166 results on '"Horn, Lisa"'
Search Results
2. Moving in synchrony with an avatar - presenting a novel and unbiased body sway synchronization paradigm
- Author
-
Scheer, Clara, Horn, Lisa, and Jansen, Petra
- Subjects
Interpersonal relations -- Psychological aspects ,Gesture -- Psychological aspects ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
Moving in synchrony with one another is a fundamental mechanism that maintains human social bonds. Yet, not all individuals are equally likely to coordinate their behaviors with others. The degree of interpersonal coordination is greatly influenced by pre-existing characteristics of the interacting partners, like the cultural homogeneity of a group, shared goals, and the likability of the other person. Considering that most research questions necessitate an experimental set-up without such uncontrolled biases, we created a novel, unbiased paradigm: a human-avatar body sway synchronization paradigm. Participants' body sway was measured by a force plate while being exposed to a medio-laterally moving avatar. Forty-nine participants were tested in a social condition (motionless vs. moving avatar) and a non-social control condition (motionless vs. moving column). The results revealed that participants increased their body sway on their medio-lateral axis while the avatar was moving. The participants did not increase their body sway in the non-social control condition, indicating that the participant's movement was not simply caused by a basal motion perception process. The current study builds a methodological fundament that can help to reduce biases due to pre-existing rapport between interaction partners and serves as a valuable experimental paradigm for future synchrony studies., Author(s): Clara Scheer [sup.1] , Lisa Horn [sup.2] , Petra Jansen [sup.1] Author Affiliations: (1) grid.7727.5, 0000 0001 2190 5763, Faculty of Human Sciences, University of Regensburg, , Universitätsstraße 31, [...]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Migratory behavior of Ontario-breeding Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) revealed by the Motus Wildlife Tracking System/Comportamiento migratorio de los gorriones Passerculus sandwichensis que se reproducen en Ontario y observados por el Sistema de Monitoreo de Vida Silvestre Motus
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa C., van Vliet, Heidi E.J., Norris, D. Ryan, and Stutchbury, Bridget J.M.
- Subjects
Communications equipment ,Birds -- Behavior ,Cellular transmission equipment ,Telecommunications equipment ,Biological sciences - Abstract
Studies on the migratory behavior of songbirds are important to inform full annual cycle conservation. We remotely tracked the early fall migratory movements of both juvenile and adult Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) that were tagged on their natal breeding territories in southwestern Ontario, Canada, where the Motus Wildlife Tracking System has the highest density of automated receivers in North America. Our primary aims were to describe and compare adult and juvenile migratory movements during the early part of migration, compare frequency of detection of juveniles versus adults as a proxy for minimum apparent survival, and describe potential migration routes and overwintering areas. Juveniles initiated migration approximately a month earlier than adults. Both juveniles and adults typically made their first migratory flight in a southwest direction, but some juveniles displayed subsequent exploratory movements that were not in a southwest direction, potentially to collect information for next year's breeding site selection. Detection rates early in migration were similar, but low. for adults and juveniles (34% and 39% respectively), suggesting that juveniles that had survived the fledgling period likely did not experience high mortality during the subsequent 2 months prior to their fall migration. Long-distance detections of 2 adults preliminarily suggest that individuals from this southern Ontario breeding region may travel west of the Appalachian Mountains along the Mississippi flyway during fall migration. Our study aligns with previously reported patterns of age-specific migratory behavior from other Savannah Sparrow populations and contributes to our overall understanding of the migratory ecology of this species. Received 6 April 2021. Accepted 23 September 2022. Key words: fall migration, migration orientation, migration timing, songbird, telemetry. Los estudios de comportamiento de ave migratorias son importames para conocer su ciclo anual completo de conservacion. Monitoreamos de manera remota los movimientos migratorios de otofio temprano tanto de juveniles como de adultos de gorrion Passerculus sandwichensis que fueron marcados en sus territories natales/ reproductivos en el sudoeste de Ontario. Canada, donde el Sistema de Monitoreo de Vida Silvestre Motus tiene la mayor densidad de receptores automaticos de Norteamerica. Nuestros objetivos primarios ilieron describir y comparar los movimientos migratorios de adultos y juveniles durante !a parte inicia! de la migracion. comparar la frecueneia de deteccion de juveniles versus de adultos como proxy de la sobrevivencia aparente minima asi como describir potenciales rutas migratorias y areas de hibernacion. Los juveniles iniciaron la migracion aproximadamente un mes antes que los adultos. Tanto juveniles como adultos tipicamente hicieron su primer vuelo migratorio en direccion sudoeste. aunque algunos juveniles desplegaron movimientos exploratorios posteriores que no eran en direccion sudoeste. posiblemente para coleetar informacion para la seleccion del sitio de reproduccion del afio siguiente. Las tasas de deteccion de migracion temprana rueron similares. aunque bajas. para adultos y juveniles (respectivamente 34% y 39%), lo que sugiere que tos juveniles que sobrevivieron a su etapa de volantones posiblemente no tuvieron una alta mortalidad durante los 2 meses que siguieron a la migracion otonat. La deteccion a larga distancia de 2 adultos sugiere preliminarmente que los individuos de esta region de reproduccion al sur de Ontario podrian viajar al oeste de las Appalachian Mountains a lo largo del rio Mississippi durante la migracion de otono. Nuestro estudio se alinea con patrones previamente reportados de patrones migratorios especificos por edad de otras poblaciones de gorrion Passerculus sandwichensis y contribuyen al entendimiento general de la ecologia migratoria de esta especie. Palabras clave: aves canoras. migracion otonal, orientacion migratoria. telemetria, tiempo migratorio., The miniaturization of archival tracking devices has allowed scientists to directly follow the movements of individual migrating birds to determine their behavior and overwintering areas (McKinnon and Love 2018), leading [...]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Cognition in Context
- Author
-
Boucherie, Palmyre H, Cimarelli, Giulia, PhD, Horn, Lisa, Šlipogor, Vedrana, and Bugnyar, Thomas
- Subjects
cognitive science - Abstract
The theory behind the evolution of cognition frames that cognitive processes have evolved in response to the complexity and challenges posed by the physical and social environment. To date, cognitive abilities have been mostly studied under controlled laboratory conditions that facilitate replicability and high-resolution measurements (Cauchoix, Hermer, Chaine, & Morand-Ferron, 2017). Yet, under these circumstances, cognitive abilities are evaluated in relatively stable and homogeneous situations that hardly match the species’ natural environments (Niemelä and Dingemanse, 2014). Thus, results drawn from these controlled studies do not necessarily scale to the range of cognitive processes displayed by individuals in naturalistic settings (Cauchoix, Chaine, & Barragan-Jason, 2020).
