30 results on '"Jennifer Heidler-Gary"'
Search Results
2. The NIHSS-Plus: Improving Cognitive Assessment with the NIHSS
- Author
-
Rebecca F. Gottesman, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Melissa Newhart, and Argye E. Hillis
- Subjects
Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Background: The National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) has been criticized for limited representation of cognitive dysfunction and bias towards dominant hemisphere functions. Patients may therefore receive a low NIHSS score despite a fairly large stroke. A broader scale including simple cognitive tests would improve the clinical and research utility of the NIHSS.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Acute Recovery of Oral Word Production Following Stroke: Patterns of Performance as Predictors of Recovery
- Author
-
Lauren Cloutman, Melissa Newhart, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, and Argye E. Hillis
- Subjects
Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Background: Impairments in oral word production are common at the onset of stroke. The identification of factors that predict early recovery has important implications for identifying those at greater risk of continued impaired functioning, and the management of the patient's care following discharge.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Characteristics and Reversibility of Dementia in Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus
- Author
-
Priyanka Chaudhry, Siddharth Kharkar, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Argye E. Hillis, Melissa Newhart, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Daniele Rigamonti, Paul Wang, David N. Irani, and Michael A. Williams
- Subjects
Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Studies of the cognitive outcome after shunt insertion for treatment of Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus have reported widely mixed results. We prospectively studied performance of 60 patients with Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus on a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests before and after shunt surgery to determine which cognitive functions improve with shunt insertion. We also administered a subset of cognitive tests before and after temporary controlled drainage of cerebrospinal fluid to determine if change on this brief subset of tests after drainage could predict which patients would show cognitive improvement three to six months after shunt insertion. There was a significant improvement in learning, retention, and delayed recall of verbal memory three to six months after surgery (using paired t-tests). The majority (74%) of patients showed significant improvement (by at least one standard deviation) on at least one of the memory tests. Absence of improvement on verbal memory after temporary drainage of cerebrospinal fluid had a high negative predictive value for improvement on memory tests at 3–6 months after surgery (96%; p = 0.0005). Also, the magnitude of improvement from Baseline to Post-Drainage on few specific tests of learning and recall significantly predicted the magnitude of improvement after shunt surgery on the same tests (r2 = 0.32–0.58; p = 0.04–0.001). Results indicate that testing before and after temporary drainage may be useful in predicting which patients are less likely to improve in memory with shunting.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Patterns of breakdown in spelling in primary progressive aphasia
- Author
-
Jennifer T. Crinion, Kyrana Tsapkini, Argye E. Hillis, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Maralyssa Bann, Kathryn Sepelyak, Zachary Epstein-Peterson, Melissa Newhart, and John Molitoris
- Subjects
Cognitive Neuroscience ,Semantic dementia ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Aphasiology ,Article ,Primary progressive aphasia ,Mental Processes ,Dysgraphia ,Aphasia ,Neural Pathways ,medicine ,Humans ,Agraphia ,Aged ,Brain Mapping ,Brain ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Spelling ,Word lists by frequency ,Aphasia, Primary Progressive ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Case-Control Studies ,Atrophy ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Introduction The objective of this study is to determine which cognitive processes underlying spelling are most affected in the three variants of primary progressive aphasia (PPA): Logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA), Semantic variant primary progressive aphasia (svPPA), and Nonfluent variant primary progressive aphasia (nfvPPA). Methods 23 PPA patients were administered The Johns Hopkins Dysgraphia Battery to assess spelling. Subtests evaluate for effects of word frequency, concreteness, word length, grammatical word class, lexicality (words vs pseudowords), and “regularity” by controlling for the other variables. Significant effects of each variable were identified with chi square tests. Responses on all spelling to dictation tests were scored by error type. 16 of the 23 subjects also had a high resolution MRI brain scan to identify areas of atrophy. Results We identified 4 patterns of spelling that could be explained by damage to one or more cognitive processes underlying spelling. Nine patients (3 unclassifiable, 4 with lvPPA, 2 with svPPA) had dysgraphia explicable by impaired access to lexical representations, with reliance on sublexical phonology-to-orthography conversion (POC). Two patients (with nfvPPA) showed dysgraphia explicable by impaired access to lexical representations and complete disruption of sublexical POC. Seven patients (4 with lvPPA, 1 with svPPA, 2 unclassifiable) showed dysgraphia explicable by impaired access to lexical-semantic representations and/or lexical representations with partially spared sublexical POC mechanisms. Five patients (1 with nfvPPA, 2 with svPPA, 1 with lvPPA, and 1 unclassifiable) showed dysgraphia explicable by impairment of the graphemic buffer. Conclusions Any cognitive process underlying spelling can be affected in PPA. Predominance of phonologically plausible errors, more accurate spelling of regular words than irregular words, and more accurate spelling of pseudowords than words (indicating spared POC mechanisms) may indicate a low probability of progression to nfvPPA.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Action versus animal naming fluency in subcortical dementia, frontal dementias, and Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
Lauren L. Cloutman, Daniele Rigamonti, Argye E. Hillis, Cameron Davis, Rebecca F. Gottesman, Jennifer T. Crinion, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Melissa Newhart, D. Soloman, and Abhay Moghekar
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Subcortical dementia ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Audiology ,Article ,Fluency ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Progressive nonfluent aphasia ,Alzheimer Disease ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Animals ,Humans ,Dementia ,Language ,Temporal cortex ,Verbal Behavior ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Frontotemporal Dementia ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Nerve Net ,Alzheimer's disease ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Psychomotor Performance ,Frontotemporal dementia - Abstract
