214 results on '"Monsell, Stephen"'
Search Results
2. Role of verbal working memory in rapid procedural acquisition of a choice response task
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen and Graham, Brontë
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Is Preparing for a Language Switch Like Preparing for a Task Switch?
- Author
-
Lavric, Aureliu, Clapp, Amanda, East, Antonia, Elchlepp, Heike, and Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
A key index of top-down control in task switching--preparation for a switch--is underexplored in language switching. The well-documented EEG "signature" of preparation for a task switch--a protracted positive-polarity modulation over the posterior scalp--has thus far not been reported in language switching, and the interpretation of previously reported effects of preparation on language switching performance is complicated by confounding factors. In an experiment using event-related potentials (ERPs) and an optimized picture-naming paradigm that addressed these confounds the language was specified by an auditory cue on every trial and changed unpredictably. There were two key manipulations. First, the cue-stimulus interval allowed either generous (1,500 ms) or little (100 ms) opportunity for preparation. Second, to explore the interplay between bottom-up and top-down language selection, we compared a highly transparent and familiar "supercue"--the name of the language spoken in that language to a relatively opaque cue (short speeded-up fragment of national anthem). Preparation for a switch elicited a brain potential strongly reminiscent of the posterior switch positivity documented in task switching. As previously shown in task switching, its amplitude inversely predicted the performance "switch cost," demonstrated by our ERP analyses contingent on reaction time (RT). This overlap in the electrophysiological correlates of preparing to switch tasks and languages suggests domain-general processes for top-down selection of task-set and language for production. But, the surprisingly small language switch cost following the supercue in the short CSI suggests that rapid and (possibly automatic) bottom-up selection--not typically observed in task switching--may also occur.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Control of mental processes
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Self-Paced Preparation for a Task Switch Eliminates Attentional Inertia but Not the Performance Switch Cost
- Author
-
Longman, Cai S., Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
The performance overhead associated with changing tasks (the "switch cost") usually diminishes when the task is specified in advance but is rarely eliminated by preparation. A popular account of the "residual" (asymptotic) switch cost is that it reflects "task-set inertia": carry-over of task-set parameters from the preceding trial(s). New evidence for a component of "task-set inertia" comes from eye-tracking, where the location associated with the previously (but no longer) relevant task is fixated preferentially over other irrelevant locations, even when preparation intervals are generous. Might such limits in overcoming task-set inertia in general, and "attentional inertia" in particular, result from suboptimal scheduling of preparation when the time available is outside one's control? In the present study, the stimulus comprised 3 digits located at the points of an invisible triangle, preceded by a central verbal cue specifying which of 3 classification tasks to perform, each consistently applied to just 1 digit location. The digits were presented only when fixation moved away from the cue, thus giving the participant control over preparation time. In contrast to our previous research with experimenter-determined preparation intervals, we found no sign of attentional inertia for the long preparation intervals. Self-paced preparation reduced but did not eliminate the performance switch cost--leaving a clear residual component in both reaction time and error rates. That the scheduling of preparation accounts for some, but not all, components of the residual switch cost, challenges existing accounts of the switch cost, even those which distinguish between preparatory and poststimulus reconfiguration processes.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Task switching without knowledge of the tasks.
- Author
-
Forrest, Charlotte, Elchlepp, Heike, Monsell, Stephen, and McLaren, Ian P.L.
- Published
- 2012
7. Shifting Attention Between Visual Dimensions as a Source of Switch Costs
- Author
-
Elchlepp, Heike, Best, Maisy, Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2017
8. Is Performance in Task-Cuing Experiments Mediated by Task Set Selection or Associative Compound Retrieval?
- Author
-
Forrest, Charlotte L. D., Monsell, Stephen, and McLaren, Ian P. L.