- Published
- 2021
5. Beyond the dichotomy between field and lab — the importance of studying cognition in context
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Cimarelli, Giulia, Boucherie, Palmyre H, Šlipogor, Vedrana, and Bugnyar, Thomas
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Responsible Research is also concerned with generalizability: Recognizing efforts to reflect upon and increase generalizability in hiring and promotion decisions in psychology
- Author
-
Stengelin, Roman, primary, Bohn, Manuel, additional, Sánchez-Amaro, Alejandro, additional, Haun, Daniel, additional, Thiele, Maleen, additional, Daum, Moritz, additional, Felsche, Elisa, additional, Fong, Frankie, additional, Gampe, Anja, additional, Giner Torréns, Marta, additional, Grueneisen, Sebastian, additional, Hardecker, David, additional, Horn, Lisa, additional, Neldner, Karri, additional, Pope-Caldwell, Sarah, additional, and Schuhmacher, Nils, additional
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Risky decision-making following prefrontal D1 receptor manipulation
- Author
-
Beyer Dominik K. E., Horn Lisa, Klinker Nadine, and Freund Nadja
- Subjects
iowa gambling task ,lentivirus ,rat ,bipolar disorder ,mania ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
The prefrontal dopamine D1 receptor (D1R) is involved in cognitive processes. Viral overexpression of this receptor in rats further increases the reward-related behaviors and even its termination induces anhedonia and helplessness. In this study, we investigated the risky decision-making during D1R overexpression and its termination. Rats conducted the rodent version of the Iowa gambling task daily. In addition, the methyl CpG–binding protein-2 (MeCP2), one regulator connecting the dopaminergic system, cognitive processes, and mood-related behavior, was investigated after completion of the behavioral tasks. D1R overexpressing subjects exhibited maladaptive risky decision-making and risky decisions returned to control levels following termination of D1R overexpression; however, after termination, animals earned less reward compared to control subjects. In this phase, MeCP2-positive cells were elevated in the right amygdala. Our results extend the previously reported behavioral changes in the D1R-manipulated animal model to increased risk-taking and revealed differential MeCP2 expression adding further evidence for a bipolar disorder-like phenotype of this model.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Measuring salivary mesotocin in birds - Seasonal differences in ravens' peripheral mesotocin levels
- Author
-
Stocker, Martina, Prosl, Jonathan, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Canoine, Virginie, and Massen, Jorg J.M.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. An automated, data‐driven approach to children's social dynamics in space and time
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, primary, Karsai, Márton, additional, and Markova, Gabriela, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Testing the effects of kinship, reciprocity and dominance on prosocial food provisioning in azure‐winged magpies (Cyanopica cyana), carrion crows (Corvus corone) and common ravens (C. corax).
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, and Massen, Jorg J. M.
- Subjects
- *
MAGPIES , *ANIMAL carcasses , *RECIPROCITY (Psychology) , *CROWS , *KINSHIP - Abstract
Prosocial behaviour (i.e. benefitting others without receiving a direct gain) has long been perceived as an evolutionary puzzle but is nevertheless relatively common among non‐human animals. Prosocial food provisioning has recently been documented in several large‐brained bird species, such as corvids and parrots. Yet, to date, little is known about which factors influence food provisioning in these species. Here, we investigated whether kinship, reciprocity and dominance affected food provisioning in the group service paradigm in three corvid species, namely azure‐winged magpies (Cyanopica cyana), carrion crows (Corvus corone) and common ravens (C. corax). In this paradigm, the subjects are tested in their regular social groups and can choose to make food available to their group members by landing on a simple seesaw apparatus. We found no evidence for an effect of kinship or reciprocity on food provisioning. Contrary to our predictions, the subjects' dominance was not positively correlated with their rate of food provisioning in any species. Among ravens, dominance was instead positively correlated with receiving food. We conclude that preferential provisioning for kin and direct reciprocity might have been impeded by the provider's inability to control who receives the food in the group service paradigm, but that our findings provide another piece of evidence that dominance is a highly important factor in the social interactions of common ravens. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. An automated, data‐driven approach to children's social dynamics in space and time.
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Karsai, Márton, and Markova, Gabriela
- Abstract
Most children first enter social groups of peers in preschool. In this context, children use movement as a social tool, resulting in distinctive proximity patterns in space and synchrony with others over time. However, the social implications of children's movements with peers in space and time are difficult to determine due to the difficulty of acquiring reliable data during natural interactions. In this article, we review research demonstrating that proximity and synchrony are important indicators of affiliation among preschoolers and highlight challenges in this line of research. We then argue for the advantages of using wearable sensor technology and machine learning analytics to quantify social movement. This technological and analytical advancement provides an unprecedented view of complex social interactions among preschoolers in natural settings, and can help integrate young children's movements with others in space and time into a coherent interaction framework. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Testing the effects of kinship, reciprocity and dominance on prosocial food provisioning in azure‐winged magpies (Cyanopica cyana), carrion crows (Corvus corone) and common ravens (C. corax)
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, primary, Bugnyar, Thomas, additional, and Massen, Jorg J. M., additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Citizen science to address the global issue of bird–window collisions
- Author
-
Loss, Scott R, primary, Li, Binbin V, additional, Horn, Lisa C, additional, Mesure, Michael R, additional, Zhu, Lei, additional, Brys, Timothy G, additional, Dokter, Adriaan M, additional, Elmore, Jared A, additional, Gibbons, Richard E, additional, Homayoun, Tania Z, additional, Horton, Kyle G, additional, Inglet, Patsy, additional, Jones, Benjamin J, additional, Keys, Taylor, additional, Lao, Sirena, additional, Loss, Sara S, additional, Parkins, Kaitlyn L, additional, Prestridge, Heather L, additional, Riggs, Georgia J, additional, Riding, Corey S, additional, Sweezey, Katherine RI, additional, Vallery, Anna C, additional, Van Doren, Benjamin M, additional, Wang, Julia, additional, Zuzula, Caley, additional, and Farnsworth, Andrew, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Responsible Research is also concerned with generalizability: Recognizing efforts to reflect upon and increase generalizability in hiring and promotion decisions in psychology
- Author
-
Stengelin, Roman, primary, Bohn, Manuel, additional, Sanchez-Amaro, Alejandro, additional, Haun, Daniel B. M., additional, Thiele, Maleen, additional, Allritz, Matthias, additional, Daum, Moritz M., additional, Felsche, Elisa, additional, Fong, Frankie T. K., additional, Gampe, Anja, additional, Torréns, Marta Giner, additional, Grueneisen, Sebastian, additional, Hardecker, David Johannes Kaspar, additional, Horn, Lisa, additional, Neldner, Karri, additional, Pope, Sarah, additional, and Schuhmacher, Nils, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Migratory behavior of Ontario-breeding Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) revealed by the Motus Wildlife Tracking System
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa C., primary, van Vliet, Heidi E.J., additional, Norris, D. Ryan, additional, and Stutchbury, Bridget J.M., additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Social status and prenatal testosterone exposure assessed via second-to-fourth digit ratio affect 6–9-year-old children’s prosocial choices
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Hungerländer, Niklas A., Windhager, Sonja, Bugnyar, Thomas, and Massen, Jorg J. M.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. International Labor Standards: History, Theory, and Policy Options
- Author
-
Kaushik Basu, Henrik Horn, Lisa Roman, Judith Shapiro, Kaushik Basu, Henrik Horn, Lisa Roman, Judith Shapiro
- Published
- 2008
18. The Absence of Reward Induces Inequity Aversion in Dogs
- Author
-