Accumulating evidence indicates action naming may rely more on frontal-subcortical circuits, and noun naming may rely more on temporal cortex. Therefore, noun versus action fluency might distinguish frontal and subcortical dementias from cortical dementias primarily affecting temporal and/or parietal cortex such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD). We hypothesized patients with subcortical dementia, e.g., normal pressure hydrocephalus (NPH) and patients with dementias predominantly affecting frontal cortex, e.g., behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bv-FTD) and progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA) have more difficulty on action fluency versus noun fluency (e.g., animal naming). Patients with AD, who have temporo parietal cortical dysfunction, should have more difficulty on noun versus verb fluency. A total of 234 participants, including healthy controls (n = 20) and patients diagnosed with NPH (n =144), AD (n = 33), bv-FTD (n = 22) or PNFA (n = 15) were administered animal fluency, action fluency, and letter fluency tasks, and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, to control for dementia severity). NPH and bv-FTD/PNFA patients had significantly higher MMSE scores and animal fluency than AD patients (after adjusting for age), but their action fluency tended to be lower than in AD. Only NPH and bvFTD/PNFA patients showed significantly lower action verb than animal fluency. Results provide novel evidence that action naming relies more on frontal-subcortical circuits while noun naming relies more on temporoparietal cortex, indicating action verb fluency may be more sensitive than noun fluency, particularly for detecting frontal-subcortical dysfunction.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Severity of Hypoperfusion in Distinct Brain Regions Predicts Severity of Hemispatial Neglect in Different Reference Frames
- Author
-
Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Cameron Davis, Argye E. Hillis, Julia Thorn, Rebecca F. Gottesman, Peyman Shirani, and Melissa Newhart
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,Perfusion Imaging ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Ischemia ,macromolecular substances ,Severity of Illness Index ,Article ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Central nervous system disease ,Angular gyrus ,Predictive Value of Tests ,Internal medicine ,medicine ,Humans ,Stroke ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Advanced and Specialized Nursing ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,business.industry ,Cognitive disorder ,Brain ,Magnetic resonance imaging ,Hemispatial neglect ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Surgery ,nervous system ,Cardiology ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Cardiology and Cardiovascular Medicine ,business - Abstract
Background and Purpose— Hemispatial neglect is among the most common and disabling consequences of right hemisphere stroke. A variety of variables have been associated with the presence or severity of neglect but have not evaluated the independent effects of location, severity, and volume of ischemia. Few have determined areas involved in different types of neglect. We identified the contributions of these variables to severity of viewer-centered versus stimulus-centered neglect in acute ischemic right hemisphere stroke. Methods— We studied 137 patients within 24 hours of stroke onset with MR diffusion- and perfusion-weighted imaging and a test of hemispatial neglect that distinguishes between viewer-centered and stimulus-centered neglect. Using multivariable linear regression, we identified the independent contributions of severity of ischemia in specific locations, volume of ischemia, and age in accounting for severity of each neglect type. Results— Severity of hypoperfusion in angular gyrus was the only variable that significantly and independently contributed to severity of viewer-centered neglect. Volume of dysfunctional tissue and hypoperfusion in posterior frontal cortex also accounted for some variability in severity of viewer-centered neglect. Severity of hypoperfusion of superior temporal cortex was the only variable that independently and significantly contributed to severity of stimulus-centered neglect. Conclusions— Location, severity, and volume of ischemia together determine the type and severity of neglect after right hemisphere stroke. Results also show that perfusion-weighted MRI can be used as a semiquantitative measure of tissue dysfunction in acute stroke and can account for a substantial proportion of the variability in functional deficits in the acute stage.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Neural Substrates of Visuospatial Processing in Distinct Reference Frames: Evidence from Unilateral Spatial Neglect
- Author
-
Vijay Kannan, Melissa Newhart, Mikolaj A. Pawlak, Jared Medina, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Argye E. Hillis, Edward H. Herskovits, and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Visual perception ,Perfusion Imaging ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Visual system ,Brain mapping ,Article ,Functional Laterality ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Visual processing ,Inferior temporal gyrus ,Orientation ,Humans ,Visual Pathways ,Cerebrum ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Brain Mapping ,Cerebral Infarction ,Middle Aged ,Stroke ,Space Perception ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Reference frame - Abstract
There is evidence for different levels of visuospatial processing with their own frames of reference: viewer-centered, stimulus-centered, and object-centered. The neural locus of these levels can be explored by examining lesion location in subjects with unilateral spatial neglect (USN) manifest in these reference frames. Most studies regarding the neural locus of USN have treated it as a homogenous syndrome, resulting in conflicting results. In order to further explore the neural locus of visuospatial processes differentiated by frame of reference, we presented a battery of tests to 171 subjects within 48 hr after right supratentorial ischemic stroke before possible structural and/or functional reorganization. The battery included MR perfusion weighted imaging (which shows hypoperfused regions that may be dysfunctional), diffusion weighted imaging (which reveals areas of infarct or dense ischemia shortly after stroke onset), and tests designed to disambiguate between various types of neglect. Results were consistent with a dorsal/ventral stream distinction in egocentric/allocentric processing. We provide evidence that portions of the dorsal stream of visual processing, including the right supramarginal gyrus, are involved in spatial encoding in egocentric coordinates, whereas parts of the ventral stream (including the posterior inferior temporal gyrus) are involved in allocentric encoding.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Therapy for naming deficits in two variants of primary progressive aphasia
- Author
-
Melissa Newhart, Vijay Kannan, Argye E. Hillis, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, and Lauren L. Cloutman
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Semantic dementia ,Disease ,Aphasiology ,Audiology ,LPN and LVN ,medicine.disease ,Language and Linguistics ,Primary progressive aphasia ,Neurology ,Otorhinolaryngology ,Progressive nonfluent aphasia ,Aphasia ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Dementia ,In patient ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Background: Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) refers to a progressive and selective decline in language due to neurodegenerative disease. There are three variants of PPA, progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA), semantic dementia (SD), and logopaenic progressive aphasia (LPA). All variants include impaired object naming, but distinct underlying deficits might interfere with naming. Therefore, individuals with different types of PPA may respond differently to naming therapy. Aims: To identify differences in patterns of success and generalisation in response to the same treatment in patient with LPA and a patient with SD. Furthermore, we wished to identify whether the treatment effect was item specific (trained words) or generalised to untrained words in trained or untrained categories. Methods & Procedures: Participants included an individual with LPA and one with SD. An assessment of lexical processing was administered before and after a naming treatment to assess underlying deficits and generalisation effec...
