige interpretatie van persoonlijke voornaamwoorden is eigenlijk een ingewikkeld proces dat op veel manieren mis kan gaan.
- Published
- 2012
232. Starting a sentence in Dutch: A corpus study of subject- and object-fronting
- Author
-
Bouma, G.J., Hendriks, Petra, and Hoeksema, Jack
- Subjects
ComputingMethodologies_DOCUMENTANDTEXTPROCESSING ,GeneralLiterature_REFERENCE(e.g.,dictionaries,encyclopedias,glossaries) - Published
- 2008
233. Prelingually Deaf Children With Cochlear Implants Show Better Perception of Voice Cues and Speech in Competing Speech Than Postlingually Deaf Adults With Cochlear Implants.
- Author
-
Nagels L, Gaudrain E, Vickers D, Hendriks P, and Başkent D
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Female, Child, Adult, Young Adult, Adolescent, Voice physiology, Case-Control Studies, Child, Preschool, Middle Aged, Cochlear Implants, Deafness rehabilitation, Speech Perception, Cues, Cochlear Implantation
- Abstract
Objectives: Postlingually deaf adults with cochlear implants (CIs) have difficulties with perceiving differences in speakers' voice characteristics and benefit little from voice differences for the perception of speech in competing speech. However, not much is known yet about the perception and use of voice characteristics in prelingually deaf implanted children with CIs. Unlike CI adults, most CI children became deaf during the acquisition of language. Extensive neuroplastic changes during childhood could make CI children better at using the available acoustic cues than CI adults, or the lack of exposure to a normal acoustic speech signal could make it more difficult for them to learn which acoustic cues they should attend to. This study aimed to examine to what degree CI children can perceive voice cues and benefit from voice differences for perceiving speech in competing speech, comparing their abilities to those of normal-hearing (NH) children and CI adults., Design: CI children's voice cue discrimination (experiment 1), voice gender categorization (experiment 2), and benefit from target-masker voice differences for perceiving speech in competing speech (experiment 3) were examined in three experiments. The main focus was on the perception of mean fundamental frequency (F0) and vocal-tract length (VTL), the primary acoustic cues related to speakers' anatomy and perceived voice characteristics, such as voice gender., Results: CI children's F0 and VTL discrimination thresholds indicated lower sensitivity to differences compared with their NH-age-equivalent peers, but their mean discrimination thresholds of 5.92 semitones (st) for F0 and 4.10 st for VTL indicated higher sensitivity than postlingually deaf CI adults with mean thresholds of 9.19 st for F0 and 7.19 st for VTL. Furthermore, CI children's perceptual weighting of F0 and VTL cues for voice gender categorization closely resembled that of their NH-age-equivalent peers, in contrast with CI adults. Finally, CI children had more difficulties in perceiving speech in competing speech than their NH-age-equivalent peers, but they performed better than CI adults. Unlike CI adults, CI children showed a benefit from target-masker voice differences in F0 and VTL, similar to NH children., Conclusion: Although CI children's F0 and VTL voice discrimination scores were overall lower than those of NH children, their weighting of F0 and VTL cues for voice gender categorization and their benefit from target-masker differences in F0 and VTL resembled that of NH children. Together, these results suggest that prelingually deaf implanted CI children can effectively utilize spectrotemporally degraded F0 and VTL cues for voice and speech perception, generally outperforming postlingually deaf CI adults in comparable tasks. These findings underscore the presence of F0 and VTL cues in the CI signal to a certain degree and suggest other factors contributing to the perception challenges faced by CI adults., (Copyright © 2024 The Authors. Ear & Hearing is published on behalf of the American Auditory Society, by Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
