313 results on '"Ernst Fehr"'
Search Results
2. A causal role for the right frontal eye fields in value comparison
- Author
-
Ian Krajbich, Andres Mitsumasu, Rafael Polania, Christian C Ruff, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
attention ,eye tracking ,brain stimulation ,drift diffusion model ,decision making ,frontal eye fields ,Medicine ,Science ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Recent studies have suggested close functional links between overt visual attention and decision making. This suggests that the corresponding mechanisms may interface in brain regions known to be crucial for guiding visual attention – such as the frontal eye field (FEF). Here, we combined brain stimulation, eye tracking, and computational approaches to explore this possibility. We show that inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the right FEF has a causal impact on decision making, reducing the effect of gaze dwell time on choice while also increasing reaction times. We computationally characterize this putative mechanism by using the attentional drift diffusion model (aDDM), which reveals that FEF inhibition reduces the relative discounting of the non-fixated option in the comparison process. Our findings establish an important causal role of the right FEF in choice, elucidate the underlying mechanism, and provide support for one of the key causal hypotheses associated with the aDDM.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. The evolution of distorted beliefs vs. mistaken choices under asymmetric error costs
- Author
-
Charles Efferson, Ryan McKay, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Cultural evolution ,error management theory ,herding ,social learning ,hyperactive agency detection ,Human evolution ,GN281-289 ,Evolution ,QH359-425 - Abstract
Why do people sometimes hold unjustified beliefs and make harmful choices? Three hypotheses include (a) contemporary incentives in which some errors cost more than others, (b) cognitive biases evolved to manage ancestral incentives with variation in error costs and (c) social learning based on choice frequencies. With both modelling and a behavioural experiment, we examined all three mechanisms. The model and experiment support the conclusion that contemporary cost asymmetries affect choices by increasing the rate of cheap errors to reduce the rate of expensive errors. Our model shows that a cognitive bias can distort the evolution of beliefs and in turn behaviour. Unless the bias is strong, however, beliefs often evolve in the correct direction. This suggests limitations on how cognitive biases shape choices, which further indicates that detecting the behavioural consequences of biased cognition may sometimes be challenging. Our experiment used a prime intended to activate a bias called ‘hyperactive agency detection’, and the prime had no detectable effect on choices. Finally, both the model and experiment show that frequency-dependent social learning can generate choice dynamics in which some populations converge on widespread errors, but this outcome hinges on the other two mechanisms being neutral with respect to choice.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. The risk of female genital cutting in Europe: Comparing immigrant attitudes toward uncut girls with attitudes in a practicing country
- Author
-
Sonja Vogt, Charles Efferson, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Female genital cutting ,Immigration ,Implicit association test ,Sudan ,Public aspects of medicine ,RA1-1270 ,Social sciences (General) ,H1-99 - Abstract
Worldwide, an estimated 200 million girls and women have been subjected to female genital cutting. Female genital cutting is defined as an intentional injury to the female genitalia without medical justification. The practice occurs in at least 29 countries in Africa, the Middle East, and Asia. In addition, globalization and migration have brought immigrants from countries where cutting is commonly practiced to countries where cutting is not traditionally practiced and may even be illegal. In countries receiving immigrants, governments and development agencies would like to know if girls with parents who immigrated from practicing countries are at risk of being cut. Risk assessments, for example, could help governments identify the need for programs promoting the abandonment of cutting among immigrants. Extrapolating from the prevalence and incidence rates in practicing countries, however, is generally not sufficient to guarantee a valid estimate of risk in immigrant populations. In particular, immigrants might differ from their counterparts in the country of origin in terms of attitudes toward female genital cutting. Attitudes can differ because migrants represent a special sample of people from the country of origin or because immigrants acculturate after arriving in a new country. To examine these possibilities, we used a fully anonymous, computerized task to elicit implicit attitudes toward female genital cutting among Sudanese immigrants living in Switzerland and Sudanese people in Sudan. Results show that Sudanese immigrants in Switzerland were significantly more positive about uncut girls than Sudanese in Sudan, and that selective migration out of Sudan likely contributed substantially to this difference. We conclude by suggesting how our method could potentially be coupled with recent efforts to refine extrapolation methods for estimating cutting risk among immigrant populations. More broadly, our results highlight the need to better understand how heterogeneous attitudes can affect the risk of cutting among immigrant communities and in countries of origin.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. A neural link between generosity and happiness
- Author
-
Soyoung Q. Park, Thorsten Kahnt, Azade Dogan, Sabrina Strang, Ernst Fehr, and Philippe N. Tobler
- Subjects
Science - Abstract
Generous behaviour increases happiness, but the neural underpinnings of this link are unknown. Here, authors show that promising to be generous changes the neural response in the temporo-parietal junction, and that the connection between this region and the ventral striatum was related to happiness.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. A Common Mechanism Underlying Food Choice and Social Decisions.
- Author
-
Ian Krajbich, Todd Hare, Björn Bartling, Yosuke Morishima, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
People make numerous decisions every day including perceptual decisions such as walking through a crowd, decisions over primary rewards such as what to eat, and social decisions that require balancing own and others' benefits. The unifying principles behind choices in various domains are, however, still not well understood. Mathematical models that describe choice behavior in specific contexts have provided important insights into the computations that may underlie decision making in the brain. However, a critical and largely unanswered question is whether these models generalize from one choice context to another. Here we show that a model adapted from the perceptual decision-making domain and estimated on choices over food rewards accurately predicts choices and reaction times in four independent sets of subjects making social decisions. The robustness of the model across domains provides behavioral evidence for a common decision-making process in perceptual, primary reward, and social decision making.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Inferring on the intentions of others by hierarchical Bayesian learning.
- Author
-
Andreea O Diaconescu, Christoph Mathys, Lilian A E Weber, Jean Daunizeau, Lars Kasper, Ekaterina I Lomakina, Ernst Fehr, and Klaas E Stephan
- Subjects
Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Inferring on others' (potentially time-varying) intentions is a fundamental problem during many social transactions. To investigate the underlying mechanisms, we applied computational modeling to behavioral data from an economic game in which 16 pairs of volunteers (randomly assigned to "player" or "adviser" roles) interacted. The player performed a probabilistic reinforcement learning task, receiving information about a binary lottery from a visual pie chart. The adviser, who received more predictive information, issued an additional recommendation. Critically, the game was structured such that the adviser's incentives to provide helpful or misleading information varied in time. Using a meta-Bayesian modeling framework, we found that the players' behavior was best explained by the deployment of hierarchical learning: they inferred upon the volatility of the advisers' intentions in order to optimize their predictions about the validity of their advice. Beyond learning, volatility estimates also affected the trial-by-trial variability of decisions: participants were more likely to rely on their estimates of advice accuracy for making choices when they believed that the adviser's intentions were presently stable. Finally, our model of the players' inference predicted the players' interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) scores, explicit ratings of the advisers' helpfulness and the advisers' self-reports on their chosen strategy. Overall, our results suggest that humans (i) employ hierarchical generative models to infer on the changing intentions of others, (ii) use volatility estimates to inform decision-making in social interactions, and (iii) integrate estimates of advice accuracy with non-social sources of information. The Bayesian framework presented here can quantify individual differences in these mechanisms from simple behavioral readouts and may prove useful in future clinical studies of maladaptive social cognition.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Does general motivation energize financial reward-seeking behavior? Evidence from an effort task.
- Author
-
Justin Chumbley and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
We aimed to predict how hard subjects work for financial rewards from their general trait and state reward-motivation. We specifically asked 1) whether individuals high in general trait "reward responsiveness" work harder 2) whether task-irrelevant cues can make people work harder, by increasing general motivation. Each trial of our task contained a 1 second earning interval in which male subjects earned money for each button press. This was preceded by one of three predictive cues: an erotic picture of a woman, a man, or a geometric figure. We found that individuals high in trait "reward responsiveness" worked harder and earned more, irrespective of the predictive cue. Because female predictive cues are more rewarding, we expected them to increase general motivation in our male subjects and invigorate work, but found a more complex pattern.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Thesen zur Ökonomie und Biologie des Vertrauens
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Philosophy. Psychology. Religion - Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. No effects of psychosocial stress on intertemporal choice.