Range, Friederike, Horn, Lisa, Viranyi, Zsófia, and Huber, Ludwig
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Motives for Veganism and the Influence of Social Media
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, primary, Budulan, Mirabela-Andreea, additional, and Loghin, Mirela, additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Donor NK and T Cells in the Periphery of Lung Transplant Recipients Contain High Frequencies of Killer Cell Immunoglobulin-Like Receptor-Positive Subsets
- Author
-
Hitz, Anna-Maria, primary, Bläsing, Kim-Alina, additional, Wiegmann, Bettina, additional, Bellmàs-Sanz, Ramon, additional, Chichelnitskiy, Evgeny, additional, Wandrer, Franziska, additional, Horn, Lisa-Marie, additional, Neudörfl, Christine, additional, Keil, Jana, additional, Beushausen, Kerstin, additional, Ius, Fabio, additional, Sommer, Wiebke, additional, Avsar, Murat, additional, Kühn, Christian, additional, Tudorache, Igor, additional, Salman, Jawad, additional, Siemeni, Thierry, additional, Haverich, Axel, additional, Warnecke, Gregor, additional, Falk, Christine S., additional, and Kühne, Jenny F., additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Carrion crows and azure-winged magpies show no prosocial tendencies when tested in a token transfer paradigm
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Zewald, Jeroen S., Bugnyar, Thomas, Massen, Jorg J.M., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
0106 biological sciences ,Veterinary medicine ,Corvid ,Corvus corone ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Security token ,010603 evolutionary biology ,01 natural sciences ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Article ,Stress (linguistics) ,SF600-1100 ,Prosociality ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Carrion ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Control (linguistics) ,General Veterinary ,05 social sciences ,Comparative methods ,Cyanopica cyana ,Cognitive complexity ,Replicate ,veterinary(all) ,Instrumental helping ,Cooperation ,Prosocial behavior ,QL1-991 ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Psychology ,Zoology ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Simple Summary Sharing your possessions or donating them to others, as well as helping others, can be summarized under the term, “prosocial behavior”. Recently, researchers have become interested in whether, and in which situations, animals help and share. In this study, we trained carrion crows and azure-winged magpies—two bird species that have previously been found to donate food to their group members—to exchange objects, called “tokens”, with an experimenter for food. We then tested whether the birds would provide these tokens to their group members when they themselves did not have access to the experimenter, but their group members did. We found, however, that there were very few attempted and/or successful token transfers between the birds, suggesting that they were not prosocial in this situation. We argue that the carrion crows and azure-winged magpies might not have fully understood the value of the tokens, either as placeholders for food or as “currency” with which food could be obtained. This limited understanding might have prevented them from exhibiting prosocial behavior in the current study. Therefore, we advocate the use of simpler and more naturalistic paradigms to study prosocial behaviors, such as providing food or resources to others, in a broader range of species. Abstract To study the evolution of humans’ cooperative nature, researchers have recently sought comparisons with other species. Studies investigating corvids, for example, showed that carrion crows and azure-winged magpies delivered food to group members when tested in naturalistic or simple experimental paradigms. Here, we investigated whether we could replicate these positive findings when testing the same two species in a token transfer paradigm. After training the birds to exchange tokens with an experimenter for food rewards, we tested whether they would also transfer tokens to other birds, when they did not have the opportunity to exchange the tokens themselves. To control for the effects of motivation, and of social or stimulus enhancement, we tested each individual in three additional control conditions. We witnessed very few attempts and/or successful token transfers, and those few instances did not occur more frequently in the test condition than in the controls, which would suggest that the birds lack prosocial tendencies. Alternatively, we propose that this absence of prosociality may stem from the artificial nature and cognitive complexity of the token transfer task. Consequently, our findings highlight the strong impact of methodology on animals’ capability to exhibit prosocial tendencies and stress the importance of comparing multiple experimental paradigms.
- Published
- 2021
22. Virus infections of the European Eel in North Rhine Westphalian rivers
- Author
-
Danne, Linna, primary, Horn, Lisa, additional, Feldhaus, Anita, additional, Fey, Daniel, additional, Emde, Sebastian, additional, Schütze, Heike, additional, Adamek, Mikolaj, additional, and Hellmann, John, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. The Green Pages: Animals.
- Author
-
Van Horn, Lisa
- Abstract
Presents environmental education activities for grades K-2, 3-5, 6-8, and 9-12 that integrate the concepts of animals into science, social studies, mathematics, language arts, and fine arts. Activities are divided by class level and subject matter to help integrate them into the curriculum. (MDH)
- Published
- 1993
24. Dogs’ attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Range, Friederike, and Huber, Ludwig
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Measuring salivary mesotocin in birds - Seasonal differences in ravens' peripheral mesotocin levels
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Stocker, Martina, Prosl, Jonathan, Vanhooland, Lisa Claire, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Canoine, Virginie, Massen, Jorg J.M., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Stocker, Martina, Prosl, Jonathan, Vanhooland, Lisa Claire, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Canoine, Virginie, and Massen, Jorg J.M.
- Published
- 2021
26. Carrion crows and azure-winged magpies show no prosocial tendencies when tested in a token transfer paradigm
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Horn, Lisa, Zewald, Jeroen S., Bugnyar, Thomas, Massen, Jorg J.M., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Horn, Lisa, Zewald, Jeroen S., Bugnyar, Thomas, and Massen, Jorg J.M.
- Published
- 2021
27. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Virányi, Zsófia, Miklósi, Ádám, Huber, Ludwig, and Range, Friederike
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children
- Author
-
Range, Friederike, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Gajdon, Gyula K., and Huber, Ludwig
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Weak evidence of carry-over effects of overwinter climate and habitat productivity on spring passage of migratory songbirds at a northern stopover site in Ontario
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa C, primary, Remmel, Tarmo K, additional, and Stutchbury, Bridget J M, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Moving in synchrony with an avatar – presenting a novel and unbiased body sway synchronization paradigm
- Author
-
Scheer, Clara, primary, Horn, Lisa, additional, and Jansen, Petra, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Jungfrau, Himmlische, Gottesgebärerin
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa
- Subjects
ddc:305.309 ,Marian worship, goddesses, virgin, cult ,ddc:300 ,Marienverehrung, Göttinnen, Jungfrau, Kult - Abstract
Lisa Horn (M. A.) verfasste einen religionsgeschichtlichen Beitrag mit dem Titel: Jungfrau, Himmlische, Gottesgebärerin. Der Kult der antiken Göttinnen Artemis und Aphrodite als Kontext früher Marienverehrung? Konzentriert auf die fünf ersten nachchristlichen Jahrhunderte und einige signifikante religiös-kultische Transfer-Zentren in Zypern und Kleinasien (etwa Ephesus) gilt das Erkenntnisinteresse der Verfasserin den Differenzen und Kontinuitäten in den Mythologien und Kulten der zwei antiken Göttinnen Artemis und Aphrodite sowie der frühchristlichen Figurierung und Verehrung Marias. Mit Hilfe verschiedener Quellensorten wird forschungsgestützt argumentiert, dass das erstarkende Christentum ältere pagane, polytheistische (und selbst hoch hybride) Kulte und Gottheiten synchretistisch adaptierte. Deutlich wird, wie die matristischen, in den beiden griechisch-römischen Göttinnen noch erinnerten Aspekte von gleichzeitiger Jungfräulichkeit und Mütterlichkeit einerseits mit dem Vorstellungskomplex von der menschlichen Empfängnis eines ‚Gottessohnes‘ andererseits verbunden und zur Deutung der ‚jungfräulichen Gottesmutter Maria‘ genutzt wurden, um so, funktionsgeschichtlich gelesen, den androzentrischen Wegfall weiblicher Gottheiten kompensieren zu helfen.