234. A cognitive modeling approach to learning and using reference biases in language.
- Author
-
Toth AG, Hendriks P, Taatgen NA, and van Rij J
- Abstract
During real-time language processing, people rely on linguistic and non-linguistic biases to anticipate upcoming linguistic input. One of these linguistic biases is known as the implicit causality bias , wherein language users anticipate that certain entities will be rementioned in the discourse based on the entity's particular role in an expressed causal event. For example, when language users encounter a sentence like "Elizabeth congratulated Tina…" during real-time language processing, they seemingly anticipate that the discourse will continue about Tina, the object referent, rather than Elizabeth, the subject referent. However, it is often unclear how these reference biases are acquired and how exactly they get used during real-time language processing. In order to investigate these questions, we developed a reference learning model within the PRIMs cognitive architecture that simulated the process of predicting upcoming discourse referents and their linguistic forms. Crucially, across the linguistic input the model was presented with, there were asymmetries with respect to how the discourse continued. By utilizing the learning mechanisms of the PRIMs architecture, the model was able to optimize its predictions, ultimately leading to biased model behavior. More specifically, following subject-biased implicit causality verbs the model was more likely to predict that the discourse would continue about the subject referent, whereas following object-biased implicit causality verbs the model was more likely to predict that the discourse would continue about the object referent. In a similar fashion, the model was more likely to predict that subject referent continuations would be in the form of a pronoun, whereas object referent continuations would be in the form of a proper name. These learned biases were also shown to generalize to novel contexts in which either the verb or the subject and object referents were new. The results of the present study demonstrate that seemingly complex linguistic behavior can be explained by cognitively plausible domain-general learning mechanisms. This study has implications for psycholinguistic accounts of predictive language processing and language learning, as well as for theories of implicit causality and reference processing., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2022 Toth, Hendriks, Taatgen and van Rij.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
235. An exploration of error-driven learning in simple two-layer networks from a discriminative learning perspective.
- Author
-
Hoppe DB, Hendriks P, Ramscar M, and van Rij J
- Subjects
- Humans, Machine Learning, Algorithms, Brain, Artificial Intelligence, Discrimination Learning
- Abstract
Error-driven learning algorithms, which iteratively adjust expectations based on prediction error, are the basis for a vast array of computational models in the brain and cognitive sciences that often differ widely in their precise form and application: they range from simple models in psychology and cybernetics to current complex deep learning models dominating discussions in machine learning and artificial intelligence. However, despite the ubiquity of this mechanism, detailed analyses of its basic workings uninfluenced by existing theories or specific research goals are rare in the literature. To address this, we present an exposition of error-driven learning - focusing on its simplest form for clarity - and relate this to the historical development of error-driven learning models in the cognitive sciences. Although historically error-driven models have been thought of as associative, such that learning is thought to combine preexisting elemental representations, our analysis will highlight the discriminative nature of learning in these models and the implications of this for the way how learning is conceptualized. We complement our theoretical introduction to error-driven learning with a practical guide to the application of simple error-driven learning models in which we discuss a number of example simulations, that are also presented in detail in an accompanying tutorial., (© 2021. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
236. Prediction Impairment May Explain Communication Difficulties in Autism.
- Author
-
Scholten I, Hartman CA, and Hendriks P
- Abstract
Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
237. Child-Like Adults: Dual-Task Effects on Collective vs. Distributive Sentence Interpretations.
- Author
-
de Koster AMB, Hendriks P, and Spenader JK
- Abstract
In this work, we consider a recent proposal that claims that the preferred interpretation of sentences containing definite plural expressions, such as "The boys are building a snowman," is not determined by semantic composition but is pragmatically derived via an implicature. Plural expressions can express that each member of a group acts individually (distributive interpretation) or that the group acts together (collective interpretation). While adults prefer collective interpretations for sentences that are not explicitly marked for distributivity by the distributive marker each , children do not show this preference. One explanation is that the adult collective preference for definite plurals arises due to a conversational implicature. If implicature calculation requires memory resources, children may fail to calculate the implicature due to memory limitations. This study investigated whether loading Dutch-speaking adults' working memory, using a dual task, would elicit more child-like distributive interpretations, as would be predicted by the implicature account. We found that loading WM in adults did lead to response patterns more similar to children. We discuss whether our results offer a plausible explanation for children's development of an understanding of distributivity and how our results relate to recent debates on the role of cognitive resources in implicature calculation., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2021 de Koster, Hendriks and Spenader.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