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Where (in the brain) do semantic errors come from?
- Author
-
Andrew Lee, Vijay Kannan, Melissa Newhart, Rebecca F. Gottesman, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Priyanka Chaudhry, Edward H. Herskovits, Lauren L. Cloutman, Mikolaj A. Pawlak, Argye E. Hillis, and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Hemispheric stroke ,Concept Formation ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Semantics ,Functional Laterality ,Article ,Stroke onset ,Text mining ,Reference Values ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Aged ,Aged, 80 and over ,Brain Mapping ,Language Disorders ,business.industry ,Cognition ,Lexical access ,Middle Aged ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Temporal Lobe ,Stroke ,Comprehension ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Case-Control Studies ,medicine.symptom ,business ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Background Semantic errors result from the disruption of access either to semantics or to lexical representations. One way to determine the origins of these errors is to evaluate comprehension of words that elicit semantic errors in naming. We hypothesized that in acute stroke there are different brain regions where dysfunction results in semantic errors in both naming and comprehension versus those with semantic errors in oral naming alone. Methods A consecutive series of 196 patients with acute left hemispheric stroke who met inclusion criteria were evaluated with oral naming and spoken word/picture verification tasks and magnetic resonance imaging within 48 h of stroke onset. We evaluated the relationship between tissue dysfunction in 10 pre-specified Brodmann's areas (BA) and the production of coordinate semantic errors resulting from (1) semantic deficits or (2) lexical access deficits. Results Semantic errors arising from semantic deficits were most associated with tissue dysfunction/infarct of left BA 22. Semantic errors resulting from lexical access deficits were associated with hypoperfusion/infarct of left BA 37. Conclusion Our study shows that semantic errors arising from damage to distinct cognitive processes reflect dysfunction of different brain regions.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Hemispherectomy sustained before adulthood does not cause persistent hemispatial neglect
- Author
-
Melissa Newhart, Argye E. Hillis, Elisabeth B. Marsh, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Eric H. Kossoff, John M. Freeman, Eileen P.G. Vining, and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,Hemispherectomy ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,medicine.medical_treatment ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Audiology ,Functional Laterality ,Developmental psychology ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Young Adult ,Epilepsy ,Adaptation, Psychological ,medicine ,Humans ,Young adult ,Child ,media_common ,Neuronal Plasticity ,Critical Period, Psychological ,Contralateral hemisphere ,Age Factors ,Hemispatial neglect ,Recovery of Function ,Take over ,Gap detection ,medicine.disease ,Adaptation, Physiological ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
Hemispatial neglect has been well established in adults following acute ischemic stroke, but has rarely been investigated in children and young adults following brain injury. It is known that young brains have a tremendous potential for reorganization; however, there is controversy as to whether functions are assumed by the opposite hemisphere, or perilesional areas in the same hemisphere. Patients with intractable epilepsy who undergo hemispherectomy for treatment are missing the entire cortex on one side following surgery. In these patients, only the opposite hemisphere is available to assume function. Therefore, they provide the unique opportunity to determine in what cases the left or right hemisphere can take over the spatial attention functions of the opposite hemisphere following damage. The objective of this study was to determine the incidence and types of hemispatial neglect in children and young adults following both right- and left-sided hemispherectomy; which types of spatial attention functions can be assumed by the opposite hemisphere; and whether factors like their age at time of surgery, handedness, or gender influence recovery.Thirty-two children and young adults who had previously undergone hemispherectomy were administered two tests to evaluate for two types of hemispatial neglect: a gap detection test and a line cancellation test. Egocentric neglect was defined as significantly more omissions of targets on the contralesional versus ipsilesional side of the page (by chi square analysis; p.05). Allocentric neglect was defined as significantly more errors in detecting contralesional versus ipsilesional gaps in circles.Only one of the patients displayed statistically significant hemispatial egocentric neglect on the line cancellation test, and none of the patients displayed statistically significant egocentric or allocentric neglect on the gap detection test.These results imply that reorganization to the contralateral hemisphere occurs peri-hemispherectomy, as there are no perilesional areas to assume function.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Unilateral neglect is more severe and common in older patients with right hemispheric stroke
- Author
-
Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Argye E. Hillis, Vijay Kannan, Cameron Davis, Rebecca F. Gottesman, and Melissa Newhart
- Subjects
Adult ,Brain Infarction ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Severity of Illness Index ,Functional Laterality ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Central nervous system disease ,Atrophy ,Internal medicine ,Severity of illness ,medicine ,Humans ,cardiovascular diseases ,Stroke ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Cerebral infarction ,Vascular disease ,Age Factors ,Articles ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Unilateral neglect ,Physical therapy ,Cardiology ,Regression Analysis ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology - Abstract
Introduction: Unilateral neglect after acute right hemispheric stroke significantly impedes poststroke recovery. We studied patients with right hemispheric stroke to determine whether increasing age was associated with more frequent or more severe neglect. Methods: Eight neglect tests within 5 days of symptom onset (and within 24 hours of admission) were administered to 204 subjects with acute right hemispheric stroke. Size of infarct was measured, and neglect tests were scored as percent error. “Any neglect” was defined by an elevated neglect test score, standardized relative to a group of normal controls. Results: When tested for neglect soon after acute stroke admission, 69.6% of subjects older than 65 years had “any neglect” (defined by comparison to a group of normal controls), compared with 49.4% of subjects aged 65 years and younger ( p = 0.008). For every additional 10 years of age, patients were 1.83 times as likely to have neglect, even after adjusting for diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) infarct volume and NIH Stroke Scale (NIHSS) score (95% CI 1.38–2.43). In addition, DWI volume and NIHSS independently predicted neglect. Score on virtually all of the neglect tests worsened as an effect of age. Percentage error on a line cancellation task was 3.8% higher for every additional 10 years of age, after adjustment for DWI volume and NIHSS ( p = 0.006). Similar results were found for other neglect tests. Conclusions: Increasing age in patients with acute right hemispheric stroke significantly increases the odds of unilateral neglect as well as severity of neglect, independently of size of the stroke or NIH Stroke Scale score. The reason for this finding in older patients may be because they have more brain atrophy and may be less able to compensate for cerebral infarction, or because they tend to have more cardioembolic strokes, which may be more cortically based.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Gender differences in unilateral spatial neglect within 24 hours of ischemic stroke