- Abstract
Task-cuing experiments are usually intended to explore control of task set. But when small stimulus sets are used, they plausibly afford learning of the response associated with a combination of cue and stimulus, without reference to tasks. In 3 experiments we presented the typical trials of a task-cuing experiment: a cue (colored shape) followed, after a short or long interval, by a digit to which 1 of 2 responses was required. In a tasks condition, participants were (as usual) directed to interpret the cue as an instruction to perform either an odd/even or a high/low classification task. In a cue + stimulus ? response (CSR) condition, to induce learning of mappings between cue-stimulus compound and response, participants were, in Experiment 1, given standard task instructions and additionally encouraged to learn the CSR mappings; in Experiment 2, informed of all the CSR mappings and asked to learn them, without standard task instructions; in Experiment 3, required to learn the mappings by trial and error. The effects of a task switch, response congruence, preparation, and transfer to a new set of stimuli differed substantially between the conditions in ways indicative of classification according to task rules in the tasks condition, and retrieval of responses specific to stimulus-cue combinations in the CSR conditions. Qualitative features of the latter could be captured by an associative learning network. Hence associatively based compound retrieval can serve as the basis for performance with a small stimulus set. But when organization by tasks is apparent, control via task set selection is the natural and efficient strategy.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Are Stimulus-Response Rules Represented Phonologically for Task-Set Preparation and Maintenance?
- Author
-
van 't Wout, Félice, Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
Accounts of task-set control generally assume that the current task's stimulus-response (S-R) rules must be elevated to a privileged state of activation. How are they represented in this state? In 3 task-cuing experiments, we tested the hypothesis that phonological working memory is used to represent S-R rules for task-set control by getting participants to switch between 2 sets of arbitrary S-R rules and manipulating the articulatory duration (Experiment 1) or phonological similarity (Experiments 2 and 3) of the names of the stimulus terms. The task cue specified which of 2 objects (Experiment 1) or consonants (Experiment 2) in a display to identify with a key press. In Experiment 3, participants switched between identifying an object/consonant and its color/visual texture. After practice, neither the duration nor the similarity of the stimulus terms had detectable effects on overall performance, task-switch cost, or its reduction with preparation. Only in the initial single-task training blocks was phonological similarity a significant handicap. Hence, beyond a very transient role, there is no evidence that (declarative) phonological working memory makes a functional contribution to representing S-R rules for task-set control, arguably because once learned, they are represented in nonlinguistic procedural working memory.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. More Attention to Attention? An Eye-Tracking Investigation of Selection of Perceptual Attributes during a Task Switch
- Author
-
Longman, Cai S., Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
Switching tasks prolongs response times, an effect reduced but not eliminated by active preparation. To explore the role of attentional selection of the relevant stimulus attribute in these task-switch costs, we measured eye fixations in participants cued to identify either a face or a letter displayed on its forehead. With only 200 ms between cue and stimulus onsets, the eyes fixated the currently relevant region of the stimulus less and the irrelevant region more on switch than on repeat trials, at stimulus onset and for 500 ms thereafter, in a pattern suggestive of delayed orientation of attention to the relevant region on switch trials. With 800 ms to prepare, both switch costs and inappropriate fixations were reduced, but on switch trials participants still tended (relative to repeat trials) to fixate the now-irrelevant region more at stimulus onset and to maintain fixation on, or refixate, the irrelevant region more during the next 500 ms. The size of this attentional persistence was associated with differences in performance costs between and within participants. We suggest that reorientation of attention is an important, albeit somewhat neglected and controversial, component of advance task-set reconfiguration and that the task-set inertia (or reactivation) to which many attribute the residual task-switch cost seen after preparation includes inertia in (or reactivation of) attentional parameters.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Task Set Regulation
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, primary
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. ERP and Eye-tracking
- Author
-
Longman, Cai, Elchlepp, Heike, Monsell, Stephen, and Lavric, Aureliu
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. TMS to V1 spares discrimination of emotive relative to neutral body postures
- Author
-
Filmer, Hannah L. and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Can the Task-Cuing Paradigm Measure an Endogenous Task-Set Reconfiguration Process?
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen and Mizon, Guy A.