- Author
-
Johannes Haushofer, Sandra Cornelisse, Maayke Seinstra, Ernst Fehr, Marian Joëls, and Tobias Kalenscher
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Intertemporal choices - involving decisions which trade off instant and delayed outcomes - are often made under stress. It remains unknown, however, whether and how stress affects intertemporal choice. We subjected 142 healthy male subjects to a laboratory stress or control protocol, and asked them to make a series of intertemporal choices either directly after stress, or 20 minutes later (resulting in four experimental groups). Based on theory and evidence from behavioral economics and cellular neuroscience, we predicted a bidirectional effect of stress on intertemporal choice, with increases in impatience or present bias immediately after stress, but decreases in present bias or impatience when subjects are tested 20 minutes later. However, our results show no effects of stress on intertemporal choice at either time point, and individual differences in stress reactivity (changes in stress hormone levels over time) are not related to individual differences in intertemporal choice. Together, we did not find support for the hypothesis that psychosocial laboratory stressors affect intertemporal choice.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. DAT1 polymorphism determines L-DOPA effects on learning about others' prosociality.
- Author
-
Christoph Eisenegger, Andreas Pedroni, Jörg Rieskamp, Christian Zehnder, Richard Ebstein, Ernst Fehr, and Daria Knoch
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Despite that a wealth of evidence links striatal dopamine to individualś reward learning performance in non-social environments, the neurochemical underpinnings of such learning during social interaction are unknown. Here, we show that the administration of 300 mg of the dopamine precursor L-DOPA to 200 healthy male subjects influences learning about a partners' prosocial preferences in a novel social interaction task, which is akin to a repeated trust game. We found learning to be modulated by a well-established genetic marker of striatal dopamine levels, the 40-bp variable number tandem repeats polymorphism of the dopamine transporter (DAT1 polymorphism). In particular, we found that L-DOPA improves learning in 10/10R genoype subjects, who are assumed to have lower endogenous striatal dopamine levels and impairs learning in 9/10R genotype subjects, who are assumed to have higher endogenous dopamine levels. These findings provide first evidence for a critical role of dopamine in learning whether an interaction partner has a prosocial or a selfish personality. The applied pharmacogenetic approach may open doors to new ways of studying psychiatric disorders such as psychosis, which is characterized by distorted perceptions of others' prosocial attitudes.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. An incentive solution to the peer review problem.
- Author
-
Marc Hauser and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Teaching self-regulation
- Author
-
Daniel Schunk, Eva M. Berger, Henning Hermes, Kirsten Winkel, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Behavioral Neuroscience ,Social Psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology - Published
- 2022
14. A Nation-Wide Laboratory: Examining Trust and Trustworthiness by Integrating Behavioral Experiments into Representative Surveys
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr, Urs Fischbacher, Bernhard von Rosenbladt, J�rgen Schupp, and Gert G. Wagner
- Subjects
jel:C92 ,jel:C82 ,jel:C93 ,History ,Polymers and Plastics ,jel:D84 ,jel:C42 ,Experiment, Survey, Trust, Trustworthiness, Altruism ,jel:A13 ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering ,jel:J24 ,Experiment,Survey,Trust,Trustworthiness,Altruism ,Business and International Management ,General Economics, Econometrics and Finance ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) - Abstract
Typically, laboratory experiments suffer from homogeneous subject pools and self-selection biases. The usefulness of survey data is limited by measurement error and by the questionability of their behavioral relevance. Here we present a method integrating interactive experiments and representative surveys thereby overcoming crucial weaknesses of both approaches. One of the major advantages of our approach is that it allows for the integration of experiments, which require interaction among the participants, with a survey of non-interacting respondents in a smooth and inexpensive way. We illustrate the power of our approach with the analysis of trust and trustworthiness in Germany by combining representative survey data with representative behavioral data from a social dilemma experiment. We identify which survey questions intended to elicit people�s trust correlate well with behaviorally exhibited trust in the experiment. People above the age of 65, highly skilled workers and people living in bigger households exhibit less trusting behavior. Foreign citizens, Catholics and people favoring the Social Democratic Party or the Christian Democratic Party exhibit more trust. People above the age of 65 and those in good health behave more trustworthy or more altruistically, respectively. People below the age of 35, the unemployed and people who say they are in favor of none of the political parties behave less trustworthy or less altruistically, respectively.
- Published
- 2023
15. Can people detect the trustworthiness of strangers based on their facial appearance?
- Author
-
Bastian Jaeger, Bastiaan Oud, Tony Williams, Eva Krumhuber, Ernst Fehr, Jan Benjamin Engelmann, Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, Experimental and Political Economics / CREED (ASE, FEB), Department of Social Psychology, Organizational Psychology, and University of Zurich
- Subjects
Face perception ,FEATURES ,INFERENCES ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,FACES ,Trust ,computer.software_genre ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,10007 Department of Economics ,evolution ,TRUTH ,ddc:330 ,SOCIAL ATTRIBUTIONS ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Accuracy ,Trustworthiness ,business.industry ,JUDGE ,behavior and systematics ,COVER ,330 Economics ,Cooperation ,Facial appearance ,Artificial intelligence ,ecology ,Prediction ,Psychology ,business ,computer ,BEHAVIOR ,Natural language processing - Abstract
Although cooperation can lead to mutually beneficial outcomes, cooperative actions only pay off for the individual if others can be trusted to cooperate as well. Identifying trustworthy interaction partners is therefore a central challenge in human social life. How do people navigate this challenge? Prior work suggests that people rely on facial appearance to judge the trustworthiness of strangers. However, the question of whether these judgments are actually accurate remains debated. The present research examines accuracy in trustworthiness detection from faces and three moderators proposed by previous research. We investigate whether people show above-chance accuracy (a) when they make trust decisions and when they provide explicit trustworthiness ratings, (b) when judging male and female counterparts, and (c) when rating cropped images (with non-facial features removed) and uncropped images. Two studies showed that incentivized trust decisions (Study 1, n = 131 university students) and incentivized trustworthiness predictions (Study 2, n = 266 university students) were unrelated to the actual trustworthiness of counterparts. Accuracy was not moderated by stimulus type (cropped vs. uncropped faces) or counterparts' gender. Overall, these findings suggest that people are unable to detect the trustworthiness of strangers based on their facial appearance, when this is the only information available to them.
- Published
- 2022
16. Revealed preferences in a sequential prisoners’ dilemma: A horse-race between six utility functions
- Author
-
Topi Miettinen, Ernst Fehr, Michael Kosfeld, Jörgen W. Weibull, University of Zurich, and Miettinen, Topi
- Subjects
Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management ,Economics and Econometrics ,Salience (language) ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Control (management) ,2002 Economics and Econometrics ,Prisoner's dilemma ,1407 Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management ,Preference ,330 Economics ,Dilemma ,Optimism ,Action (philosophy) ,10007 Department of Economics ,0502 economics and business ,050207 economics ,Psychology ,Explanatory power ,Social psychology ,050205 econometrics ,media_common - Abstract
We experimentally investigate behavior and beliefs in a sequential prisoner’s dilemma. Each subject had to choose an action as first mover and a conditional action as second mover. All subjects also had to state their beliefs about others’ second-mover choices. Using these elicited beliefs, we apply the transparent Selten–Krischker approach to compare the explanatory power of a few current models of social and moral preferences. We find clear differences in explanatory power between the preference models, both without and with control for the number of free parameters. The best-performing models explain about 80% of the observed behavior. We compare our results with those obtained from a conventional maximum-likelihood approach, and find that the results by and large agree. We also present a structural model of belief formation. We find a consensus bias—whereby subjects believe others behave like themselves—and payoff-salience driven optimism—whereby subjects overestimate the probabilities for favorable outcomes.