- Author
-
Jonathan T. Kleinman, Melissa Newhart, Cameron Davis, Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, and Rebecca F. Gottesman
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Spatial ability ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Neurological disorder ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Functional Laterality ,Article ,Brain Ischemia ,Developmental psychology ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Young Adult ,Sex Factors ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Stroke ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Cerebral infarction ,Cognitive disorder ,Hemispatial neglect ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Space Perception ,Visual Perception ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
Hemispatial neglect is a common and disabling consequence of stroke. Previous reports examining the relationship between gender and the incidence of unilateral spatial neglect (USN) have included either a large numbers of patients with few neglect tests or small numbers of patients with multiple tests. To determine if USN was more common and/or severe in men or women, we examined a large group of patients (312 right-handed) within 24 hours of acute right hemisphere ischemic stroke. Multiple spatial neglect tasks were used to increase the sensitivity of neglect detection. No differences based upon gender were observed for the prevalence, severity, or a combined task measure of USN.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Site of the ischemic penumbra as a predictor of potential for recovery of functions
- Author
-
Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Vijay Kannan, Rebecca F. Gottesman, Lauren L. Cloutman, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Melissa Newhart, Rafael H. Llinas, E. Aldrich, L. Gold, and Argye E. Hillis
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Lateralization of brain function ,Brain Ischemia ,Predictive Value of Tests ,Internal medicine ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Effective diffusion coefficient ,Stroke ,Aged ,Aged, 80 and over ,Temporal cortex ,Penumbra ,Recovery of Function ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Temporal Lobe ,Surgery ,Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Brodmann area 37 ,Reperfusion Injury ,Predictive value of tests ,Cardiology ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Brodmann area - Abstract
Background and Purpose: Diffusion–perfusion mismatch has been used to estimate salvageable tissue and predict potential for recovery in acute stroke. Location of the salvageable tissue may be as important as volume or percentage in predicting potential for recovery of specific functions. Impaired naming, a common and disabling deficit after left hemisphere stroke, is often associated with tissue dysfunction of left Brodmann area (BA) 37, posterior inferior temporal cortex. We tested the hypothesis that the presence of diffusion–perfusion mismatch within left BA 37 predicts probability and extent of short-term improvement of naming. Methods: One hundred five patients with acute left hemisphere ischemic stroke had diffusion-weighted imaging, perfusion-weighted imaging, a test of picture naming, and other language tests at admission and 2 to 4 days later. Linear regression was used to determine whether diffusion–perfusion mismatch in any BA in language cortex, total volume of mismatch, or diffusion or perfusion abnormality predicted degree of improvement in naming by days 3 to 5. Results: The presence of >20% diffusion–perfusion mismatch in left BA 37 and total volumes of diffusion and perfusion abnormality at day 1 each independently predicted degree of improvement in naming. Mismatch in this area did not predict the degree of improvement in other language tests or the NIH Stroke Scale in this study. Conclusions/Relevance: Diffusion–perfusion mismatch in left Brodmann area 37 was strongly associated with acute improvement in naming, independently of volume or percentage of total mismatch or diffusion or perfusion abnormality. These data indicate that mismatch in a particular area is a marker of salvageable tissue and an important predictor of potential for recovery of functions that depend on that area. Location of mismatch before treatment may help to predict potential benefits of reperfusion. GLOSSARY: ADC = apparent diffusion coefficient; BA = Brodmann area; DWI = diffusion-weighted imaging; NIHSS = NIH Stroke Scale; PWI = perfusion-weighted imaging; TE = echo time; TR = repetition time; TTP = time to peak.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Neural regions essential for reading and spelling of words and pseudowords
- Author
-
Argye E. Hillis, Melissa Newhart, Lisa E. Philipose, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Edward H. Herskovits, Elisabeth B. Marsh, Mikolaj A. Pawlak, and Rebecca F. Gottesman
- Subjects
Adult ,medicine.medical_specialty ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Audiology ,Logistic regression ,Supramarginal gyrus ,Reading (process) ,medicine ,Humans ,Aged ,Language ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Brain Mapping ,Communication ,Language Tests ,Fusiform gyrus ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,business.industry ,Brain ,Magnetic resonance imaging ,Odds ratio ,Middle Aged ,Confidence interval ,Spelling ,Stroke ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Reading ,Neurology ,Neurology (clinical) ,Nerve Net ,business ,Psychology - Abstract
Objective To identify dysfunctional brain regions critical for impaired reading/spelling of words/pseudowords by evaluating acute stroke patients on lexical tests and magnetic resonance imaging, before recovery or reorganization of structure–function relationships. Methods A series of 106 consenting patients were administered oral reading and spelling tests within 24 hours of left supratentorial stroke onset. Patients underwent diffusion- and perfusion-weighted magnetic resonance examination the same day to identify regions of hypoperfusion/infarct of 16 Brodmann areas. Results Simultaneous logistic regression analysis demonstrated that dysfunction of left Brodmann areas 40 (supramarginal gyrus) and 37 (posterior-inferior temporal/fusiform gyrus) best predicted impairment in reading words (odds ratio [OR], 6.20 [95% confidence interval (CI), 1.54–24.96] and 2.71 [95% CI, 0.87–8.45], respectively), reading pseudowords (OR, 39.65 [95% CI 3.9–400.78] and 4.41 [95% CI, 1.1–17.51], respectively), spelling words (OR, 14.11 [95% CI 1.37–144.93] and 7.41 [95% CI, 1.48–37.24], respectively), and spelling pseudowords (OR, 4.84 [95% CI, 0.73–32.13] and 7.74 [95% CI, 1.56–38.51], respectively). Whole-brain voxel-wise analyses demonstrated voxel clusters within these regions that were most strongly associated with task deficits. Interpretation Results indicate that a shared network of regions including parts of left Brodmann areas 37 and 40 is necessary for reading and spelling of words and pseudowords. Further studies may define the precise roles of these brain regions in language. Identification of any neural regions specific to one of these tasks or one type of stimuli will require study of more patients with selective deficits. Ann Neurol 2007
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Right hemispatial neglect: Frequency and characterization following acute left hemisphere stroke
- Author
-
