Description: When we are under conditions of potential threat, cognitive processes important for its detection and avoidance, such as attention and perception, improve (Ohman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001). For example, previous research has shown that visual spatial attention is enhanced when monetary loss (punishment) depends on task performance (Engelmann & Pessoa, 2009; 2014). In their study, perceptual sensitivity (measured with signal detection, d’) was increased in a spatial cueing task due to monetary incentive. Specifically, a peripheral cue captured attention after which a target stimulus was presented at the cued location (orienting) or the opposite location (re-orienting of attention). Both orienting and re-orienting, with a stronger effect for re-orienting of spatial attention, was improved when losing (or winning) the money was dependent on the participants’ performance (relative to no incentives). This study supports the view that improvement in attention is an evolutionarily mechanism that helps us to avoid aversive cues (Ohman et al., 2001; Öhman & Wiens, 2002). However, in many situations, our behavior may benefit not only ourselves, but also others. Previous studies have demonstrated, that people are in fact more sensitive and willing to sacrifice more when it comes to the avoidance of threat to the others compared to oneself. For example, participants were willing to pay more money to grant safety (no shocks) for the other participant (stranger) than for themselves (Crockett, Kurth-Nelson, Siegel, Dayan, & Dolan, 2015). This is especially the case when threats are immediate (Vieira, Schellhaas, Enstrom, & Olsson, 2020). In addition to this explicit bias in decision making, people show an implicit tendency to perform better when the safety of others depend on it. For example, in an implicit learning task, participants demonstrated a better discrimination of aversive and safe cues when the safety of others depended on it (Lengersdorff, Wagner, Lockwood, & Lamm, 2020). These results suggest that people are willing to help others avoid threat, even at their own cost, and that this process is associated with enhanced cognitive performance. In the current study, we will investigate whether attention improves when the safety of others depends on it. We will use an exogenous spatial cueing task that measures automatic shifts of attention independent of top-down control (Posner & Cohen, 1984) . References: Crockett, M. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Siegel, J. Z., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 112(4), E381. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25572965. doi:10.1073/pnas.1424572112 Lengersdorff, L. L., Wagner, I. C., Lockwood, P. L., & Lamm, C. (2020). When Implicit Prosociality Trumps Selfishness: The Neural Valuation System Underpins More Optimal Choices When Learning to Avoid Harm to Others Than to Oneself. J Neurosci, 40(38), 7286-7299. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32839234. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0842-20.2020 Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J Exp Psychol Gen, 130(3), 466-478. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11561921. doi:10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.466 Öhman, A., & Wiens, S. (2002). On the automaticity of autonomic responses in emotion: an evolutionary perspective. In: Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K., Hill, H.H. (Eds.). Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford University Press, New York., 256–275. Vieira, J. B., Schellhaas, S., Enstrom, E., & Olsson, A. (2020). Help or flight? Increased threat imminence promotes defensive helping in humans. Proc Biol Sci, 287(1933), 20201473. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32842931. doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.1473