- Published
- 2020
17. Dopaminergic D1 Receptor Stimulation Affects Effort and Risk Preferences
- Author
-
Philippe N. Tobler, Sridhar Duvvuri, Alexander Soutschek, Geraldine Gvozdanovic, Nicholas de Martinis, Rouba Kozak, David Gray, Alexander Jetter, Brian Harel, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Agonist ,business.industry ,medicine.drug_class ,Dopaminergic ,Stimulation ,Placebo ,Bioinformatics ,03 medical and health sciences ,030104 developmental biology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Dopamine receptor D1 ,Medicine ,Temporal discounting ,Biological psychiatry ,Receptor ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Biological Psychiatry - Abstract
Background Activation of D1 receptors has been related to successful goal-directed behavior, but it remains unclear whether D1 receptor activation causally tips the balance of weighing costs and benefits in humans. Here, we tested the impact of pharmacologically stimulated D1 receptors on sensitivity to risk, delay, and effort costs in economic choice and investigated whether D1 receptor stimulation would bias preferences toward options with increased costs in a cost-specific manner. Methods In a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group phase 1 study, 120 healthy young volunteers received either placebo or 1 of 3 doses (6 mg, 15 mg, or 30 mg) of a novel, selective D1 agonist (PF-06412562). After drug administration, participants performed decision tasks measuring their preferences for risky, delayed, and effortful outcomes. Results Higher doses of the D1 agonist increased the willingness to exert physical effort for reward as well as reduced the preference for risky outcomes. We observed no effects on preferences for delayed rewards. Conclusions The current results provide evidence that D1 receptor stimulation causally affects core aspects of cost-benefit decision making in humans.
- Published
- 2020
18. Super-Additive Cooperation
- Author
-
Charles Efferson, Helen Bernhard, Urs Fischbacher, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
History ,Polymers and Plastics ,Business and International Management ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering - Published
- 2022
19. A causal role for the right frontal eye fields in value comparison
- Author
-
Andres Mitsumasu, Christian C. Ruff, Ernst Fehr, Rafael Polania, Ian Krajbich, University of Zurich, and Krajbich, Ian
- Subjects
Male ,genetic structures ,medicine.medical_treatment ,0302 clinical medicine ,10007 Department of Economics ,2400 General Immunology and Microbiology ,Biology (General) ,Eye-Tracking Technology ,0303 health sciences ,General Neuroscience ,05 social sciences ,2800 General Neuroscience ,General Medicine ,Frontal eye fields ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,Frontal Lobe ,330 Economics ,Medicine ,Female ,Psychology ,Research Article ,Computational and Systems Biology ,Human ,Adult ,genetics and molecular biology ,QH301-705.5 ,Science ,general biochemistry ,brain stimulation ,eye tracking ,050105 experimental psychology ,decision making ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,03 medical and health sciences ,1300 General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,Reaction Time ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,030304 developmental biology ,Discounting ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,Mechanism (biology) ,drift diffusion model ,Gaze ,attention ,Transcranial magnetic stimulation ,frontal eye fields ,Brain stimulation ,Eye tracking ,Value (mathematics) ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Recent studies have suggested close functional links between overt visual attention and decision making. This suggests that the corresponding mechanisms may interface in brain regions known to be crucial for guiding visual attention – such as the frontal eye field (FEF). Here, we combined brain stimulation, eye tracking, and computational approaches to explore this possibility. We show that inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the right FEF has a causal impact on decision making, reducing the effect of gaze dwell time on choice while also increasing reaction times. We computationally characterize this putative mechanism by using the attentional drift diffusion model (aDDM), which reveals that FEF inhibition reduces the relative discounting of the non-fixated option in the comparison process. Our findings establish an important causal role of the right FEF in choice, elucidate the underlying mechanism, and provide support for one of the key causal hypotheses associated with the aDDM., eLife, 10, ISSN:2050-084X
- Published
- 2021
20. Teaching self-regulation
- Author
-
Daniel, Schunk, Eva M, Berger, Henning, Hermes, Kirsten, Winkel, and Ernst, Fehr
- Abstract
Children's self-regulation abilities are key predictors of educational success and other life outcomes such as income and health. However, self-regulation is not a school subject, and knowledge about how to generate lasting improvements in self-regulation and academic achievements with easily scalable, low-cost interventions is still limited. Here we report the results of a randomized controlled field study that integrates a short self-regulation teaching unit based on the concept of mental contrasting with implementation intentions into the school curriculum of first graders. We demonstrate that the treatment increases children's skills in terms of impulse control and self-regulation while also generating lasting improvements in academic skills such as reading and monitoring careless mistakes. Moreover, it has a substantial effect on children's long-term school career by increasing the likelihood of enroling in an advanced secondary school track three years later. Thus, self-regulation teaching can be integrated into the regular school curriculum at low cost, is easily scalable, and can substantially improve important abilities and children's educational career path.
- Published
- 2021
21. Author response: A causal role for the right frontal eye fields in value comparison
- Author
-
Rafael Polania, Ian Krajbich, Christian C. Ruff, Andres Mitsumasu, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Frontal eye fields ,Value (mathematics) ,Mathematics ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2021
22. Thesen zur Ökonomie und Biologie des Vertrauens
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr
- Published
- 2021
23. Testing models at the neural level reveals how the brain computes subjective value
- Author
-
Philippe N. Tobler, Kerstin Preuschoff, Ernst Fehr, Stephan Nebe, Tony Williams, and Christopher J. Burke
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Computer science ,Computation ,Decision Making ,Models, Neurological ,Theoretical models ,risk taking ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Models, Psychological ,Machine learning ,computer.software_genre ,Choice Behavior ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Humans ,physiological foundation of behavior ,neural valuation systems ,030304 developmental biology ,0303 health sciences ,Brain Mapping ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Brain ,Cognition ,Bayes Theorem ,Biological Sciences ,neuroeconomics ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Probability distribution ,Female ,Model implementation ,Artificial intelligence ,Neuroeconomics ,Risk taking ,business ,Value (mathematics) ,computer ,Perceptual Masking ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Significance In recent years, models have played an increasingly important role for understanding the brain in cognitive, behavioral, and systems neuroscience. Decision neuroscience in particular has benefitted greatly from the application of economic models of choice preferences to neural data. However, an often-overlooked aspect is that many models of preferences have a generic problem—they make extremely similar behavioral predictions. Here, we demonstrate that to understand the mechanisms of valuation in the brain, it is useful to compare models of choice preferences not only at the behavioral but also at the neural level., Decisions are based on the subjective values of choice options. However, subjective value is a theoretical construct and not directly observable. Strikingly, distinct theoretical models competing to explain how subjective values are assigned to choice options often make very similar behavioral predictions, which poses a major difficulty for establishing a mechanistic, biologically plausible explanation of decision-making based on behavior alone. Here, we demonstrate that model comparison at the neural level provides insights into model implementation during subjective value computation even though the distinct models parametrically identify common brain regions as computing subjective value. We show that frontal cortical regions implement a model based on the statistical distributions of available rewards, whereas intraparietal cortex and striatum compute subjective value signals according to a model based on distortions in the representations of probabilities. Thus, better mechanistic understanding of how cognitive processes are implemented arises from model comparisons at the neural level, over and above the traditional approach of comparing models at the behavioral level alone.