238. Children's Pronoun Interpretation Problems Are Related to Theory of Mind and Inhibition, But Not Working Memory.
- Author
-
Kuijper SJM, Hartman CA, and Hendriks P
- Abstract
In several languages, including English and Dutch, children's acquisition of the interpretation of object pronouns (e.g., him ) is delayed compared to that of reflexives (e.g., himself ). Various syntactic and pragmatic explanations have been proposed to account for this delay in children's acquisition of pronoun interpretation. This study aims to provide more insight into this delay by investigating potential cognitive mechanisms underlying this delay. Dutch-speaking children between 6 and 12 years old with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n = 47), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; n = 36) or typical development (TD; n = 38) were tested on their interpretation and production of object pronouns and reflexives and on theory of mind, working memory, and response inhibition. It was found that all three groups of children had difficulty with pronoun interpretation and that their performance on pronoun interpretation was associated with theory of mind and inhibition. These findings support an explanation of object pronoun interpretation in terms of perspective taking, according to which listeners need to consider the speaker's perspective in order to block coreference between the object pronoun and the subject of the same sentence. Unlike what is predicted by alternative theoretical accounts, performance on pronoun interpretation was not associated with working memory, and the children made virtually no errors in their production of object pronouns. As the difficulties with pronoun interpretation were similar for children with ASD, children with ADHD and typically developing children, this suggests that certain types of perspective taking are unaffected in children with ASD and ADHD., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2021 Kuijper, Hartman and Hendriks.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
239. Complex Inferential Processes Are Needed for Implicature Comprehension, but Not for Implicature Production.
- Author
-
Mognon I, Sprenger SA, Kuijper SJM, and Hendriks P
- Abstract
Upon hearing "Some of Michelangelo's sculptures are in Rome," adults can easily generate a scalar implicature and infer that the intended meaning of the utterance corresponds to "Some but not all Michelangelo's sculptures are in Rome." Comprehension experiments show that preschoolers struggle with this kind of inference until at least 5 years of age. Surprisingly, the few studies having investigated children's production of scalar expressions like some and all suggest that production is adult-like already in their third year of life. Thus, children's production of implicatures seems to develop at least 2 years before their comprehension of implicatures. In this paper, we present a novel account of scalar implicature generation in the framework of Bidirectional Optimality Theory: the Asymmetry Account. We show that the production-comprehension asymmetry is predicted to emerge because the comprehension of some requires the hearer to consider the speaker's perspective, but the production of some does not require the speaker to consider the hearer's perspective. Hence, children's comprehension of scalar expressions, but not their production of scalar expressions, is predicted to be related to their theory of mind development. Not possessing fully developed theory of mind abilities yet, children thus have difficulty in comprehending scalar expressions such as some in an adult-like way. Our account also explains why variable performance is found in experimental studies testing children's ability to generate scalar implicatures; moreover, it describes the differences between children's and adults' implicature generation in terms of their ability to recursively apply theory of mind; finally, it sheds new light on the question why the interpretation of numerals does not require implicature generation., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2021 Mognon, Sprenger, Kuijper and Hendriks.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
240. The acquisition of compositional meaning.
- Author
-
Hendriks P
- Subjects
- Child, Humans, Language Development, Speech Perception, Comprehension physiology, Language
- Abstract
How do people create meaning from a string of sounds or pattern of dots? Insights into this process can be obtained from the way children acquire sentence meanings. According to the well-known principle of compositionality, the meaning of an expression is a function of the meanings of its parts and the way they are syntactically combined. However, children frequently seem to ignore syntactic structure in their sentence interpretations, suggesting that syntax is merely one of the sources of information constraining meaning and does not have a special status. A fundamental assumption in the argument in favour of compositionality is that speakers and listeners generally agree upon the meanings of sentences. Remarkably, however, children as listeners do not always understand what they are able to produce as speakers, and vice versa. For example, children's production of word order appears to develop ahead of their comprehension of word order in the acquisition of languages like English and Dutch. Such production-comprehension asymmetries are not uncommon in child language and motivate a view of compositionality as a principle pertaining to the result of perspective taking, and of meaning composition as a process of speaker-listener coordination. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards mechanistic models of meaning composition'.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