- Abstract
In 6 task-cuing experiments, with 2 cues per task, the authors varied cue-stimulus interval to investigate G. D. Logan and C. Bundesen's (2003) claim that when cue repetition is controlled for, task-switch cost and its reduction with preparation are largely eliminated and hence cannot index an endogenous control process. Experiment 1 replicates their result, but Experiments 2 and 3, with similar designs, demonstrate a substantial task-switch cost, reducing with increasing cue-stimulus interval. Experiments 4 to 6 show that the critical difference is the probability of a task change: If it is kept low enough to discourage reconfiguration of task set unless and until the cue signals a task change, robust evidence for anticipatory task-set reconfiguration is obtained, even in Experiment 6, modeled closely on Logan and Bundesen's.
- Published
- 2006
15. The Cost of Switching between Kanji and Kana While Reading Japanese.
- Author
-
Shafiullah, Mohamm and Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
In four experiments, Japanese readers were required to switch between reading words in Kanji and Kana. In every case, performance was significantly slower and less accurate on the trials following a change of script. (Author/VWL)
- Published
- 1999
16. A Change of Task Prolongs Early Processes: Evidence From ERPs in Lexical Tasks
- Author
-
Elchlepp, Heike, Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Is It Harder to Switch Among a Larger Set of Tasks?
- Author
-
van ’t Wout, Félice, Lavric, Aureliu, and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Attentional Inertia and Delayed Orienting of Spatial Attention in Task-Switching
- Author
-
Longman, Cai S., Lavric, Aureliu, Munteanu, Cristian, and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. How Task Set and Task Switching Modulate Perceptual Processes: Is Recognition of Facial Emotion an Exception?
- Author
-
Elchlepp, Heike, primary, Monsell, Stephen, additional, and Lavric, Aureliu, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, Sumner, Petroc, and Waters, Helen
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis
- Author
-
Nieuwenhuis, Sander and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. The effects of recent practice on task switching
- Author
-
Yeung, Nick and Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
Context switching -- Research ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
Four experiments investigated the effect of recent selective practice on the cost of switching between 2 tasks afforded by letter-digit pairs: alphabet arithmetic and shape comparison. Experiments 1 and 2 found a greater cost associated with switching to the more recently practiced task: evidence that task-set inertia contributes to switching costs. Experiment 3 found this effect to be limited to trials on which a recently trained stimulus followed another such stimulus: a result problematic for all current theories of task-set priming. Experiment 4 showed that the effect of recent practice was eliminated by active preparation for a task switch: It appears that endogenous task-set preparation reduces the effects of task-set inertia.
- Published
- 2003
23. Task-set switching deficits in early-stage Huntington's disease: implications for basal ganglia function
- Author
-
Aron, Adam R., Watkins, Laura, Sahakian, Barbara J., Monsell, Stephen, Barker, Roger A., and Robbins, Trevor W.
- Subjects
Huntington's chorea -- Research ,Health ,Psychology and mental health - Published
- 2003
24. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection
- Author
-
Yeung, Nick and Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
Context switching -- Psychological aspects ,Context switching -- Research ,Psychology, Experimental -- Research ,Motor ability -- Psychological aspects ,Motor ability -- Research ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
It has been reported that it is harder to switch to a strong, well-practiced task from a weaker, less-practiced task than vice versa. Three experiments replicated this surprising asymmetry and investigated how it is affected by a reduction in interference between tasks. Experiment 1 progressively delayed the onset of the stimulus attribute associated with the stronger task. Experiments 2 and 3 separated the response sets of the tasks. Both manipulations reduced, without eliminating, interference of the stronger with the weaker task but reversed the asymmetry of switch costs, resulting in a larger cost of switching to the weaker task. The results are interpreted in terms of a model of the interactions between control input, task strength, and task priming.
- Published
- 2003
25. Naming the color of a word: Is it responses or task sets that compete?
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, Taylor, Tim J., and Murphy, Karen
- Published
- 2001
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. A brain-potential study of preparation for and execution of a task-switch with stimuli that afford only the relevant task
- Author
-
Elchlepp, Heike, Lavric, Aureliu, Mizon, Guy A., and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. PEP Does Not Dispense with but Implements Task-Set Reconfiguration. Can It Handle Phenomena More Diagnostic of Endogenous Control?