- Published
- 2021
24. The rise of affectivism
- Author
-
Agnes Moors, Joseph E. LeDoux, Jane W. Davidson, Antonio R. Damasio, Eran Halperin, Ad Foolen, Catherine Pelachaud, Brian Knutson, Mariska E. Kret, Patrik Vuilleumier, Zanna Clay, Terry A. Maroney, Andrew Beatty, Patricia Greenspan, Jan E. Stets, Jeanne L. Tsai, Carien M. van Reekum, Paula M. Niedenthal, Tim Wharton, Antony Stephen Reid Manstead, Kathryn Abrams, Susan Broomhall, George Loewenstein, Agneta H. Fischer, Tobias Brosch, Julien A. Deonna, James J. Gross, Beatrice de Gelder, Seth D. Pollak, Ralph Adolphs, Jonathan H. Turner, Justin D'Arms, Fabrice Teroni, James A. Russell, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, Klaus R. Scherer, Ernst Fehr, Gilles Pourtois, David Sander, Didier Maurice Grandjean, Mohammed Emtiaz Ahmed, Paul Ekman, Peter N. Stearns, Jonathan Gratch, Joseph J. Campos, Kent C. Berridge, Fabrice Clément, Jennifer S. Lerner, Leslie S. Greenberg, David Konstan, Daniel Dukes, Christine Tappolet, Dacher Keltner, Andrea Scarantino, Ute Frevert, Hanna Damasio, Ioannis Pavlidis, William A. Cunningham, Brian Parkinson, Birgitt Roettger-Roessler, Arvid Kappas, Ronnie de Sousa, Disa Sauter, Robert W. Levenson, RS: FSE BISS, Emotion, RS: FPN CN 10, Pelachaud, Catherine, Amsterdam Interdisciplinary Centre for Emotion (AICE, Psychology, FMG), Brain and Cognition, FMG, Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), University of Geneva [Switzerland], Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), and Sorbonne Université (SU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)
- Subjects
cognition ,Consensus ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Social Sciences ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Behaviorism ,Affect (psychology) ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cognition ,Psychological Theory ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Humans ,[INFO.INFO-HC]Computer Science [cs]/Human-Computer Interaction [cs.HC] ,030304 developmental biology ,media_common ,0303 health sciences ,Behavior ,affectivism ,behaviour ,Affect ,Feeling ,[INFO.INFO-HC] Computer Science [cs]/Human-Computer Interaction [cs.HC] ,Explanatory power ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Research over the past decades has demonstrated the explanatory power of emotions, feelings, motivations, moods, and other affective processes when trying to understand and predict how we think and behave. In this consensus article, we ask: has the increasingly recognized impact of affective phenomena ushered in a new era, the era of affectivism? ispartof: Nature Human Behaviour vol:5 issue:7 pages:816-820 ispartof: location:England status: published
- Published
- 2021
25. The Complementary Nature of Trust and Contract Enforcement
- Author
-
Björn Bartling, Ernst Fehr, David Huffman, and Nick Netzer
- Subjects
History ,Polymers and Plastics ,Business and International Management ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering - Published
- 2021
26. Other-regarding preferences and redistributive politics
- Author
-
Julien Senn, Thomas Epper, Ernst Fehr, University of Zurich, Lille économie management - UMR 9221 (LEM), Université d'Artois (UA)-Université catholique de Lille (UCL)-Université de Lille-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), and IÉSEG School Of Management [Puteaux]
- Subjects
History ,JEL: D - Microeconomics/D.D7 - Analysis of Collective Decision-Making/D.D7.D72 - Political Processes: Rent-Seeking, Lobbying, Elections, Legislatures, and Voting Behavior ,Polymers and Plastics ,Inequality ,H23 ,media_common.quotation_subject ,H24 ,Population ,preference heterogeneity ,Social preferences ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering ,JEL: H - Public Economics/H.H2 - Taxation, Subsidies, and Revenue/H.H2.H24 - Personal Income and Other Nonbusiness Taxes and Subsidies ,ECON Department of Economics ,D72 ,Aversion ,10007 Department of Economics ,Political agenda ,Voting ,0502 economics and business ,demand for redistribution ,Schweiz ,Economics ,ddc:330 ,050207 economics ,Business and International Management ,10. No inequality ,education ,D31 ,050205 econometrics ,media_common ,Soziale Ungleichheit ,education.field_of_study ,Public economics ,JEL: H - Public Economics/H.H2 - Taxation, Subsidies, and Revenue/H.H2.H23 - Externalities • Redistributive Effects • Environmental Taxes and Subsidies ,05 social sciences ,1. No poverty ,Direct democracy ,Redistribution (cultural anthropology) ,[SHS.ECO]Humanities and Social Sciences/Economics and Finance ,Altruismus ,JEL: D - Microeconomics/D.D3 - Distribution/D.D3.D31 - Personal Income, Wealth, and Their Distributions ,330 Economics ,altruism ,8. Economic growth ,inequality aversion ,Politisches Verhalten ,Inequity aversion - Abstract
Increasing inequality and associated egalitarian sentiments have again put redistribution on the political agenda. Support for redistribution may also be affected by altruistic and egalitarian preferences, but knowledge about the distribution of these preferences in the broader population and how they relate to political support for redistributive policies is still scarce. In this paper, we take advantage of Swiss direct democracy, where people voted several times in national plebiscites on strongly redistributive policies, to study the link between other-regarding preferences and support for redistribution in a broad sample of the Swiss population. Based on a recently developed non-parametric clustering procedure, we identify three disjunct groups of individuals with fundamentally different other-regarding preferences: (i) a large share of inequality averse people, (ii) a somewhat smaller yet still large share of people with an altruistic concern for social welfare and the worse off, and (iii) a considerable minority of primarily selfish individuals. Controlling for a large number of determinants of support for redistribution, we document that inequality aversion and altruistic concerns play an important role for redistributive voting that is particularly pronounced for above-median income earners. However, the role of these motives differs depending on the nature of redistributive proposals. Inequality aversion has large and robust effects in plebiscites that demand income reductions for the rich, while altruistic concerns play no significant role in these plebiscites.
- Published
- 2021
27. Intra-individual variability in task performance after cognitive training is associated with long-term outcomes in children
- Author
-
Henning Hermes, Daniel Schunk, Ernst Fehr, Todd A. Hare, A. Cubillo, Kirsten Winkel, and Eva M. Berger
- Subjects
Working memory training ,Socioemotional selectivity theory ,Psychological intervention ,Brain Structure and Function ,Psychology ,Mental health ,Cognitive training ,Cognitive psychology ,Task (project management) ,Term (time) - Abstract
The benefits and mechanistic effects of working memory training in children are the subject of much research and debate. The cumulative evidence indicates that training can alter brain structure and function in the short term and have lasting effects on behaviour. We show that five weeks of working memory training led to greater activity in prefrontal and striatal brain regions, better accuracy, and reduced intra-individual variability in response times. The reduction in intra-individual variability can be explained by changes to the evidence accumulation rates and thresholds in a sequential sampling decision model. Critically, intra-individual variability was more closely associated with academic skills and mental health 6-12 months after the end of training than task accuracy. These results indicate that intra-individual variability may be a useful way to quantify the immediate impact of cognitive training interventions and predict the future emergence of academic and socioemotional skills.
