Older adults’ functional outcomes, such as the ability to live independently, psychological well-being (Lawton, 1987; Willis, 1991), health care utilization, and institutionalization (Miller & Weissert, 2000; Wolinsky, et al., 1983, 2007; Wolinsky, Callahan, Fitzgerald, & Johnson, 1993), have been tied to their ability to perform instrumental activities of daily living (IADL, Lawton & Brody, 1969). IADL include meal preparation, telephone use, financial management, and other self- and home-maintenance tasks (Willis & Marsiske, 1991; Willis & Schaie, 1993). Successful IADL performance has physical, emotional, and cognitive components (Willis, 1996). For example, elders’ ability to use public transportation might depend on their physical health (e.g., ambulation), as well as the cognitive capacity to remember a bus schedule. The cognitive aspect of IADL performance has been termed “everyday cognition” and conceptualized as the application of “basic” cognitive skills like reasoning, memory, and processing speed to solving IADL problems (Marsiske, & Margrett, 2006; Willis, 1996; Willis & Marsiske, 1991; Willis & Schaie, 1986). Numerous studies to date have used “everyday cognitive” measures (e.g., Everyday Problems Test, Willis & Marsiske, 1993) as performance-based means of assessing older adults’ cognitive IADL competence (e.g., Allaire et al., 2009; Gross et al., 2011; see Yam & Marsiske, in press, for an analysis of correlated change in performance-based everyday cognition and self-reported IADL). Everyday cognition is assessed objectively by asking older adults to perform everyday-type tasks, such as looking up a phone number in the phone book or reading a nutrition label (Owsley, Sloane, McGwin, & Ball, 2002). With regard to “basic” cognition, the term “basic” is used to reflect an underlying hierarchical model which suggests that acontextual, construct-pure measures assessed via laboratory and experimental techniques represent the cognitive constituents that are compiled to produce complex everyday performances (Cattell, 1987; Salthouse. 1990; Willis & Marsiske, 1991). Everyday cognition is thought to rely on multiple basic cognitive skills like reasoning, memory, and speed of processing (Marsiske & Margrett, 2006; Salthouse, 1990; Willis & Marsiske, 1991; Willis & Schaie, 1986), as well as crystallized and procedural knowledge stemming from prior experience performing IADL (Marsiske & Margrett, 2006). Because crystalized and procedural knowledge is relatively preserved against age-related decline (Ackerman, 2008; Salthouse, 1990; Singer et al., 2003), some authors have speculated that everyday cognition might be “buffered” against the age related decline observed in basic abilities (Cornelius & Caspi, 1987; see also Marsiske & Willis, 1995). The underlying conceptual rationale for why everyday cognition might be relatively buffered against age-associated declines rests in the putatively greater relevance, familiarity (e.g., can of food) and enacted practice (Cornelius & Caspi, 1987) of everyday tasks. At the same time, to the extent that basic information processes are involved in everyday cognitive performance, the general understanding of aging in domains like attention, working memory, episodic memory, speed of processing, and executive functioning is one of normative decline in the later decades of life (Ghisletta & Lindenberger, 2004; Ghisletta, Rabbitt, Lunn, & Lindenberger, 2012; Park, Lautenschlager, Hedden, Davidson, Smith, & Smith, 2002; Ronnlund, Nyberg, Backman, & Nilsson, 2005; Schaie, 2012; Singer, Verhaeghen, Ghisletta, Lindenberger, & Baltes, 2003; Zimprich, Martin, Kliegel, Dellenbach, Rast, & Zeintl, 2008). Given the multidirectionality in the cognitive dimensions underlying everyday cognition (Baltes, 1987), it is not surprising that empirical support has been mixed with regard to the potential preservation of everyday cognitive skills against age-related decline. In a meta-analysis of age differences in everyday cognition, Thornton and Dumke (2005) concluded that results from 28 cross-sectional studies they reviewed do not support theories of the preservation of everyday cognitive abilities. In terms of intraindividual