- Published
- 2020
32. A pleading for the relevance of comparative psychology for the understanding of human development
- Author
-
Haun, Daniel B. M., Liebal, Katja, Amici, Federica, Bender, Andrea, Bohn, Manuel, Braeuer, Juliane, Buttelmann, David, Burkart, Judith, Cacchione, Trix, DeTroy, Sarah, Fassbender, Ina, Fichtel, Claudia, Fischer, Julia, Gampe, Anja, Gray, Russel, Horn, Lisa, Ona, Linda, Kaertner, Joscha, Kaminski, Juliane, Kanngiesser, Patricia, Keller, Heidi, Koester, Moritz, Kopp, Kathrin Susanne, Kornadt, Hans-Joachim, Rakoczy, Hannes, Schuppli, Caroline, Stengelin, Roman, Trommsdorff, Gisela, van Leeuwen, Edwin, and van Schaik, Carel
- Subjects
Psychology - Published
- 2020
33. Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids
- Author
-
Sub Animal Ecology, Animal Ecology, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Griesser, Michael, Hengl, Marietta, Izawa, Ei-Ichi, Oortwijn, Tim, Rössler, Christiane, Scheer, Clara, Schiestl, Martina, Suyama, Masaki, Taylor, Alex H, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, von Bayern, Auguste Mp, Zürcher, Yvonne, Massen, Jorg Jm, Sub Animal Ecology, Animal Ecology, Horn, Lisa, Bugnyar, Thomas, Griesser, Michael, Hengl, Marietta, Izawa, Ei-Ichi, Oortwijn, Tim, Rössler, Christiane, Scheer, Clara, Schiestl, Martina, Suyama, Masaki, Taylor, Alex H, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, von Bayern, Auguste Mp, Zürcher, Yvonne, and Massen, Jorg Jm
- Published
- 2020
34. Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to grooming in great apes
- Author
-
Sub Animal Ecology, Animal Ecology, Morales Picard, Alejandra, Mundry, Roger, Auersperg, Alice M., Boeving, Emily R., Boucherie, Palmyre H., Bugnyar, Thomas, Dufour, Valérie, Emery, Nathan J., Federspiel, Ira G., Gajdon, Gyula K., Guéry, Jean Pascal, Hegedič, Matjaž, Horn, Lisa, Kavanagh, Eithne, Lambert, Megan L., Massen, Jorg J.M., Rodrigues, Michelle A., Schiestl, Martina, Schwing, Raoul, Szabo, Birgit, Taylor, Alex H., van Horik, Jayden O., von Bayern, Auguste M.P., Seed, Amanda, Slocombe, Katie E., Sub Animal Ecology, Animal Ecology, Morales Picard, Alejandra, Mundry, Roger, Auersperg, Alice M., Boeving, Emily R., Boucherie, Palmyre H., Bugnyar, Thomas, Dufour, Valérie, Emery, Nathan J., Federspiel, Ira G., Gajdon, Gyula K., Guéry, Jean Pascal, Hegedič, Matjaž, Horn, Lisa, Kavanagh, Eithne, Lambert, Megan L., Massen, Jorg J.M., Rodrigues, Michelle A., Schiestl, Martina, Schwing, Raoul, Szabo, Birgit, Taylor, Alex H., van Horik, Jayden O., von Bayern, Auguste M.P., Seed, Amanda, and Slocombe, Katie E.
- Published
- 2020
35. Cross-cultural differences in domestic dogs’ interactions with humans – preliminary results from ainsworth’s strange situation test
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Marshall-Pescini, Sarah, Virányi, Zsófia, and Range, Friederike
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) attention toward humans: does it depend on the relationship?
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Huber, Ludwig, and Range, Friederike
- Published
- 2011
37. Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, primary, Bugnyar, Thomas, additional, Griesser, Michael, additional, Hengl, Marietta, additional, Izawa, Ei-Ichi, additional, Oortwijn, Tim, additional, Rössler, Christiane, additional, Scheer, Clara, additional, Schiestl, Martina, additional, Suyama, Masaki, additional, Taylor, Alex H, additional, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, additional, von Bayern, Auguste MP, additional, Zürcher, Yvonne, additional, and Massen, Jorg JM, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Risk Aversion, Entrepreneurial Attitudes, Intention and Entry Among Young People in Uganda and Germany: A Gendered Analysis
- Author
-
Baluku, Martin Mabunda, primary, Nansubuga, Florence, additional, Otto, Kathleen, additional, and Horn, Lisa, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Author response: Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, primary, Bugnyar, Thomas, additional, Griesser, Michael, additional, Hengl, Marietta, additional, Izawa, Ei-Ichi, additional, Oortwijn, Tim, additional, Rössler, Christiane, additional, Scheer, Clara, additional, Schiestl, Martina, additional, Suyama, Masaki, additional, Taylor, Alex H, additional, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, additional, von Bayern, Auguste MP, additional, Zürcher, Yvonne, additional, and Massen, Jorg JM, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Kommentare zu Daum, M. M., Greve, W., Pauen, S., Schuhrke, B. und Schwarzer, G. (2020). Positionspapier der Fachgruppe Entwicklungspsychologie: Ein Versuch einer Standortbestimmung
- Author
-
Wentura, Dirk, primary, Bermeitinger, Christina, additional, Eder, Andreas, additional, Giesen, Carina G., additional, Michalkiewicz, Martha, additional, Hartwigsen, Gesa, additional, Röder, Brigitte, additional, Lischke, Alexander, additional, Kübler, Andrea, additional, Pauli, Paul, additional, Renner, Karl-Heinz, additional, Ziegler, Matthias, additional, Spengler, Marion, additional, Christiansen, Hanna, additional, Richter, Tobias, additional, Souvignier, Elmar, additional, Heyder, Anke, additional, Kunina-Habenicht, Olga, additional, Hertel, Silke, additional, Sparfeldt, Jörn, additional, Bischof, Norbert, additional, Glück, Judith, additional, Haun, Daniel, additional, Liebal, Katja, additional, Amici, Federica, additional, Bender, Andrea, additional, Bohn, Manuel, additional, Bräuer, Juliane, additional, Buttelmann, David, additional, Burkart, Judith, additional, Cacchione, Trix, additional, DeTroy, Sarah, additional, Faßbender, Ina, additional, Fichtel, Claudia, additional, Fischer, Julia, additional, Gampe, Anja, additional, Gray, Russel, additional, Horn, Lisa, additional, Oña, Linda, additional, Kärtner, Joscha, additional, Kaminski, Juliane, additional, Kanngießer, Patricia, additional, Keller, Heidi, additional, Köster, Moritz, additional, Kopp, Kathrin Susanne, additional, Kornadt, Hans-Joachim, additional, Rakoczy, Hannes, additional, Schuppli, Caroline, additional, Stengelin, Roman, additional, Trommsdorff, Gisela, additional, Leeuwen, Edwin van, additional, Schaik, Carel van, additional, Jüttemann, Gerd, additional, Loh, Werner, additional, and Paulus, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to grooming in great apes
- Author
-