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, primary and McLaren, Ian, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. The chronometrics of task-set control
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, primary
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Inhibitory semantic priming: does syntactic class play a role in determining competitor status?
- Author
-
Tree, Jeremy J., Hirsh, Katherine W., and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Information processing in short-term memory tasks
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
150.724 ,Human information processing ,Short-term memory - Abstract
This thesis is about the nature of a man's immediate memory for a short sequence of verbal items. Two components of immediate memory may be distinguished: an 'active' memory component, which is progressively destroyed within a few seconds if, subsequent to presentation of the sequence, the man is required to perform a distracting task which prevents rehearsal of the items, and a residual 'inactive' component which is more permanent. My concern is with behavioural evidence relating to the question of how the attributes of a verbal sequence are represented in active memory subsequent to identification of the constituent items. It is possible selectively to probe the retrieval of different attributes of the sequence by the use of appropriate tasks. Evidence on the performance of normal adult subjects in various short-term memory tasks, from the literature and from my own experiments, is extensively reviewed. There emerges a fundamental contrast between two groups of tasks: on the one hand, tasks which require the subject to indicate whether a test item was present in the memorised sequence (item recognition) or to judge the relative recency with which items have occurred; on the other,tasks which require the subject to base his response on the order or precise position of items in the sequence. To account for the nature of the contrast I put forward a general hypothesis which is an amended and elaborated version of parts of a general model of memory described by Morton (1970). It is argued that subsequent to identification of an item, representations of it may temporarily be held in active memory by two distinct storage mechanisms. Thefirst is as a decaying trace at the neural unit, called (following Morton) a 'logogen', responsible for the identification of that item. This trace may be treated as possessing a unidimensional 'strength' whose magnitude is dependent on how recently the item has occurred. Item recognition and judgements of recency are held to be mediated by assessment of the test item's trace strength with respect to a decision criterion located on the strength continuum. Secondly, the item may also be represented as one of several maintained in a serially- organised limited-capacity storage mechanism called (following Morton) the 'response buffer' . This holds a small number of the verbal responses recently made available by the logogens, coded as a string of articulatory descriptions or commands. It has two crucial properties. Firstly, items represented in it as potential responses may be fed back in sequence to the logogens for re-identification (the process of sub-vocal rehearsal). Secondly, since the serial order in which items enter the response buffer is retained as an intrinsic property of its structure, retrieval from it provides a ready mode of access to the order or position of items in the memorised sequence, which item-traces at logogens do not. The thesis falls into two parts. The first three chapters contain a theoretical review of the literature. In Ch.1, the active/ inactive distinction is introduced with some reference to the historical background (1.1). Some points of technique are raised (1.2), the aims, of the present work are outlined (1.3) and the twostore hypothesis is described (1.4) In Ch.2, evidence is reviewed from short-term memory experiments in which accuracy is the main dependent variable, beginning with experiments in which either serial recall of the whole sequence or probed recall of a single item is required. The hypothesis that the order of items is retained in active memory as an intrinsic property of the memory structure is contrasted with theories (e.g. Wickelgren, 1972) emphasising the formation of temporary associations between the representations of items adjacent in the list and/or between item and position representations. It is concluded that inter-item associations play no major role in active memory, and that item-position associations cannot account both for the partial independence of order and item errors and for the relationship between order errors and phonemic similarity. Conrad's (1965) model is introduced as a precursor of my own. It is concluded that items are recalled primarily from the (ordered) response buffer, but that traces at logogens influence the availability of responses as guesses when items are wholly or partly lost from the response buffer (2.11). Some evidence is then described which suggests that variables may be identified which differentially influence retention of information about the order of items and about their occurrence (2.12). Experiments comparing the effectiveness of position, context or both,as cues for recall are argued to imply that order and position are coded by the same mechanism, but that access to items retained by it may be more direct given a position rather than a contextual cue (2.13). We then