Rebecca F. Gottesman, Melissa Newhart, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Argye E. Hillis, Cameron Davis, and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Adult ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Functional Laterality ,Article ,Lateralization of brain function ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Parietal Lobe ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,education ,Stroke ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,Temporal cortex ,education.field_of_study ,Cerebral infarction ,Hemispatial neglect ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Temporal Lobe ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Space Perception ,Cerebral hemisphere ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
The frequency of various types of unilateral spatial neglect and associated areas of neural dysfunction after left hemisphere stroke are not well characterized. Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) in distinct spatial reference frames have been identified after acute right, but not left hemisphere stroke. We studied 47 consecutive right handed patients within 48 hours of left hemisphere stroke to determine the frequency and distribution of types of right USN using cognitive testing and MRI imaging. The distribution of USN types was different from the previously reported distribution following acute right hemisphere stroke. In this left hemisphere stroke population, allocentric neglect was more frequent than egocentric neglect.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Neural regions essential for distinct cognitive processes underlying picture naming
- Author
-
Melissa Newhart, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Andrew L. Lee, Cameron Davis, Jessica Deleon, Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, and Rebecca F. Gottesman
- Subjects
Lateralization of brain function ,Angular gyrus ,Cortex (anatomy) ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,Language disorder ,Temporal cortex ,Memory Disorders ,Language Tests ,Fusiform gyrus ,Brain ,Cognition ,medicine.disease ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Stroke ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Reading ,Linear Models ,Visual Perception ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Cognition Disorders ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
We hypothesized that distinct cognitive processes underlying oral and written picture naming depend on intact function of different, but overlapping, regions of the left hemisphere cortex, such that the distribution of tissue dysfunction in various areas can predict the component of the naming process that is disrupted. To test this hypothesis, we evaluated 116 individuals within 24 h of acute ischaemic stroke using a battery of oral and written naming and other lexical tests, and with magnetic resonance diffusion and perfusion imaging to identify the areas of tissue dysfunction. Discriminant function analysis, using the degree of hypoperfusion in various Brodmann's areas--BA 22 (including Wernicke's area), BA 44 (part of Broca's area), BA 45 (part of Broca's area), BA 21 (inferior temporal cortex), BA 37 (posterior, inferior temporal/fusiform gyrus), BA 38 (anterior temporal cortex) and BA 39 (angular gyrus)--as discriminant variables, classified patients on the basis of the primary component of the naming process that was impaired (defined as visual, semantics, modality-independent lexical access, phonological word form, orthographic word form and motor speech by the pattern of performance and types of errors across lexical tasks). Additionally, linear regression analysis demonstrated that the areas contributing the most information to the identification of patients with particular levels of impairment in the naming process were largely consistent with evidence for the roles of these regions from functional imaging. This study provides evidence that the level of impairment in the naming process reflects the distribution of tissue dysfunction in particular regions of the left anterior, inferior and posterior middle/superior temporal cortex, posterior inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex. While occipital cortex is also critical for picture naming, it is likely that bilateral occipital damage is necessary to disrupt visual recognition. These findings provide new evidence that a network of brain regions supports naming, but separate components of this network are differentially required for distinct cognitive processes or representations underlying the complex task of naming pictures.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Restoring Cerebral Blood Flow Reveals Neural Regions Critical for Naming
- Author
-
Rebecca F. Gottesman, Peter B. Barker, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Priyanka Chaudhry, Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Rafael H. Llinas, E. Aldrich, Melissa Newhart, and Robert J. Wityk
- Subjects
Male ,Ischemia ,Brain mapping ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Stroke ,Cerebral Cortex ,Aphasia, Broca ,Brain Mapping ,Fusiform gyrus ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,General Neuroscience ,Magnetic resonance imaging ,Recovery of Function ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Treatment Outcome ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Cerebral blood flow ,Cerebral cortex ,Cerebrovascular Circulation ,Reperfusion ,Female ,Nerve Net ,medicine.symptom ,Brief Communications ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
We identified areas of the brain that are critical for naming pictures of objects, using a new methodology for testing which components of a network of brain regions are essential for that task. We identified areas of hypoperfusion and structural damage with magnetic resonance perfusion- and diffusion-weighted imaging immediately after stroke in 87 individuals with impaired picture naming. These individuals were reimaged after 3–5 d, after a subset of patients underwent intervention to restore normal blood flow, to determine areas of the brain that had reperfused. We identified brain regions in which reperfusion was associated with improvement in picture naming. Restored blood flow to left posterior middle temporal/fusiform gyrus, Broca's area, and/or Wernicke's area accounted for most acute improvement after stroke. Results show that identifying areas of reperfusion that are associated with acute improvement of a function can reveal the brain regions essential for that function.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Naming and comprehension in primary progressive aphasia: The influence of grammatical word class
- Author
-
Melissa Newhart, MA Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Shannon Chang, Thomas H. Bak, and Lynda Ken
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,Semantic dementia ,Aphasiology ,LPN and LVN ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Language and Linguistics ,Comprehension ,Primary progressive aphasia ,Atrophy ,Neurology ,Otorhinolaryngology ,Progressive nonfluent aphasia ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Frontotemporal dementia ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Background: Various clinical types of primary progressive aphasia have been associated with distinct areas of atrophy and pathological changes. Therefore, differences in the patterns of language deterioration in the various types might reveal the types of language processes and representations that depend on the areas of brain that are disproportionately affected. Aims: To test the hypotheses (1) that individuals with progressive nonfluent aphasia (associated with left posterior, inferior frontal, and insular atrophy) show progressive motor speech impairment and disproportionate deterioration in naming actions relative to objects, and (2) that individuals with semantic dementia (associated with anterior and inferior temporal atrophy) show disproportionate deterioration in semantic representations of objects relative to actions. Methods & Procedures: The study population consisted of 56 participants with primary progressive aphasia, including 27 with progressive nonfluent aphasia, 16 with semantic dementia...