Morales Picard, Alejandra, primary, Mundry, Roger, additional, Auersperg, Alice M., additional, Boeving, Emily R., additional, Boucherie, Palmyre H., additional, Bugnyar, Thomas, additional, Dufour, Valérie, additional, Emery, Nathan J., additional, Federspiel, Ira G., additional, Gajdon, Gyula K., additional, Guéry, Jean‐Pascal, additional, Hegedič, Matjaž, additional, Horn, Lisa, additional, Kavanagh, Eithne, additional, Lambert, Megan L., additional, Massen, Jorg J. M., additional, Rodrigues, Michelle A., additional, Schiestl, Martina, additional, Schwing, Raoul, additional, Szabo, Birgit, additional, Taylor, Alex H., additional, Horik, Jayden O., additional, Bayern, Auguste M. P., additional, Seed, Amanda, additional, and Slocombe, Katie E., additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Virus infections of the European Eel in North Rhine Westphalian rivers.
- Author
-
Danne, Linna, Horn, Lisa, Feldhaus, Anita, Fey, Daniel, Emde, Sebastian, Schütze, Heike, Adamek, Mikolaj, and Hellmann, John
- Subjects
- *
ANGUILLA anguilla , *HERPESVIRUSES , *PATHOGENIC viruses , *EELS , *VIRUS diseases , *GLASS eels , *CELL culture - Abstract
Viral infections have been suggested to play a role in the decline of the panmictic population of the European eel (Anguilla anguilla). However, despite the importance of knowledge about pathogenic eel viruses, little is known about their spread in the wild European eel population and only a few eel pathogenic viruses have been described so far. In this study, we aimed to investigate the health status of the A. anguilla stock in North Rhine Westphalia (NRW) State of Germany. For this purpose, we examined tissue samples of 16 elvers, 100 yellow eels and 6 silver eels, sampled from the rivers Rhine, Lippe and Ems. Virus detection was performed via a combination of cell culture and PCR. Next to the detection of frequently encountered pathogenic eel viruses (anguillid herpesvirus 1 and eel virus European X (EVEX)), we isolated the eel picornavirus 1 (EPV‐1) from tissue of yellow eels and elvers and demonstrate the distribution of EPV‐1 in wild eel population in NRW. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Joint EIFAAC/ICES/GFCM Working Group on Eels (WGEEL)
- Author
-
Amilhat, Elsa, Basic, Tea, Beaulaton, Laurent, Belpaire, Claude, Bernotas, Priit, Briand, Cédric, Bryhn, Andreas, Capoccioni, Fabrizio, Ciccotti, Eleonora, Dekker, Willem, Diaz, Estibaliz, Domingos, Isabel, Drouineau, Hilaire, Durif, Caroline M. F., Evans, Derek, Giedrojc, Lukasz, Gollock, Matthew, van der Hammen, Tessa, Hanel, Reinhold, Horn, Lisa, Observer, Katarzyna Janiak, Observer, Kenzo Kaifu, Leone, Chiara, Lozys, Linas, Marohn, Lasse, Nermer, Tomasz, O’Leary, Ciara, Pedersen, Michael Ingemann, Pohlmann, Jan-Dag, Poole, Russell, Povliūnas, Justas, Rosell, Robert, Rohtla, Mehis, Sapoundis, Argyris, Simon, Janek, Sundin, Josefin, Svagzydys, Arvydas, Thorstad, Eva B., Vesala, Sami, Walker, Alan M., and Wickström, Håkan
- Subjects
SDG 14 - Life Below Water - Abstract
The WGEEL has this year adjusted the manner in which it reports data in time-series. First, readers should note that some data reported to the WGEEL in the most recent year are always provisional but are then finalised in the report of the following year. Where data have been updated from those reported in the 2018 report this is indicated in the 2019 report; and provisional data are similarly highlighted. Second, the mean of the previous 5 years data is also presented to help place the data from the most recent year(s) in context of this most recent period. The recruitment of European eel from the ocean remained low in 2019. The glass eel recruitment compared to the 1960–1979 in the ‘North Sea’ index area was 1.4% in 2019 (provisional), 1.9% in 2018 (finalised) and the previous 5-year mean was 1.7% (2012-2016); and in the ‘Elsewhere Europe’ index series it was 6.0% in 2019 (provisional), 8.9% in 2018 (final) and the previous 5-year mean was 8.7%, based on available dataseries. For the yellow eel dataseries, recruitment for 2018 was 26.4% of the 1960–1979 level and the previous 5-year mean was 16.6% (2013–2017); 2019 data collection is ongoing so data not available at time of writing. Statistical analyses of time-series from 1980–2019 show that there was a change in the trend of glass eel recruitment indices in 2011; the recruitment has stopped decreasing and has been increasing in the period 2011–2019 with a rate statistically significantly different from zero. The highest point during the period from 2011–2019 was in 2014. Landings data were updated according to those reported to the WGEEL, either through responses to the 2019 Data call or Country Reports, or integrated by the WGEEL using data from its previous reports. When data are absent and presumed missing for a country or year, a predicted (reconstructed) catch is used to account for non-reporting, but this is not a complete solution and therefore even the raised estimates should be considered as minima. Here we present both reported and reconstructed values. Glass eel fisheries within the EU take place in France, UK, Spain, Portugal and Italy. Glass eel landings have declined sharply from 1980, when reported and reconstructed landings were larger than 2000 tonnes, to 62.2 t in 2018 (final, full reporting), 58.6 t in 2019 (provisional, no reconstruction), and a mean for the previous 5 years (2013–2017) of 56.5 t (full reporting). Yellow and silver eel landings are not always reported separately, so are combined here. The WGEEL has reconstructed the time-series to fill in some gaps in reporting. Reconstructed total commercial landings of yellow and silver eels were around 20 000 t in the 1950s to 2000–3500 t around 2009, most recently being 2393 t in 2017 (final), 2694 t in 2018 (provisional) and a mean of 2729 t for the preceding 5 years (2012–2016). The reported landings were around 10 000 to 12 000 t in the 1950s, declining to 2000 to 3000 t around 2009, and more recently being 2249 t in 2017 (final), 2375 t in 2018 (provisional, only 14 countries reported) and a mean of 2729 t for the preceding 5 years (2012–2016).Recreational catches and landings are poorly reported, so amounts must be treated as minima. Spain reports a recreational fishery for glass eel, with landings estimated as 0.9 t for 2019 (provisional), with a mean of 2 t for the preceding 5 years (2014–2018). Recreational landings for yellow and silver eel combined were 543 t for 2016 (ten countries reporting), 