- Published
- 2020
28. Behavioral constraints on the design of subgame-perfect implementation mechanisms
- Author
-
Michael Powell, Tom Wilkening, Ernst Fehr, and University of Zurich
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Incomplete Contracts ,Implementation Theory ,Verhaltensökonomie ,asymmetrische Information ,Subgame perfect equilibrium ,ECON Department of Economics ,D71 ,10007 Department of Economics ,0502 economics and business ,Economics ,ddc:330 ,D91 ,C92 ,Incomplete contracts ,050207 economics ,incomplete contracts ,050205 econometrics ,Implementation theory ,experimentelle Spieltheorie ,Mechanism (biology) ,05 social sciences ,Behavioral pattern ,experiments ,Reciprocity (evolution) ,experimentelle Wirtschaftsforschung ,330 Economics ,D82 ,Risk analysis (engineering) ,Arbitration ,D23 ,Experiments ,Lying ,D86 ,D44 - Abstract
We study subgame-perfect implementation (SPI) mechanisms that have been proposed as a solution to incomplete contracting problems. We show that these mechanisms, which are based on off-equilibrium arbitration clauses that impose large fines for lying and the inappropriate use of arbitration, have severe behavioral constraints because the fines induce retaliation against legitimate uses of arbitration. Incorporating reciprocity preferences into the theory explains the observed behavioral patterns and helps us develop a new mechanism that is more robust and achieves high rates of truth-telling and efficiency. Our results highlight the importance of tailoring implementation mechanisms to the underlying behavioral environment. (JEL C92, D44, D82, D86, D91)
- Published
- 2020
29. Motivated misremembering of selfish decisions
- Author
-
Molly J. Crockett, Ernst Fehr, Michel André Maréchal, Ryan W. Carlson, Bastiaan Oud, University of Zurich, and Carlson, Ryan W
- Subjects
Male ,Decision ,General Physics and Astronomy ,Choice Behavior ,generosity ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology|Moral Behavior ,memory ,0302 clinical medicine ,10007 Department of Economics ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology ,lcsh:Science ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology|Motivational Behavior ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,3100 General Physics and Astronomy ,Test (assessment) ,330 Economics ,Personal gain ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Social Psychology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Personality and Social Contexts ,Female ,Social psychology ,Generosity ,Science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology|Individual Differences ,1600 General Chemistry ,Genetics and Molecular Biology ,050105 experimental psychology ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,motivation ,making ,1300 General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,Human behaviour ,Humans ,Selfishness ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Behavior ,Recall ,General Chemistry ,morality ,Morality ,Self Concept ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Mental Recall ,General Biochemistry ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,lcsh:Q ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Hindsight bias - Abstract
People often prioritize their own interests, but also like to see themselves as moral. How do individuals resolve this tension? One way to both pursue personal gain and preserve a moral self-image is to misremember the extent of one’s selfishness. Here, we test this possibility. Across five experiments (N = 3190), we find that people tend to recall being more generous in the past than they actually were, even when they are incentivized to recall their decisions accurately. Crucially, this motivated misremembering effect occurs chiefly for individuals whose choices violate their own fairness standards, irrespective of how high or low those standards are. Moreover, this effect disappears under conditions where people no longer perceive themselves as responsible for their fairness violations. Together, these findings suggest that when people’s actions fall short of their personal standards, they may misremember the extent of their selfishness, thereby potentially warding off threats to their moral self-image., People often prioritize their own interests, but also like to see themselves as moral. Here the authors show how distortions in memory might resolve this tension by demonstrating that people tend to remember being more generous in the past than they actually were.
- Published
- 2020
30. Activation of D1 receptors affects human reactivity and flexibility to valued cues
- Author
-
Nicholas de Martinis, Philippe N. Tobler, Ernst Fehr, Rouba Kozak, William M. Howe, Christopher J. Burke, Alexander Jetter, Alexander Soutschek, University of Zurich, and Soutschek, Alexander
- Subjects
Agonist ,Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,medicine.drug_class ,Reversal Learning ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,Young Adult ,2738 Psychiatry and Mental Health ,0302 clinical medicine ,Dopamine receptor D1 ,Double-Blind Method ,Reward ,Dopamine ,10007 Department of Economics ,Conditioning, Psychological ,medicine ,Humans ,Reactivity (psychology) ,Neurotransmitter ,Pharmacology ,Motivation ,Working memory ,Receptors, Dopamine D1 ,Flexibility (personality) ,030227 psychiatry ,330 Economics ,psychiatry and mental health ,3004 Pharmacology ,chemistry ,Cue reactivity ,10199 Clinic for Clinical Pharmacology and Toxicology ,Dopamine Agonists ,Female ,Cues ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Reward-predicting cues motivate goal-directed behavior, but in unstable environments humans must also be able to flexibly update cue-reward associations. While the capacity of reward cues to trigger motivation ('reactivity') as well as flexibility in cue-reward associations have been linked to the neurotransmitter dopamine in humans, the specific contribution of the dopamine D1 receptor family to these behaviors remained elusive. To fill this gap, we conducted a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind pharmacological study testing the impact of three different doses of a novel D1 agonist (relative to placebo) on reactivity to reward-predicting cues (Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer) and flexibility of cue-outcome associations (reversal learning). We observed that the impact of the D1 agonist crucially depended on baseline working memory functioning, which has been identified as a proxy for baseline dopamine synthesis capacity. Specifically, increasing D1 receptor stimulation strengthened Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer in individuals with high baseline working memory capacity. In contrast, higher doses of the D1 agonist improved reversal learning only in individuals with low baseline working memory functioning. Our findings suggest a crucial and baseline-dependent role of D1 receptor activation in controlling both cue reactivity and the flexibility of cue-reward associations.
- Published
- 2020
31. A registered replication study on oxytocin and trust
- Author
-
Christophe Boone, Loren Pauwels, Bodo Vogt, Ernst Fehr, Carolyn H. Declerck, University of Zurich, and Fehr, Ernst
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Social Psychology ,Social contact ,Post hoc ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Oxytocin ,Trust ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Dictator game ,Double-Blind Method ,10007 Department of Economics ,2802 Behavioral Neuroscience ,Humans ,Psychology ,Administration, Intranasal ,030304 developmental biology ,0303 health sciences ,3207 Social Psychology ,3205 Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Reproducibility of Results ,Disposition ,Replication (computing) ,330 Economics ,Games, Experimental ,Human medicine ,Social psychology ,Engineering sciences. Technology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
An influential 2005 study by Kosfeld et al. suggested that oxytocin increases trust in strangers. This registered replication study by some of the original authors found no effect of oxytocin on trusting behaviour under the same conditions. In an influential paper, Kosfeld et al. (2005) showed that intranasal administration of oxytocin (OT) increases the transfers made by investors in the trust game-suggesting that OT increases trust in strangers. Subsequent studies investigating the role of OT in the trust game found inconclusive effects on the trusting behaviour of investors but these studies deviated from the Kosfeld et al. study in an important way-they did not implement minimal social contact (MSC) between the investors and the trustees in the trust game. Here, we performed a large double-blind and placebo-controlled replication study of the effects of OT on trusting behaviour that yields a power of more than 95% and implements an MSC condition as well as a no-social-contact (NoC) condition. We find no effect of OT on trusting behaviour in the MSC condition. Exploratory post hoc analyses suggest that OT may increase trust in individuals with a low disposition to trust in the NoC condition, but this finding requires confirmation in future research.
- Published
- 2020
32. Does Market Interaction Erode Moral Values?
- Author
-
Björn Bartling, Yagiz Özdemir, Ernst Fehr, and University of Zurich
- Subjects
05 social sciences ,market interaction ,330 Economics ,social sciences (miscellaneous) ,ECON Department of Economics ,Side effect (computer science) ,D62 ,10007 Department of Economics ,Market interaction ,0502 economics and business ,C91 ,Economics ,D91 ,ddc:330 ,Treatment effect ,D02 ,050207 economics ,Positive economics ,Economics and econometrics ,D63 ,moral values ,050205 econometrics - Abstract
The widespread use of markets leads to unprecedented material well-being in many societies. We study whether market interaction, as a side effect, erodes moral values. An encompassing understanding of the virtues and vices of markets, including their possible impact on moral values, is necessary to make informed decisions on the spheres in society where the allocation and incentive functions of markets should exercise their power, and where this may not be desirable. In a seminal and highly influential paper, Falk and Szech (2013) provide experimental data that seem to suggest that "market interaction erodes moral values." Although we replicate their main treatment effect, we show that additional treatments are necessary to corroborate their conclusion. These treatments, however, reveal that repeated play and not market interaction causes the erosion of moral values. Our paper thus shows that neither Falk and Szech's data nor our data support the claim that market interaction erodes moral values.