241. Cognitive architectures and language acquisition: a case study in pronoun comprehension.
- Author
-
VAN Rij J, VAN Rijn H, and Hendriks P
- Subjects
- Child, Child Language, Child, Preschool, Databases, Factual, Female, Humans, Linguistics, Male, Psycholinguistics, Speech, Theory of Mind, Time Factors, Cognition, Comprehension, Computer Simulation, Learning, Models, Psychological, Speech Perception
- Abstract
In this paper we discuss a computational cognitive model of children's poor performance on pronoun interpretation (the so-called Delay of Principle B Effect, or DPBE). This cognitive model is based on a theoretical account that attributes the DPBE to children's inability as hearers to also take into account the speaker's perspective. The cognitive model predicts that child hearers are unable to do so because their speed of linguistic processing is too limited to perform this second step in interpretation. We tested this hypothesis empirically in a psycholinguistic study, in which we slowed down the speech rate to give children more time for interpretation, and in a computational simulation study. The results of the two studies confirm the predictions of our model. Moreover, these studies show that embedding a theory of linguistic competence in a cognitive architecture allows for the generation of detailed and testable predictions with respect to linguistic performance.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
242. Fill the gap! Combining pragmatic and prosodic information to make gapping easy.
- Author
-
Hoeks JC, Redeker G, and Hendriks P
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Analysis of Variance, Cognition, Cues, Female, Humans, Male, Reaction Time, Semantics, Speech, Young Adult, Psycholinguistics, Speech Perception
- Abstract
Two studies investigated the effects of prosody and pragmatic context on off-line and on-line processing of sentences like John greeted Paul yesterday and Ben today. Such sentences are ambiguous between the so-called 'nongapping' reading, where John greeted Ben, and the highly unpreferred 'gapping' reading, where Ben greeted Paul. In the first experiment, participants listened to dialogues and gave a speeded response as to which reading of an ambiguous target sentence first comes to mind. In the second experiment, they also responded to a visual probe that was presented during the presentation of the ambiguous target. The results show that context and prosody have independent and strong effects on both on-line processing and off-line interpretation of gapping; in the right combination they can make gapping as easy as the normally preferred nongapping reading.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
243. Coherent discourse solves the pronoun interpretation problem.
- Author
-
Spenader J, Smits EJ, and Hendriks P
- Subjects
- Analysis of Variance, Child, Child, Preschool, Comprehension, Humans, Speech Perception, Child Language, Psycholinguistics, Speech
- Abstract
Many comprehension studies have shown that children as late as age 6 ; 6 misinterpret object pronouns as co-referring with the referential subject about half the time. A recent review of earlier experiments testing children's interpretation of object pronouns in sentences with quantified subjects (Elbourne, 2005) also suggests that there is a 'Pronoun Interpretation Problem'. In contrast, two experiments addressing English children's pronoun production (Bloom, Barss, Nicol & Conway, 1994; de Villiers, Cahillane & Altreuter, 2006) show almost perfect usage. The aim of this study is to verify this asymmetry between pronoun production and pronoun comprehension for Dutch, and to investigate the effects of coherent discourse and topicality on pronoun production and comprehension. Employing a truth-value judgment task and an elicited production task, this study indeed finds such an asymmetry in 83 Dutch children (age range 4 ; 5-6 ; 6). When object pronouns were clearly established as the topic of the target sentence, the Pronoun Interpretation Problem dissolved entirely. These results are compatible with the asymmetrical grammar hypothesis of Hendriks & Spenader (2005/2006) and suggest, contrary to many previous claims, that children are highly proficient at using pragmatic clues in interpretation.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.