195 t for 2017 (eight countries reported) and 148 t for 2018 (five countries reported). Overall, the impact of recreational fisheries on the eel stock remains largely unquantified although landings can be thought to be at a similar order of magnitude to those of commercial fisheries. Aquaculture production of eel is presented from 1984 onwards. It increased until the mid-2000s, peaking around 8000 t. Production was reported in 2017 (the most recent year of most countries reporting: 10) as 5497 t in 2017 and the preceding 5-year mean was 6429 t (2012–2016). It should be noted that eel aquaculture is based on wild recruits, and part of the production is subsequently released as on-grown eel for stocking. Restocking (the process of capture, translocation and restocking to new locations in the wild) of eel increased after the implementation of management plans in EU Member States in 2009. Although the definition of restocking is clear, the process is complex with a varied and broad sequence of steps and even life stages. As there is still some variation in the way that countries report some of these actions, the WGEEL broadly categorises them as RELEASES, though the term RESTOCKING is still used here for some circumstances. Most recent relatively complete data show 36.3 million glass eel (2017, 15 countries), 14 million yellow eels (2016, six countries) and about 0.25 million silver eels (2018, three countries) were restocked or released (combined). The WG has made substantial progress in developing the use of the Data call and database to refine data submission, checking, analyses and reporting. The Data call for 2020 will request updates for recruitment, landings, aquaculture and releases, plus abundance indices for yellow and silver eels. The emerging threats and opportunities reported by WGEEL in each of the last three years continue to develop/evolve from their initial reporting. In addition, a new eel virus (picornavirus EPV-1) has been detected in eels in several German waters. The WG has a new standing annual activity to examine quantification of the impacts of nonfishery factors, and to review methods for reducing these mortalities. A crude estimate of loss to all non-fishery anthropogenic factors (largely hydropower and pumps) of eel was estimated from reported mortality indicators from approximately half of countries reporting to WG. This amounted to 1625 tonnes annually. Given better and more consistent data, this estimate could be improved in the future. Evidence on the impacts of hydropower facilities and water pumps was reviewed, with new studies ranging from direct measurements of eel mortality at individual facilities, through models to extend empirical data at individual sites to estimate impacts to regional levels, to overarching reviews and national and international advisory reports. Ranges of mortality as eel pass by or through hydropower stations are highly variable, and within previously published ranges. Mitigation measures to reduce eel losses from hydropower and pumping facilities provide clear technical scope for individual site and collective actions to reduce current losses. The WG considered the potential impact of changes to fishery regulations on the time-series used in support of the ICES advice. Many fishery-based time-series are used to assess temporal trends in recruitment and escapement. This is especially true for recruitment in the so-called ‘Elsewhere Europe’ area. New fishery regulations might introduce biases in those time-series and compromise their use in the analyses. Losing fishery-based indices would increase the noise in the stock assessment. As such, it seems worthwhile implementing new fishery-independent time-series. The WG considered the consequences of the Precautionary Approach on advice for European eel. Based on the FAO Code of Conduct, the ICES form of advice, and the EU Eel Regulation, the WG developed a proposal for a coherent framework for advice on eel, consisting of a doubletiered approach: an international tier focused on the status of the whole stock and the long-term objectives (overall stock status, recruitment trends, biomass reference points), and a national (or lower) tier focused on mortality levels and related management actions, addressed per management unit. This proposal suggests adoption of the reference point of the Eel Regulation, as Bmgt = 40% escapement of pristine, and a corresponding mortality limit of ΣAmgt = 0.92. Below Bmgt, mortality should be reduced further, to allow recovery of the stock. It is suggested to adopt a provisional time frame in terms of number of generations for this, which would translate into a corresponding mortality limit for each management unit. Noting that the proposed comprehensive framework for advice deviates from conventional ICES approaches, it is concluded that a followup workshop convened by ACOM might be appropriate, to discuss and evaluate the proposed framework and consider any now unforeseen or unintentional consequences. An international process of Quality Assurance of national assessments and stock indicators is also required as a matter of urgency. The WG considered the challenge of quantifying the effort that is undertaken in the commercial eel fisheries around Europe, based on new data provided by countries through the Data call. It was concluded that for many countries, the licensing of commercial eel fisheries needs to be improved in order to supply fishery managers and WGEEL with the appropriate information to assess the state of the stock. Difficulties encountered include inadequate reporting of levels of effort, lack of recording on number of gears per licence and generic multispecies licensing. The level of reporting can be at the national, regional or local level and this has resulted in some countries having different licence requirements per waterbody. The WGEEL has recommended a workshop on harmonising the reporting of fishing effort.
- Published
- 2019
44. Kleinkinder unter sich : Peerbeziehungen im dynamischen sozialen Netzwerk einer Kindergartengruppe
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa
- Published
- 2019
45. Risk Aversion, Entrepreneurial Attitudes, Intention and Entry Among Young People in Uganda and Germany: A Gendered Analysis.
- Author
-
Baluku, Martin Mabunda, Nansubuga, Florence, Otto, Kathleen, and Horn, Lisa
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to grooming in great apes.