- Published
- 2020
33. The evolution of distorted beliefs vs. mistaken choices under asymmetric error costs
- Author
-
Ryan McKay, Charles Efferson, and Ernst Fehr
- Subjects
Cultural Studies ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Cognition ,Social learning ,Affect (psychology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Cognitive bias ,Outcome (probability) ,Incentive ,Anthropology ,Agency (sociology) ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Herding ,Psychology ,Applied Psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Why do people sometimes hold unjustified beliefs and make harmful choices? Three hypotheses include (a) contemporary incentives in which some errors cost more than others, (b) cognitive biases evolved to manage ancestral incentives with variation in error costs and (c) social learning based on choice frequencies. With both modelling and a behavioural experiment, we examined all three mechanisms. The model and experiment support the conclusion that contemporary cost asymmetries affect choices by increasing the rate of cheap errors to reduce the rate of expensive errors. Our model shows that a cognitive bias can distort the evolution of beliefs and in turn behaviour. Unless the bias is strong, however, beliefs often evolve in the correct direction. This suggests limitations on how cognitive biases shape choices, which further indicates that detecting the behavioural consequences of biased cognition may sometimes be challenging. Our experiment used a prime intended to activate a bias called ‘hyperactive agency detection’, and the prime had no detectable effect on choices. Finally, both the model and experiment show that frequency-dependent social learning can generate choice dynamics in which some populations converge on widespread errors, but this outcome hinges on the other two mechanisms being neutral with respect to choice.
- Published
- 2020
34. The Impact of Working Memory Training on Children's Cognitive and Noncognitive Skills
- Author
-
Eva M. Berger, Ernst Fehr, Henning Hermes, Daniel Schunk, and Kirsten Winkel
- Published
- 2020
35. Does Market Interaction Erode Moral Values?
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr, Björn Bartling, and Yagiz Özdemir
- Subjects
Side effect (computer science) ,Economics ,Treatment effect ,Positive economics - Abstract
The widespread use of markets leads to unprecedented material well-being in many societies. We study whether market interaction, as a side effect, erodes moral values. In an influential paper, Falk and Szech (2013) provide experimental data that seem to suggest that “market interaction erodes moral values.” Although we replicate their main treatment effect, we show that additional treatments are necessary to corroborate their conclusion. These treatments reveal that playing repeatedly, and not market interaction, causes the erosion of moral values. Our paper thus shows that neither Falk and Szech’s data nor our data support the claim that markets erode morals.
- Published
- 2020
36. The promise and the peril of using social influence to reverse harmful traditions
- Author
-
Sonja Vogt, Charles Efferson, Ernst Fehr, University of Zurich, and Efferson, Charles
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Cultural identity ,Adult ,Circumcision, Female/psychology ,Culture ,Decision Making ,Female ,Humans ,Models, Psychological ,Social Conformity ,Social Identification ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Identity (social science) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,psychology ,Conformity ,social policy ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Collective identity ,10007 Department of Economics ,2802 Behavioral Neuroscience ,Positive economics ,education ,030304 developmental biology ,Social influence ,Social policy ,media_common ,0303 health sciences ,education.field_of_study ,3207 Social Psychology ,sociology ,3205 Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,economics ,330 Economics ,Intervention (law) ,Anthropology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
For a policy-maker promoting the end of a harmful tradition, conformist social influence is a compelling mechanism. If an intervention convinces enough people to abandon the tradition, this can spill over and induce others to follow. A key objective is thus to activate such spillovers and amplify an intervention’s effects. With female genital cutting as a motivating example, we develop empirically informed analytical and simulation models to examine this idea. Even if conformity pervades decision-making, spillovers can range from irrelevant to indispensable. Our analysis highlights three considerations. First, ordinary forms of individual heterogeneity can severely limit spillovers, and understanding the heterogeneity in a population is essential. Second, although interventions often target samples of the population biased towards ending the harmful tradition, targeting a representative sample is a more robust way to achieve spillovers. Finally, if the harmful tradition contributes to group identity, the success of spillovers can depend critically on disrupting the link between identity and tradition. Efferson et al. use models to examine the reversal of traditions such as female genital cutting. They find that interventions should avoid targeting agents amenable to change and should disrupt any link between cultural identity and traditional practice.
- Published
- 2020
37. The Impact of Working Memory Training on Children’s Cognitive and Noncognitive Skills
- Author
-
Kirsten Winkel, Daniel Schunk, Henning Hermes, Ernst Fehr, Eva M. Berger, University of Zurich, and Berger, Eva M
- Subjects
Working memory training ,Samfunnsvitenskap ,Earnings ,Working memory ,media_common.quotation_subject ,education ,J24 ,Cognition ,Affect (psychology) ,noncognitive skills ,330 Economics ,Developmental psychology ,ECON Department of Economics ,Working Memory, Education ,cognitive skills ,10007 Department of Economics ,Reading (process) ,Intervention (counseling) ,Human capital ,I20 ,Cognitive skill ,I21 ,working memory training ,media_common - Abstract
Working memory capacity is thought to play an important role for a wide range of cognitive and noncognitive skills such as fluid intelligence, math, reading, the inhibition of pre-potent impulses or more general self-regulation abilities. Because these abilities substantially affect individuals' life trajectories in terms of health, education, and earnings, the question of whether working memory (WM) training can improve them is of considerable importance. However, whether WM training leads to improvements in these far-transfer skills is contested. Here, we examine the causal impact of WM training embedded in regular school teaching by a randomized educational intervention involving a sample of 6–7 years old first graders. We find substantial immediate and lasting gains in working memory capacity. In addition, we document relatively large positive effects on geometry skills, reading skills, Raven's fluid IQ measure, the ability to inhibit pre-potent impulses and self-regulation abilities. Moreover, these far-transfer effects emerge over time and only become fully visible after 12-13 months. Finally, we document that 3–4 years after the intervention, the children who received training have a roughly 16 percentage points higher probability of entering the academic track in secondary school.
- Published
- 2020
38. 認知症に脳の炎症の影
- Author
-
Charles Efferson and Ernst Fehr
- Published
- 2018
39. Reply to Schild et al.: Antisocial personality moderates the causal influence of costly punishment on trust and trustworthiness
- Author
-
Carsten K. W. De Dreu, Ernst Fehr, Jan B. Engelmann, Basil Schmid, Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, and Experimental and Political Economics / CREED (ASE, FEB)
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,Punishment ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Context (language use) ,Social dilemma ,Antisocial Personality Disorder ,Deception ,Personality psychology ,Behavioral economics ,Trust ,050105 experimental psychology ,Dictator game ,Games, Experimental ,Personality ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Letters ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
A growing literature at the intersection of personality psychology and behavioral economics investigates the interplay between personality and decision making in social dilemmas (1, 2). Engelmann et al. (3) extend prior research in this area by investigating the role of antisocial personality in the context of a trust game with and without punishment. Previous research suggested that the possibility to punish individuals who fail to reciprocate trust causes an increase in trust and trustworthiness (4, 5) but remained silent about how antisocial personality affects the causal influence of punishment on trust and trustworthiness. Some older work in personality psychology reviewed in ref. 6 relied on deception and used hypothetical payments and scenarios. This methodological approach … [↵][1]1To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: j.b.engelmann{at}uva.nl. [1]: #xref-corresp-1-1
- Published
- 2020
40. Selective participation may undermine replication attempts
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr, Michel André Maréchal, Alain Cohn, University of Zurich, and Cohn, Alain
- Subjects
1000 Multidisciplinary ,Multidisciplinary ,Truth Disclosure ,Decision Making ,MEDLINE ,Commerce ,Organizational culture ,Reproducibility of Results ,Organizational Culture ,Replication (computing) ,330 Economics ,10007 Department of Economics ,Humans ,Business ,Social psychology - Published
- 2019
41. The many faces of human sociality: uncovering the distribution and stability of social preferences
- Author
-
Daniel Schunk, Adrian Bruhin, Ernst Fehr, and University of Zurich
- Subjects
2000 General Economics, Econometrics and Finance ,individual behavior ,Verhaltensökonomie ,Social preferences ,ECON Department of Economics ,Entscheidungsfindung ,10007 Department of Economics ,0502 economics and business ,C91 ,Economics ,Econometrics ,Heterogenität ,ddc:330 ,Heterogeneity ,Stability ,Finite mixture models ,050207 economics ,Strukturmodell ,Preference (economics) ,Sociality ,050205 econometrics ,finite mixture models ,05 social sciences ,Stochastic game ,Behavioral microeconomics (underlying principles) ,Representative agent ,stability ,Präferenz ,Reciprocity (evolution) ,Altruismus ,330 Economics ,Predictive power ,D03 ,C49 ,heterogeneity ,General Economics, Econometrics and Finance ,Value (mathematics) ,laboratory - Abstract
There is vast heterogeneity in the human willingness to weigh others' interests in decision making. This heterogeneity concerns the motivational intricacies as well as the strength of other-regarding behaviors, and raises the question how one can parsimoniously model and characterize heterogeneity across several dimensions of social preferences while still being able to predict behavior over time and across situations. We tackle this task with an experiment and a structural model of preferences that allows us to simultaneously estimate outcome-based and reciprocity-based social preferences. We find that non-selfish preferences are the rule rather than the exception. Neither at the level of the representative agent nor when we allow for several preference types do purely selfish types emerge. Instead, three temporally stable and qualitatively different other-regarding types emerge endogenously, i.e., without pre-specifying assumptions about the characteristics of types. When ahead, all three types value others' payoffs significantly more than when behind. The first type, which we denote as strongly altruistic type, is characterized by a relatively large weight on others' payoffs - even when behind - and moderate levels of reciprocity. The second type, denoted as moderately altruistic type, also puts positive weight on others' payoff, yet at a considerable lower level, and displays no positive reciprocity while the third type is behindness averse, i.e., puts a large negative weight on others' payoffs when behind and behaves selfishly otherwise. We also find that there is an unambiguous and temporally stable assignment of individuals to types. Moreover, the three-type model substantially improves the (out-of-sample) predictions of individuals' behavior across additional games while the information contained in subject-specific parameter estimates leads to no or only minor additional predictive power. This suggests that a parsimonious model with three types captures the bulk of the predictive power contained in the preference estimates.