turn to experiments on short-term recognition and judgements of recency, which are discussed in relation to the trace strength theory of Wickelgren and Norman (1966) (2.21). It is concluded that an item's recent identification is represented as an exponentially-decaying trace located at a unique permanent representation of that item, to which access is direct (2.22). But order-recognition experiments provide little evidence that associations between items are represented in active memory in the same way (2.23). The two-store hypothesis is then applied to some general problems of the functions of active memory: in speech comprehension (2.31) as an 'address register' for inactive memory (cf. Broadbent, 1971) (2.32) and as a working memory. Finally, the logogen system is discussed in relation to Sperling's (1967) 'recognition buffer' (2.34) and evidence for articulatory as opposed to acoustic coding in immediate memory is reviewed (2.35). In Ch.3, I introduce the experimental paradigms pioneered by Sternberg (1966), which involve presentation on each trial of a different sub-span list for memorisation, followed by a probe to which reaction-time (RT) is measured. Several models are outlined of the nature, dynamics and format of the representations mediating performance in Sternberg's item-recognition paradigm (IRn), in which the subject must indicate whether the probe was or was not a member of the list. Predictions are derived from Sternberg's two scanning models and two versions of a trace strength hypothesis and compared to data available in the literature. The evidence favours the hypothesis that subjects perform the IRn task by judging the strength of the trace at the amodal representation (logogen) to which the probe provides direct access (3.1). Evidence from a version of the IRn paradigm in which the memory set remains constant from trial to trial is then reviewed; it is argued that the results obtained are necessarily equivocal with respect to active memory; nevertheless, the concept of trace strength may be of use in this context also (3.2). Next, we see that Sternberg's own experiments on contextual recall (CR), in which the subject must respond with the item following the probed item in the list, yield a pattern of results very different from that obtained in IRn. His own explanation is in terms of different strategies of search within a single store. In view of the failure of the exhaustive scanning model of IRn, the differences may be better accounted for by the two-store model. Since these RT paradigms provide a relatively pure and sensitive way of probing the retrieval of different attributes from active memory, a strategy is suggested of looking for factors which differentially influence performance in IRn, CR and related tasks. Evidence already available suggests that these factors may include serial position, phonemic similarity and the availability of a position cue. The second part of the thesis describes my own experiments.
- Published
- 1973
31. Neurophysiological signature of effective anticipatory task-set control: a task-switching investigation
- Author
-
Lavric, Aureliu, Mizon, Guy A., and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2008
32. The disorder-salient Stroop effect as a measure of psychopathology in eating disorders
- Author
-
Jones-Chesters, Matthew H., Monsell, Stephen, and Cooper, Peter J.
- Subjects
Eating disorders -- Psychological aspects ,Anorexia nervosa -- Psychological aspects ,Bulimia -- Psychological aspects ,Food/cooking/nutrition ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
The relationship between the disorder-salient Stroop effect and the psychopathology of eating disorders was investigated. Thirty-two female subjects participated in the experiment that used four word types to determine the amount of Stroop interference in women with eating disorders. Results show that patients with anorexia nervosa displayed a slower response to color-name words relating to food and eating as well as weight and shape than patients with bulimia nervosa. Also, there was no reliable interference effect for a set of emotion words in patients with anorexia nervosa.
- Published
- 1998
33. A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex
- Author
-
Aron, Adam R., Monsell, Stephen, Sahakian, Barbara J., and Robbins, Trevor W.
- Published
- 2004
34. Costs of predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks
- Author
-
Rogers, Robert A. and Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
Cognitive psychology -- Research ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
In an investigation of task-set reconfiguration, participants switched between 2 tasks on every 2nd trial in 5 experiments and on every 4th trial in a final experiment. The tasks were to classify either the digit member of a pair of characters as even/odd or the letter member as consonant/vowel. As the response-stimulus interval increased up to 0.6 s, the substantial cost to performance of this predictable task-switch fell: Participants could partially reconfigure in advance of the stimulus. However, even with 1.2 s available for preparation, a large asymptotic reaction time (RT) cost remained, but only on the 1st trial of the new task. This is attributed to a component of reconfiguration triggered exogenously, i.e., only by a task-relevant stimulus. That stimuli evoke associated task-sets also explains why RT and switch costs increased when the stimulus included a character associated with the currently irrelevant task.