- Author
-
Morales Picard, Alejandra, Mundry, Roger, Auersperg, Alice M., Boeving, Emily R., Boucherie, Palmyre H., Bugnyar, Thomas, Dufour, Valérie, Emery, Nathan J., Federspiel, Ira G., Gajdon, Gyula K., Guéry, Jean‐Pascal, Hegedič, Matjaž, Horn, Lisa, Kavanagh, Eithne, Lambert, Megan L., Massen, Jorg J. M., Rodrigues, Michelle A., Schiestl, Martina, Schwing, Raoul, and Szabo, Birgit
- Subjects
PARROTS ,SOCIAL bonds ,HOMINIDS ,SOCIAL skills ,CHIMPANZEES ,PRIMATES - Abstract
Allogrooming in primates serves not only a hygienic function, but also plays a crucial role in maintaining strong affiliative bonds between group members, which in turn, underpin the emergence of cooperative behavior. In contrast, although allopreening occurs in many avian species, we know little about its social functions. Our study addresses this issue by investigating allopreening in a broad comparative data set including six corvid and nine parrot species. We assessed whether rates of allopreening initiations, proportion of time spent allopreening, and the number of grooming partners in captive group‐housed birds were comparable to patterns observed in captive chimpanzees and bonobos. While parrots and corvids were found to have similar rates of social grooming to bonobos and chimpanzees, Pan species dedicated significantly more time to social grooming. Animals in larger groups had more grooming partners, but when controlling for the number of potential partners, birds tended to have fewer grooming interaction partners than Pan species. We then investigated whether allopreening in parrots and corvids was predicted by behavioral markers of affiliative social bonds (close physical proximity, active feeding, and low levels of agonistic behavior). Results revealed that providing allopreening to a partner was significantly predicted by often being in close proximity, but not engagement in active feeding or agonistic behavior. We examined the region allopreened in a subset of species and found that preening a partner's head was predicted by both close physical proximity and active feeding, while body allopreening was only predicted by close physical proximity. Head preening may confer more hygienic benefits to recipients, and thus, may be more selectively provided to valued partners. Results support the hypothesis that allopreening in corvids and parrots helps maintain social bonds with an individual's most important social partners, showing some similarities to allogrooming in primates. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Report of the Working Group on Eels (WGEEL), 2016:ICES WGEEL REPORT 2016
- Author
-
Amilhat, Elsa, Beaulaton, Laurent, Belpaire, Claude, Bernotas, Pritt, Besta, Mehrez, Briand, Cedric, Bryhn, Andreas, Camara, Karin, Ciccotti, Eleonora, Dekker, Willem, Diaz, Estabaliz, Dolan, Conora, Domingos, Isabel, Dorow, Malte, Durif, Caroline, Evans, Derek, Obererver, Evangelia Georgitsi, Geidrojc, Lukasz, Godfrey, Jason, De Graaf, Martin, Hanel, Reinhold, Horn, Lisa, and Ingemann Pedersen, Michael
- Subjects
Europe ,fisheries policy ,Fish ,freshwater eels (Anguillidae) ,Species management plan ,B280-animal-ecology - Abstract
In 2016, the WGEEL glass eel recruitment indices remain low at 2.7% of the 1960–1979 reference level in the ‘North Sea’ series, and 10.7% in the ‘Elsewhere’ series. The ‘recruiting yellow eel’ index was 21% of the level during the reference period.The Eel Management Plan silver eel biomass and mortality rate estimates (reported in 2015) indicate the stock in the EU-assessed area is not within the biomass limits of the Eel Regulation and in most management units, anthropogenic mortality exceeds a level that can be expected to lead to recovery.FAO reports the total landings from Commercial fisheries in 2014 were about 3321 t of eel. Six countries account for 73% of the FAO landings: France, Egypt, UK, Netherlands, Sweden and Denmark. Five EU Member States have a glass eel fishery (France, UK, Spain, Portugal and Italy): some non-EU countries (e.g. Morocco) also have glass eel fisheries but data from these were not available for analysis. The best estimates of the total EU catch of glass eel in 2015 and 2016 were 51.6 and 59.2 t, respectively.About 10.6 million glass eels and 9.2 million yellow eels were stocked in 2015. Stocking is a component of many Eel Management Plans (EMPs) and in some cases the commitment could not be achieved in 2016 due to timing, market and other glass eel availability issues. Aquaculture production was about 4000–6500 t in 2015/2016 (data from FAO, FEAP and WGEEL Country Reports).WGEEL attempts to cross-check glass eel catch with records of their fate (consumption, restocking and aquaculture) reveal major discrepancies between reporting systems. About 32% of the catch for 2015 has no recorded fate (about 36% for 2016). EuroStat trade data show France and UK declared exports of glass eel to Hong Kong in 2015/2016 despite these being ‘banned’ by the EU application of CITES.The EU Eel Regulation effectively implements a Distributed Control System, in which common objectives (protection and recovery, minimal spawner production of 40% relative to the notional pristine production) are achieved by collective action (national management plans, reducing mortalities). Effective governance across the whole stock requires other areas to adopt the same approach of distributed control. Most non-EU areas have only recently joined this process, and further development - of reference points, assessment procedures, and feedback mechanisms - might be required, to cope with unforeseen complications and/or to familiarise local experts, and involve them in future standardisation processes. Additionally, reference points, assessment procedures and feedback mechanisms will need to be agreed for the whole distribution area.A mechanism needs to found between the EU and the ICES rules to facilitate feedback on the status of the implementation of the EMPs, as in the Eel Management Plan Evaluation workshop (WKEPEMP) in 2013 (ICES, 2013). This lack leaves a void between the formal Precautionary Advice and scientific support for the recovery plan on eel.Knowledge gaps and research needs were identified regarding impacts of pollutants and hydropower, habitat preferences, and monitoring across environments. A recent review shows that evidence on net benefits of eel stocking is inconclusive. Emerging threats include climate change, pollution and post-release mortalities from recreational fisheries. New opportunities include research on migratory triggers and habitat use, survey methods in large water bodies, protection for eel passing hydropower facilities, and coordinating eel management and data collection in the Mediterranean. In 2016, the WGEEL glass eel recruitment indices remain low at 2.7% of the 1960–1979 reference level in the ‘North Sea’ series, and 10.7% in the ‘Elsewhere’ series. The ‘recruiting yellow eel’ index was 21% of the level during the reference period.The Eel Management Plan silver eel biomass and mortality rate estimates (reported in 2015) indicate the stock in the EU-assessed area is not within the biomass limits of the Eel Regulation and in most management units, anthropogenic mortality exceeds a level that can be expected to lead to recovery.FAO reports the total landings from Commercial fisheries in 2014 were about 3321 t of eel. Six countries account for 73% of the FAO landings: France, Egypt, UK, Netherlands, Sweden and Denmark. Five EU Member States have a glass eel fishery (France, UK, Spain, Portugal and Italy): some non-EU countries (e.g. Morocco) also have glass eel fisheries but data from these were not available for analysis. The best estimates of the total EU catch of glass eel in 2015 and 2016 were 51.6 and 59.2 t, respectively.About 10.6 million glass eels and 9.2 million yellow eels were stocked in 2015. Stocking is a component of many Eel Management Plans (EMPs) and in some cases the commitment could not be achieved in 2016 due to timing, market and other glass eel availability issues. Aquaculture production was about 4000–6500 t in 2015/2016 (data from FAO, FEAP and WGEEL Country Reports).WGEEL attempts to cross-check glass eel catch with records of their fate (consumption, restocking and aquaculture) reveal major discrepancies between reporting systems. About 32% of the catch for 2015 has no recorded fate (about 36% for 2016). EuroStat trade data show France and UK declared exports of glass eel to Hong Kong in 2015/2016 despite these being ‘banned’ by the EU application of CITES.The EU Eel Regulation effectively implements a Distributed Control System, in which common objectives (protection and recovery, minimal spawner production of 40% relative to the notional pristine production) are achieved by collective action (national management plans, reducing mortalities). Effective governance across the whole stock requires other areas to adopt the same approach of distributed control. Most non-EU areas have only recently joined this process, and further development - of reference points, assessment procedures, and feedback mechanisms - might be required, to cope with unforeseen complications and/or to familiarise local experts, and involve them in future standardisation processes. Additionally, reference points, assessment procedures and feedback mechanisms will need to be agreed for the whole distribution area.A mechanism needs to found between the EU and the ICES rules to facilitate feedback on the status of the implementation of the EMPs, as in the Eel Management Plan Evaluation workshop (WKEPEMP) in 2013 (ICES, 2013). This lack leaves a void between the formal Precautionary Advice and scientific support for the recovery plan on eel.Knowledge gaps and research needs were identified regarding impacts of pollutants and hydropower, habitat preferences, and monitoring across environments. A recent review shows that evidence on net benefits of eel stocking is inconclusive. Emerging threats include climate change, pollution and post-release mortalities from recreational fisheries. New opportunities include research on migratory triggers and habitat use, survey methods in large water bodies, protection for eel passing hydropower facilities, and coordinating eel management and data collection in the Mediterranean.