- Published
- 2019
42. On the psychology and economics of antisocial personality
- Author
-
Jan B. Engelmann, Justin R. Chumbley, Ernst Fehr, C.K.W. de Dreu, Basil Schmid, University of Zurich, Engelmann, Jan B, Microeconomics (ASE, FEB), Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, and Experimental and Political Economics / CREED (ASE, FEB)
- Subjects
Youth Violence ,punishment ,Punishment (psychology) ,Economics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Culture ,Social Sciences ,Context (language use) ,Trust ,Antisocial ,Personality ,Punishment ,Person situation ,person situation ,Experimental Psychopathology and Treatment ,Experimental ,Social ,10007 Department of Economics ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Humans ,Psychology ,Antisocial personality ,Big Five personality traits ,Behavioral ,media_common ,Pediatric ,Violence Research ,1000 Multidisciplinary ,Multidisciplinary ,trust ,Antisocial Personality Disorder ,antisocial ,330 Economics ,Trustworthiness ,personality ,Honor ,Games ,Social psychology - Abstract
How do fundamental concepts from economics, such as individuals’ preferences and beliefs, relate to equally fundamental concepts from psychology, such as relatively stable personality traits? Can personality traits help us better understand economic behavior across strategic contexts? We identify an antisocial personality profile and examine the role of strategic context (the “situation”), personality traits (the “person”), and their interaction on beliefs and behaviors in trust games. Antisocial individuals exhibit a specific combination of beliefs and preferences that is difficult to reconcile with a rational choice approach that assumes that beliefs about others’ behaviors are formed rationally and therefore, independently from preferences. Variations in antisocial personality are associated with effect sizes that are as large as strong variations in strategic context. Antisocial individuals have lower trust in others unless they know that they can punish them. They are also substantially less trustworthy, believe that others are like themselves, and respond to the possibility of being sanctioned more strongly, suggesting that they anticipate severe punishment if they betray their partner’s trust. Antisocial individuals are not simply acting in their economic self-interest, because they harshly punish those who do not reciprocate their trust, although that reduces their economic payoff, and they do so nonimpulsively and in a very calculated manner. Antisocial individuals honor others’ trust significantly less (if they cannot be punished) but also, harshly punish those who betray their trust. Overall, it seems that antisocial individuals have beliefs and behaviors based on a view of the world that assumes that most others are as antisocial as they themselves are., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116 (26), ISSN:0027-8424, ISSN:1091-6490
- Published
- 2019
43. Dopaminergic D
- Author
-
Alexander, Soutschek, Geraldine, Gvozdanovic, Rouba, Kozak, Sridhar, Duvvuri, Nicholas, de Martinis, Brian, Harel, David L, Gray, Ernst, Fehr, Alexander, Jetter, and Philippe N, Tobler
- Subjects
Reward ,Dopamine ,Receptors, Dopamine D1 ,Decision Making ,Dopamine Antagonists ,Humans - Abstract
Activation of DIn a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group phase 1 study, 120 healthy young volunteers received either placebo or 1 of 3 doses (6 mg, 15 mg, or 30 mg) of a novel, selective DHigher doses of the DThe current results provide evidence that D
- Published
- 2019
44. The neural circuitry of affect-induced distortions of trust
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr, Friederike Meyer, Jan B. Engelmann, Christian C. Ruff, University of Zurich, Engelmann, Jan B, Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, and Experimental and Political Economics / CREED (ASE, FEB)
- Subjects
Male ,Economics ,ComputingMilieux_LEGALASPECTSOFCOMPUTING ,0302 clinical medicine ,10007 Department of Economics ,Parietal Lobe ,Research Articles ,Posterior superior temporal sulcus ,Multidisciplinary ,Functional connectivity ,05 social sciences ,SciAdv r-articles ,Shock ,Amygdala ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Temporal Lobe ,330 Economics ,Mental Health ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neurological ,Female ,Psychology ,Research Article ,Adult ,Adolescent ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Data_MISCELLANEOUS ,Decision Making ,Temporoparietal junction ,Stress ,Trust ,Affect (psychology) ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Clinical Research ,Underpinning research ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Connectome ,medicine ,Biological neural network ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Investments ,3101 Physics and Astronomy (miscellaneous) ,Association (psychology) ,1000 Multidisciplinary ,Neurosciences ,ComputingMilieux_PERSONALCOMPUTING ,Electric Stimulation ,Affect ,Psychological ,Nerve Net ,Neuroscience ,Stress, Psychological ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Incidental aversive affect distorts trust decisions and changes activity and connectivity in social cognitive neural circuitry., Aversive affect is likely a key source of irrational human decision-making, but still, little is known about the neural circuitry underlying emotion-cognition interactions during social behavior. We induced incidental aversive affect via prolonged periods of threat of shock, while 41 healthy participants made investment decisions concerning another person or a lottery. Negative affect reduced trust, suppressed trust-specific activity in the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ), and reduced functional connectivity between the TPJ and emotion-related regions such as the amygdala. The posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) seems to play a key role in mediating the impact of affect on behavior: Functional connectivity of this brain area with left TPJ was associated with trust in the absence of negative affect, but aversive affect disrupted this association between TPJ-pSTS connectivity and behavioral trust. Our findings may be useful for a better understanding of the neural circuitry of affective distortions in healthy and pathological populations.
- Published
- 2019
45. Time Discounting and Wealth Inequality
- Author
-
Helga Fehr-Duda, David Dreyer Lassen, Claus Thustrup Kreiner, Gregers Nytoft Rasmussen, Ernst Fehr, Søren Leth-Petersen, and Thomas Epper
- Subjects
Inequality ,Demographics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Patience ,language.human_language ,Large sample ,Danish ,language ,Economics ,Demographic economics ,Wealth distribution ,Time preference ,Association (psychology) ,media_common - Abstract
This paper documents a large association between individuals’ time discounting in incentivized experiments and their positions in the real-life wealth distribution derived from Danish high-quality administrative data for a large sample of middle-aged individuals. The association is stable over time, exists through the wealth distribution and remains large after controlling for education, income profile, school grades, initial wealth, parental wealth, credit constraints, demographics, risk preferences and additional behavioral parameters. Our results suggest that savings behavior is a driver of the observed association between patience and wealth inequality as predicted by standard savings theory.