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Motor extinction in distinct reference frames: a double dissociation
- Author
-
Jennifer, Heidler-Gary, Mikolaj, Pawlak, Edward H, Herskovits, Melissa, Newhart, Cameron, Davis, Lydia A, Trupe, and Argye E, Hillis
- Subjects
Brain Infarction ,Male ,social sciences ,Extinction ,Middle Aged ,humanities ,Functional Laterality ,Perceptual Disorders ,bimanual motor ,left space ,left body ,Humans ,Female ,Other ,Psychomotor Disorders ,Aged - Abstract
Objective: Test the hypothesis that right hemisphere stroke can cause extinction of left hand movements or movements of either hand held in left space, when both are used simultaneously, possibly depending on lesion site. Methods: 93 non-hemiplegic patients with acute right hemisphere stroke were tested for motor extinction by pressing a counter rapidly for one minute with the right hand, left hand, or both simultaneously with their hands held at their sides, or crossed over midline. Results: We identified two distinct types of motor extinction in separate patients; 20 patients extinguished left hand movements held in left or right space (left canonical body extinction); the most significantly associated voxel cluster of ischemic tissue was in the right temporal white matter. Seven patients extinguished either hand held in left space (left space extinction), and the most significantly associated voxel cluster of ischemic tissue was in right parietal white matter. Conclusions: There was a double dissociation between left canonical body extinction and left space motor extinction. Left canonical body extinction seems to be associated with more dorsal (parietal) ischemia, and left canonical body extinction seems to be associated with more ventral (temporal) ischemia.
- Published
- 2012
21. Neuroanatomical correlates of oral reading in acute left hemispheric stroke
- Author
-
Lauren L. Cloutman, Cameron Davis, Melisssa Newhart, Argye E. Hillis, and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Adult ,Linguistics and Language ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Neurology ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Middle temporal gyrus ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Brain mapping ,Language and Linguistics ,Lateralization of brain function ,Functional Laterality ,Article ,Brain Ischemia ,Speech and Hearing ,Reading (process) ,medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,Stroke ,media_common ,Language ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Brain ,Magnetic resonance imaging ,medicine.disease ,Functional imaging ,Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Reading ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Oral reading is a complex skill involving the interaction of orthographic, phonological, and semantic processes. Functional imaging studies with non-impaired adult readers have identified a widely distributed network of frontal, inferior parietal, posterior temporal, and occipital brain regions involved in the task. However, while functional imaging can identify cortical regions engaged in the process under examination, it cannot identify those brain regions essential for the task. The current study aimed to identify those neuroanatomical regions critical for successful oral reading by examining the relationship between word and nonword oral reading deficits and areas of tissue dysfunction in acute stroke. We evaluated 91 patients with left hemisphere ischemic stroke with a test of oral word and nonword reading, and magnetic resonance diffusion-weighted and perfusion-weighted imaging, within 24–48 hours of stroke onset. A voxel-wise statistical map showed that impairments in word and nonword reading were associated with a distributed network of brain regions, including the inferior and middle frontal gyri, the middle temporal gyrus, the supramarginal and angular gyri, and the middle occipital gyrus. In addition, lesions associated with word deficits were found to be distributed more frontally, while nonword deficits were associated with lesions distributed more posteriorly.
- Published
- 2009
22. A neural network critical for spelling
- Author
-
Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Jennifer T. Crinion, Leila Gingis, Melissa Newhart, Cameron Davis, and Lauren L. Cloutman
- Subjects
Male ,Time Factors ,Writing ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Neuropsychological Tests ,computer.software_genre ,Lateralization of brain function ,Article ,Brain Ischemia ,White matter ,Voxel ,medicine ,Humans ,Prefrontal cortex ,Stroke ,Analysis of Variance ,Language Tests ,Working memory ,Brain ,Linguistics ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Spelling ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Memory, Short-Term ,Neurology ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,computer ,Magnetic Resonance Angiography - Abstract
We aimed to identify neuroanatomical regions associated with deficits to the graphemic buffer, a working memory component of the spelling system that holds the sequence of letter identities during production. We evaluated 331 patients with left hemisphere ischemic stroke with various spelling tests and magnetic resonance diffusion-weighted imaging and perfusion-weighted imaging, within 48 hours of stroke onset. A voxel-wise statistical map showed that ischemia in voxels in posterior and inferior frontal and parietal cortex, subcortical white matter underlying prefrontal cortex, lateral occipital gyrus, or caudate was associated with impairment in maintaining the sequence of letter identities while spelling.