change, however, Willis, Jay, Diehl, and Marsiske (1992) demonstrated that in the presence of overall mean decline with age, 62% of the sample exhibited stability or improvement in everyday cognitive abilities over a 7-year period. In the absence of studies comparing the longitudinal slopes of basic and everyday cognition, it is difficult to conclude whether everyday cognitive abilities are as vulnerable to decline as basic cognitive skills. In line with the idea that everyday cognition is associated with basic cognitive abilities, mostly cross-sectional studies reported correlations ranging from 0.31 to 0.86 between basic and everyday cognitive skills (Allaire & Marsiske, 1999; Burton, Strauss, Hultsch, Hunter, 2006; Diehl et al., 1995; Thornton, Deria, Gelb, Shapiro, Hill, 2007; Weatherbee & Allaire, 2008; Wood et al., 2005; see Marsiske & Margrett, 2006 for a review). Controlling for demographic variables, basic cognitive predictors together explained between 18% and 55% of the variance in everyday cognition (e.g., Gross et al., 2011; Mitchell & Miller, 2008; Thornton et al., 2007; Wood et al., 2005). Overall, between-persons relationships between basic cognitive abilities and everyday cognition have received considerable attention (e.g., Allaire & Marsiske, 1999; Burton, Strauss, Hultsch, Hunter, 2006; Diehl et al., 1995; Thornton, Deria, Gelb, Shapiro, Hill, 2007; Weatherbee & Allaire, 2008; Wood et al., 2005), while relatively few studies have examined this relationship over time. In one of the few longitudinal investigations, Willis et al. (1992) reported that baseline level of fluid intelligence uniquely explained 52% of total variance in a measure of everyday document literacy assessed seven years later. More recently, using data from the ACTIVE study, Gross et al. (2011) reported that baseline inductive reasoning was a significant unique predictor of baseline everyday cognition (R2=0.18), while baseline memory was a significant predictor of 5-year longitudinal trajectory of everyday cognition (R2=0.06). Also using ACTIVE data, Tucker-Drob (2011) reported significant bivariate associations between 5-year trajectories of basic abilities, and tasks assessing everyday cognitive skills (rs .31 to .94). Both Gross et al. (2011) and Tucker-Drob (2011) modeled only linear trajectories over time, while prior longitudinal work in aging suggests a trajectory of quadratic decline (i.e., inverse-U function) in cognitive abilities (Grady & Craik, 2000; Lindenberger & Ghisletta, 2009; MacDonald et al., 2011; Schaie, 1994; however see Salthouse, 2010). The present study aimed to extend the findings of Gross et al. (2011) and Tucker-Drob (2011) by describing the longitudinal trajectories of everyday cognitive and basic abilities over 10 years of assessment. Results are thought to contribute a fundamental understanding of normative 10-year change in these skills. Unique contributions of the ACTIVE study include, (a) utilization of multiple distinct measures of everyday cognition; (b) inclusion of multiple cognitive factors as time-varying predictors of everyday cognitive growth; and (c) enrollment of a very large, diverse sample of older adults. Specific contributions of the current study include (a) availability of 10-year longitudinal data for basic and everyday cognition; and (b) more careful statistical adjustment for potential confounders of the basic-everyday cognition relationship than previous studies have allowed. Thus, specific aims of the current study were to (a) characterize the 10-year longitudinal growth in a multivariate construct of everyday cognition; (b) to statistically compare longitudinal growth in everyday cognition to that observed in basic laboratory/experimental measures (thereby assessing whether everyday cognition is relatively buffered against age associated decline); and (c) evaluate complex multivariate models that simultaneously investigate how well correlated growth in multiple basic abilities, health, and sensory functioning predict everyday cognition. The fundamental question driving the current study was whether growth in basic cognition over ten years “travelled together” with growth in everyday cognition.