- Published
- 2016
48. Proactive prosociality in a cooperatively breeding corvid, the azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyana)
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa, Scheer, Clara, Bugnyar, Thomas, and Massen, Jorg J. M.
- Subjects
cooperative breeding ,Behavior, Animal ,prosociality ,cooperation ,animal behavior ,social behavior ,motivation ,physiology ,Animals ,Animal Behaviour ,animal ,Passeriformes ,Cooperative Behavior ,group service paradigm - Abstract
One of the contemporary hypotheses concerning the evolution of human altruism is the cooperative breeding hypothesis (CBH) which has recently been tested in non-human primates. Using a similar paradigm, we investigated prosociality in a cooperatively breeding corvid, the azure-winged magpie. We found that the magpies delivered food to their group members at high rates, and unlike other corvids, they did so without any cues provided by others. In two control conditions, the magpies stopped participating over time, indicating that they learned to discriminate prosocial tests from controls. Azure-winged magpies are thus the first birds that experimentally show proactive prosociality. Our findings are in line with the CBH; however, additional corvid species need to be tested in this promising paradigm.
- Published
- 2016
49. Unravelling theLineus ruber/viridisspecies complex (Nemertea, Heteronemertea)
- Author
-
Krämer, Daria, primary, Schmidt, Christian, additional, Podsiadlowski, Lars, additional, Beckers, Patrick, additional, Horn, Lisa, additional, and von Döhren, Jörn, additional
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. The link between the dog-human relationship and dogs' (Canis familiaris) performance in socio-cognitive tasks
- Author
-
Horn, Lisa
- Abstract
Die Beziehung zwischen Menschen und Hunden (Canis familiaris) begann vor mehr als 15.000 Jahren, als die Menschen erstmals die Vorfahren der heutigen Hunde domestizierten. Ein Merkmal, das die folgende Ausbreitung des Hundes in alle menschlichen Kulturen ermöglichte und seine Position als eine der erfolgreichsten domestizierten Tierarten stärkte, war seine Fähigkeit, enge und sehr individuelle Beziehungen mit seinen menschlichen Besitzern einzugehen. Während heutzutage bekannt ist, dass diese Beziehungen einen starken positiven Effekt auf die Besitzer haben, wurden Fragen bezüglich des Einflusses der Hund-Mensch-Beziehung auf das Verhalten und die kognitiven Fähigkeiten des Hundes bisher größtenteils vernachlässigt. In dieser Dissertation untersuche ich die Verbindung zwischen der Hund-Mensch-Beziehung und den Leistungen von Hunden in sozial-kognitiven Aufgaben. In der ersten Studie testete ich, ob die An- oder Abwesenheit des Besitzers oder sein Verhalten die Leistungen des Hundes in einer Problemlöseaufgabe beeinflussten. Ich zeigte, dass das Verhalten des Besitzers nur wenig Einfluss auf den Hund hatte, die Anwesenheit des Besitzers hingegen dem Hund eine sichere Basis für die Exploration während des Versuchs bot. In der zweiten Studie führte ich zwei Experimente durch, in denen ich Hunde mit zwei verschiedenen Problemlöseaufgaben konfrontierte. Im ersten Experiment konnte ich zeigen, dass die Blicke der Hunde zu ihrem Besitzer durch die vorangegangene Interaktion beeinflusst wurden. Im zweiten Experiment konnten die Hunde ihr Verhalten auch flexibel an die situations-spezifischen Charakteristika von zwei vorher unbekannten Personen anpassen. Die dritte Studie konzentrierte sich schlussendlich auf die Frage, ob die Aufmerksamkeit von Hunden gegenüber den Handlungen eines bestimmten Menschen von der Beziehung zu diesem Menschen oder nur von Vertrautheit beeinflusst wurde. Die Ergebnisse zeigten, dass die Vertrautheit allein nicht ausschlaggebend für die große Aufmerksamkeit von Hunden gegenüber ihren Besitzern sein konnte, weil sie nicht zwischen bekannten und unbekannten Menschen unterschieden, solange die Vertrautheit nicht auch mit einer engen Beziehung gekoppelt war. Die Ergebnisse dieser Studien weisen auf beträchtliche wechselseitige Verbindung zwischen der Hund-Mensch-Beziehung und den Leistungen von Hunden in sozial-kognitiven Aufgaben hin, welche umfangreiche Implikationen für kognitive Tests mit Hunden haben., The relationship between humans and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) started when humans tamed the dogs’ ancestors about 15.000 years ago. One trait that enabled dogs’ subsequent spread through every human culture and strengthened its position as one of the most successful domesticated species was their ability to form close and highly individualized relationships with their human owners. While nowadays it is known that those relationships have a strong beneficial effect on the owners, questions regarding the influence of the dog-human relationship on dogs’ behaviour and cognitive abilities have largely been neglected. Here, I set out to specifically investigate the connection between the dog-human relationship and dogs’ performance in socio-cognitive tasks. In the first study I tested if the presence/absence or the behaviour of the owner had an influence on dogs’ performance in a problem-solving task. I showed that while the presence of the owner provided a secure base for the dogs during the experiment, the behaviour of the owner had very little effect. In the second study I conducted two experiments, in which I confronted dogs with two problem-solving situations. In the first experiment I could show that dogs’ looking behaviour directed at their owners was influenced by owners’ previous interaction style. In the second experiment dogs were also able to flexibly adjust their behaviour to the situation-specific characteristics of two previously unknown humans. Finally, the third study was centred on the question whether the attention of dogs toward the actions of humans depended on the type of relationship between the dog and the human or merely on social familiarity. Results showed that familiarity alone could not account for dogs’ increased attention towards their owners since they did not differentiate between familiar and unfamiliar people, unless familiarity was linked to having a high-quality relationship. The results of these studies point to a substantial interconnection between the dog-human relationship and dogs’ performances in socio-cognitive tasks, which has wide implications for cognitive testing in domestic dogs.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.