- Published
- 2009
23. Distinctions between the dementia in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with frontotemporal dementia and the dementia of Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
Argye E. Hillis and Jennifer Heidler-Gary
- Subjects
Adult ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Disease ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Personality changes ,Alzheimer Disease ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Dementia ,Humans ,Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis ,Psychiatry ,Aged ,Aged, 80 and over ,Analysis of Variance ,business.industry ,Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis ,Neuropsychology ,Age Factors ,Cognition ,General Medicine ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Cognitive test ,Neurology ,Neurology (clinical) ,business ,Mental Status Schedule ,Frontotemporal dementia - Abstract
The clinical entity of Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis with Frontotemporal Dementia (ALS-FTD) has only recently been recognized as an important neurodegenerative disease. As in isolated FTD, the behavioral and personality changes in ALS-FTD might be more characteristic than its cognitive changes. We aimed to characterize the behavioral and cognitive deficits in ALS-FTD, and contrast this profile with that of the most common form of dementia, AD, to assist ALS clinicians in recognizing the syndrome early in its course. Specifically, we hypothesized that a modified version of the Frontal Behavioral Inventory (FBI-mod), a brief questionnaire self-administered by a caregiver, along with just a few cognitive tests, would be clinically useful in distinguishing the dementia in ALS-FTD from the dementia of AD. We administered a battery of neuropsychological tests to 15 patients who met established criteria for Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis with Frontotemporal Dementia and to 30 patients who met established criteria for probable Alzheimer's disease. The FBI-mod was completed by caregivers. We found that the FBI-mod, age-corrected Z scores for the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), a test of delayed recall, and a word fluency measure together discriminated between ALS-FTD and AD. ALS-FTD was characterized by more abnormal FBI scores and poor word fluency, in the presence of relatively normal overall cognitive status (MMSE) and/or delayed recall.
- Published
- 2007
24. Neural networks essential for naming and word comprehension
- Author
-
Jonathan T. Kleinman, Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Lynda Ken, and Melissa Newhart
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Word error rate ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Brain mapping ,Functional Laterality ,Task (project management) ,Communication disorder ,Aphasia ,Parietal Lobe ,Neural Pathways ,medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,Language disorder ,Aged ,Cerebral Cortex ,Brain Mapping ,Language Tests ,Cognitive disorder ,General Medicine ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Temporal Lobe ,Frontal Lobe ,Functional imaging ,Stroke ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Acute Disease ,Linear Models ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Magnetic Resonance Angiography ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Lesion/deficit association studies of aphasia commonly focus on one brain region as primarily responsible for a particular language deficit. However, functional imaging and some lesion studies indicate that multiple brain regions are likely necessary for any language task. We tested 156 acute stroke patients on basic language tasks (naming and spoken and written word comprehension) and magnetic resonance diffusion and perfusion imaging to determine the relative contributions of various brain regions to each task. Multivariate linear regression analysis indicated that the error rate on each task was best predicted by dysfunction in several perisylvian regions, with both common and distinct regions for the 3 tasks.
- Published
- 2007
25. Neural correlates of modality-specific spatial extinction
- Author
-
Argye E. Hillis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Lynda Ken, Peter B. Barker, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, Eric Aldrich, Melissa Newhart, and Shannon Chang
- Subjects
Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Sensation ,Brain mapping ,Extinction, Psychological ,Perceptual Disorders ,Superior temporal gyrus ,Cortex (anatomy) ,Visual extinction ,medicine ,Humans ,natural sciences ,Stroke ,Aged ,Brain Mapping ,Chi-Square Distribution ,Brain ,Inferior parietal lobule ,social sciences ,Middle Aged ,musculoskeletal system ,medicine.disease ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,humanities ,Visual field ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Extinction (neurology) ,Female ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,geographic locations - Abstract
Sites of lesions responsible for visual, tactile, and/or motor extinction have not been clearly identified. We sought to determine the frequency of extinction in various modalities immediately after acute ischemic stroke, the rate of co-occurrence of extinction across modalities, and areas of infarct and/or hypoperfusion associated with each modality of extinction. A total of 148 patients with right supratentorial stroke were studied. In Study 1, 88 patients without hemiplegia, hemianesthesia, or visual field cuts were tested within 24 hours of onset for visual, tactile, and motor extinction, and underwent magnetic resonance diffusion and perfusion imaging. Associations between modality of extinction and areas of neural dysfunction (hypoperfusion/infarct) were identified. Of the 88 patients, 19 had only tactile extinction, 8 had only visual extinction, 12 had only motor extinction, 14 had extinction in two or more modalities, and 35 had no extinction. Tactile extinction was associated with neural dysfunction in the inferior parietal lobule; visual extinction was associated with dysfunction in the visual association cortex; and motor extinction was associated with neural dysfunction in the superior temporal gyrus. In Study 2, data from 60 patients who were excluded from Study 1 because of motor deficits were analyzed in the same way to determine whether frontal lesions contributed to visual or tactile extinction. Results again demonstrated that tactile extinction is associated with inferior parietal dysfunction, and visual extinction is associated with dysfunction of the visual association cortex. Potential accounts of the results, based on the “hemisphere rivalry” model of extinction and the limited attentional capacity model, are considered.