- Published
- 2019
46. The brain's functional network architecture reveals human motives
- Author
-
Susanne Leiberg, Yosuke Morishima, Ernst Fehr, Grit Hein, and Sunhae Sul
- Subjects
Nerve net ,media_common.quotation_subject ,300 Social sciences, sociology & anthropology ,Empathy ,Human behavior ,Altruism ,Gyrus Cinguli ,050105 experimental psychology ,Functional networks ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social Behavior ,610 Medicine & health ,Anterior cingulate cortex ,media_common ,Cerebral Cortex ,Motivation ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Ventral striatum ,Reciprocity (evolution) ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Female ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Brain activity shows underlying motives In humans, two completely different motives may nevertheless lead to exactly the same behavior. Because we can't directly observe motives, modern economists often completely disregard them. However, Hein et al. , using fMRI, show that different human motives can yield observable responses in the brain (see the Perspective by Gluth and Fontanesi). In empathy-based and reciprocity-based altruistic behavior, the direction and the strength of functional connectivity between specific brain regions were different for each motive. Moreover, the connectivity patterns were independent of the behavioral implications of the motives. Science , this issue p. 1074 ; see also p. 1028
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Behavioral foundations of corporate culture
- Author
-
Ernst Fehr, University of Zurich, and Fehr, Ernst
- Subjects
Value (ethics) ,Incentive ,10007 Department of Economics ,Organizational culture ,11198 UBS Center for Economics in Society ,Business ,Positive economics ,UBSCPPS UBS Center Public Paper Series ,Behavioral economics ,330 Economics - Abstract
Talking about corporate culture has become quite popular in the business world. But why should companies care about corporate culture at all? Why do “soft” concepts like culture matter? Can’t companies simply rely on “hard” facts – the value of clear and efficient institutional rules and incentives? In this Public Paper, we argue that corporate culture is important because human behavior is always co-determined by the prevailing social norms. It is in the company’s interest to shape these norms through a cooperative culture that mobilizes employees’ voluntary cooperation in the pursuit of the firm’s performance goals. Our research provides behavioral foundations for cooperative cultures, based on important scientific insights from contract economics as well as from experimental and behavioral economics.
- Published
- 2018
48. Normative foundations of human cooperation
- Author
-
Ivo Schurtenberger, Ernst Fehr, University of Zurich, and Fehr, Ernst
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Experimental psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Behavioural sciences ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Social preferences ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,10007 Department of Economics ,0502 economics and business ,2802 Behavioral Neuroscience ,Social Norms ,Humans ,Cooperative Behavior ,050207 economics ,Positive economics ,Social Behavior ,media_common ,3207 Social Psychology ,3205 Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,05 social sciences ,Causal effect ,Social Control, Formal ,330 Economics ,Normative ,Norm (social) ,Psychology ,Welfare ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Social control - Abstract
A large literature shares the view that social norms shape human cooperation, but without a clean empirical identification of the relevant norms almost every behaviour can be rationalized as norm driven, thus rendering norms useless as an explanatory construct. This raises the question of whether social norms are indeed causal drivers of behaviour and can convincingly explain major cooperation-related regularities. Here, we show that the norm of conditional cooperation provides such an explanation, that powerful methods for its empirical identification exist and that social norms have causal effects. Norm compliance rests on fundamental human motives (‘social preferences’) that also imply a willingness to punish free-riders, but normative constraints on peer punishment are important for its effectiveness and welfare properties. If given the chance, a large majority of people favour the imposition of such constraints through the migration to institutional environments that enable the normative guidance of cooperation and norm enforcement behaviours. Fehr and Schurtenberger show that the prevailing evidence supports the view that social norms are causal drivers of human cooperation and explain major cooperation-related regularities. Norms also guide peer punishment and people have strong preferences for institutions that support norm formation.
- Published
- 2018
49. Simple moral code supports cooperation
- Author
-
Charles Efferson, Ernst Fehr, and University of Zurich
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,1000 Multidisciplinary ,Multidisciplinary ,Computer science ,Morals ,Moral rule ,Epistemology ,330 Economics ,03 medical and health sciences ,Interpersonal relationship ,Moral code ,030104 developmental biology ,10007 Department of Economics ,Interpersonal Relations ,Simple (philosophy) - Abstract
The evolution of cooperation is a frequently debated topic. A study assessing scenarios in which people judge each other shows that a simple moral rule suffices to drive the evolution of cooperation. The evolution of cooperation is a frequently debated topic. A study assessing scenarios in which people judge each other shows that a simple moral rule suffices to drive the evolution of cooperation.
- Published
- 2018
50. Genome-wide association analyses of risk tolerance and risky behaviors in over 1 million individuals identify hundreds of loci and shared genetic influences1
- Author
-
Edward Kong, Pascal Timshel, Danielle Posthuma, Tõnu Esko, Maël Lebreton, Kathleen Mullan Harris, Philipp Koellinger, Murray B. Stein, Abraham A. Palmer, Urs Fischbacher, Robert J. Ursano, Erdogan Taskesen, Peter Eibich, Paul R. H. J. Timmers, Anke R. Hammerschlag, Ann H. Caplin, Jian Yang, Sandra Sanchez-Roige, Peter K. Joshi, Matthias Sutter, Pierre Fontanillas, Chia-Yen Chen, Albert Hofman, Patrick Turley, David A. Hinds, Futao Zhang, David Cesarini, Christina M. Lill, Laura Buzdugan, Ville Karhunen, Abdel Abdellaoui, S. Fleur W. Meddens, Henning Tiemeier, Christian L. Zund, Gert G. Wagner, Richard Karlsson Linnér, Lars Bertram, David W. Clark, Roy Thurik, André G. Uitterlinden, Maciej Trzaskowski, Mohammad Arfan Ikram, David Laibson, Cornelius A. Rietveld, Arcadi Navarro, Ernst Fehr, Yang Wu, Matthew B. McQueen, Ronald C. Kessler, Magnus Johannesson, Patrick J. F. Groenen, Gregor Hasler, James F. Wilson, Daniel Schunk, Stephen P. Tino, Pietro Biroli, Mark Alan Fontana, Juan R. González, Meena Kumari, Gerardus A. Meddens, Jonathan P. Beauchamp, Michelle N. Meyer, Jason D. Boardman, James J. Lee, Carlos Morcillo-Suarez, Aaron Kleinman, Minna Männikkö, Andrew Conlin, Adam Auton, Tune H. Pers, Michel G. Nivard, Ronald de Vlaming, Jon White, Robert Karlsson, Daniel J. Benjamin, Maarten Kooyman, Jacob Gratten, Aysu Okbay, Remy Z. Levin, Melissa C. Smart, Harriet de Wit, Conor C. Dolan, Frank J. A. van Rooij, Patrik K. E. Magnusson, Sonia Jain, Rauli Svento, Klaus M. Schmidt, Dorret I. Boomsma, Gerard Muntané, James MacKillop, Robbee Wedow, and Yanchun Bao
- Subjects
Genetics ,0303 health sciences ,Gabaergic neurotransmission ,Single-nucleotide polymorphism ,Genome-wide association study ,Biology ,Phenotype ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,SNP ,Gene ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,030304 developmental biology ,Genetic association - Abstract
Humans vary substantially in their willingness to take risks. In a combined sample of over one million individuals, we conducted genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of general risk tolerance, adventurousness, and risky behaviors in the driving, drinking, smoking, and sexual domains. We identified 611 approximately independent genetic loci associated with at least one of our phenotypes, including 124 with general risk tolerance. We report evidence of substantial shared genetic influences across general risk tolerance and risky behaviors: 72 of the 124 general risk tolerance loci contain a lead SNP for at least one of our other GWAS, and general risk tolerance is moderately to strongly genetically correlated (to 0.50) with a range of risky behaviors. Bioinformatics analyses imply that genes near general-risk-tolerance-associated SNPs are highly expressed in brain tissues and point to a role for glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission. We find no evidence of enrichment for genes previously hypothesized to relate to risk tolerance.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.