- Published
- 2006
26. Utility of behavioral versus cognitive measures in differentiating between subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
Melissa Newhart, Lynda Ken, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Shannon Chang, Argye E. Hillis, and Rebecca F. Gottesman
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Trail Making Test ,Semantic dementia ,Behavioral Symptoms ,Audiology ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Verbal learning ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Diagnosis, Differential ,Progressive nonfluent aphasia ,Alzheimer Disease ,medicine ,Dementia ,Humans ,Psychiatry ,Aged ,Aged, 80 and over ,Analysis of Variance ,Language Tests ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Discriminant Analysis ,Neuropsychological test ,Frontotemporal lobar degeneration ,Verbal Learning ,medicine.disease ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Female ,Geriatrics and Gerontology ,Psychology ,Cognition Disorders ,Psychomotor Performance ,Frontotemporal dementia - Abstract
We hypothesized that a modified version of the Frontal Behavioral Inventory (FBI-mod), along with a few cognitive tests, would be clinically useful in distinguishing between clinically defined Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD): frontotemporal dementia (dysexecutive type), progressive nonfluent aphasia, and semantic dementia. We studied 80 patients who were diagnosed with AD (n = 30) or FTLD (n = 50), on the basis of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, imaging, neurological examination, and history. We found significant between-group differences on the FBI-mod, two subtests of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (verbal learning and delayed recall), and the Trail Making Test Part B (one measure of ‘executive functioning’). AD was characterized by relatively severe impairment in verbal learning, delayed recall, and executive functioning, with relatively normal scores on the FBI-mod. Frontotemporal dementia was characterized by relatively severe impairment on the FBI-mod and executive functioning in the absence of severe impairment in verbal learning and recall. Progressive nonfluent aphasia was characterized by severe impairment in executive functioning with relatively normal scores on verbal learning and recall and FBI-mod. Finally, semantic dementia was characterized by relatively severe deficits in delayed recall, but relatively normal performance on new learning, executive functioning, and on FBI-mod. Discriminant function analysis confirmed that the FBI-mod, in conjunction with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and the Trail Making Test Part B categorized the majority of patients as subtypes of FTLD or AD in the same way as a full neuropsychological battery, neurological examination, complete history, and imaging. These tests may be useful for efficient clinical diagnosis, although progressive nonfluent aphasia and semantic dementia are likely to be best distinguished by language tests not included in standard neuropsychological test batteries.
- Published
- 2006
27. Where (in the brain) do semantic errors come from?
- Author
-
Argye E. Hillis, Priyanka Chaudhry, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Cameron Davis, and Melissa Newhart
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Speech and Hearing ,Linguistics and Language ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,Language and Linguistics - Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Speech and language functions that depend on Broca’s area
- Author
-
Jonathan T. Kleinman, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Argye E. Hillis, Melissa Newhart, and Cameron Davis
- Subjects
Speech and Hearing ,Linguistics and Language ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Broca's area ,Psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Linguistics - Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Spatial Neglect during Electrocortical Stimulation Mapping in the Right Hemisphere
- Author
-
Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Leila Gingis, Jehuda P. Sepkuty, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Nathan E. Crone, Argye E. Hillis, and Frederick A. Lenz
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Audiology ,Brain mapping ,Functional Laterality ,Temporal lobe ,Neglect ,Perceptual Disorders ,Epilepsy ,Postoperative Complications ,Parietal Lobe ,Preoperative Care ,medicine ,Humans ,Epilepsy surgery ,media_common ,Cerebral Cortex ,Brain Mapping ,Parietal lobe ,Hemispatial neglect ,medicine.disease ,Electric Stimulation ,Temporal Lobe ,Electrodes, Implanted ,Surgery ,Neurology ,Extinction (neurology) ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
Spatial processing was assessed following implantation of subdural electrodes in the nondominant hemisphere with electrocortical stimulation mapping (ESM) in two patients before epilepsy surgery. The first patient had mild hemispatial neglect/extinction during ESM of posterior temporal and inferior parietal areas. These areas were resected, and the patient had postoperative deficits that were similar to those occurring with ESM. The second patient was found to have marked hemispatial neglect during stimulation of parietal areas. These areas were not resected, and the patient had no neglect following surgery. These results suggest that ESM can help predict spatial processing deficits associated with cortical resection, and may help prevent postoperative impairments following resection in right parietal or temporal regions.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Neural regions essential for distinct cognitive processes underlying picture naming.
- Author
-
Jessica DeLeon, Rebecca F. Gottesman, Jonathan T. Kleinman, Melissa Newhart, Cameron Davis, Jennifer Heidler-Gary, Andrew Lee, and Argye E. Hillis
- Subjects
ISCHEMIA ,ALLOCATION of organs, tissues, etc. ,SEMANTICS ,MULTIVARIATE analysis - Abstract
We hypothesized that distinct cognitive processes underlying oral and written picture naming depend on intact function of different, but overlapping, regions of the left hemisphere cortex, such that the distribution of tissue dysfunction in various areas can predict the component of the naming process that is disrupted. To test this hypothesis, we evaluated 116 individuals within 24?h of acute ischaemic stroke using a battery of oral and written naming and other lexical tests, and with magnetic resonance diffusion and perfusion imaging to identify the areas of tissue dysfunction. Discriminant function analysis, using the degree of hypoperfusion in various Brodmanns areas—BA 22 (including Wernickes area), BA 44 (part of Brocas area), BA 45 (part of Brocas area), BA 21 (inferior temporal cortex), BA 37 (posterior, inferior temporal/fusiform gyrus), BA 38 (anterior temporal cortex) and BA 39 (angular gyrus)—as discriminant variables, classified patients on the basis of the primary component of the naming process that was impaired (defined as visual, semantics, modality-independent lexical access, phonological word form, orthographic word form and motor speech by the pattern of performance and types of errors across lexical tasks). Additionally, linear regression analysis demonstrated that the areas contributing the most information to the identification of patients with particular levels of impairment in the naming process were largely consistent with evidence for the roles of these regions from functional imaging. This study provides evidence that the level of impairment in the naming process reflects the distribution of tissue dysfunction in particular regions of the left anterior, inferior and posterior middle/superior temporal cortex, posterior inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex. While occipital cortex is also critical for picture naming, it is likely that bilateral occipital damage is necessary to disrupt visual recognition. These findings provide new evidence that a network of brain regions supports naming, but separate components of this network are differentially required for distinct cognitive processes or representations underlying the complex task of naming pictures. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.