- Published
- 1995
35. Lexical and sublexical translation of spelling to sound: strategic anticipation of lexical status
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, Graham, Andrew, Hughes, Claire H., Patterson, Karalyn E., and Milroy, Robert
- Subjects
Lexical phonology -- Research ,Word recognition -- Research ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
Two experiments on oral reading of single words compared naming performance in pure blocks of nonwords or exception words with performance in blocks of randomly mixed nonwords and exception words. Ss named exception words faster and made fewer regularization errors when they were not also prepared for nonwords. These data suggest Ss inhibit or ignore the computation of assembled phonology when only exception words are expected. Ss named nonwords faster, but no more accurately, when low-frequency exception words were not also anticipated. Thus, Ss' readiness to execute assembled phonology appears to be adjusted in relation to the likely time course of retrieval of learned pronunciations, when the latter must be attended to. This evidence for strategic dissociation between sublexical and lexical translation is discussed in relation to current models.
- Published
- 1992
36. Task switching
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Frequency effects in lexical tasks: reply to Balota and Chumbley
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
Lexicology -- Psychological aspects ,Categorization (Psychology) -- Research ,Psychology and mental health - Abstract
In replying to Balota and Chumbley's (1990) commentary on Monsell, Doyle, and Haggard's (1989) article, the author addresses four issues. New data show that the effect of frequency on semantic categorization time reported by Monsell et al. was not a disguised typicality effect. An account of the small size of the effect of stress pattern on immediate naming latency observed by Monsell et al. is supplied. Inferences that may and may not be drawn from effects of frequency on delayed naming latency are discussed. The main conclusions and methodological recommendations of Monsell et al. are clarified.
- Published
- 1990
38. Can we prepare to attend to one of two simultaneous voices?
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen, primary, Lavric, Aureliu, additional, Strivens, Amy, additional, and Paul, Emilia, additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Competitor Priming in Spoken Word Recognition
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen and Hirsh, Katherine W.
- Published
- 1998
40. Costs of a Predictable Switch Between Simple Cognitive Tasks
- Author
-
Rogers, Robert D. and Monsell, Stephen
- Published
- 1995
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Recency, Immediate Recognition Memory, and Reaction Time.
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
Four possible mechanisms for short-term item recognition are distinguished. Manipulations of recency, particularly of negative probe items, provide critical tests. Two experiments were conducted using Sternberg's varied-set reaction time paradigm, coupled with procedures intended to minimize rehearsal and control the recency of probes and memory set items. (Author/RD)
- Published
- 1978
42. Representations, Processes, Memory Mechanisms: The Basic Components of Cognition.
- Author
-
Monsell, Stephen
- Abstract
Provides an introduction to research on the basic components of cognitive skill. Perceptual processing and representation, the components of long- and short-term memory, and the nature of attention, mental performance, and consciousness are discussed. A 101-item reference list is provided. (JL)
- Published
- 1981
43. Mackintosh lecture—: Association and cognition: Two processes, one system
- Author
-
McLaren, IPL, primary, McAndrew, Amy, additional, Angerer, Katharina, additional, McLaren, Rossy, additional, Forrest, Charlotte, additional, Bowditch, Will, additional, Monsell, Stephen, additional, and Verbruggen, Frederick, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Information processing in short-term memory tasks
- Author
-
Monsell, S and Monsell, Stephen
- Subjects
Human information processing ,Short-term memory - Abstract
This thesis is about the nature of a man's immediate memory for a short sequence of verbal items. Two components of immediate memory may be distinguished: an 'active' memory component, which is progressively destroyed within a few seconds if, subsequent to presentation of the sequence, the man is required to perform a distracting task which prevents rehearsal of the items, and a residual 'inactive' component which is more permanent. My concern is with behavioural evidence relating to the question of how the attributes of a verbal sequence are represented in active memory subsequent to identification of the constituent items. It is possible selectively to probe the retrieval of different attributes of the sequence by the use of appropriate tasks. Evidence on the performance of normal adult subjects in various short-term memory tasks, from the literature and from my own experiments, is extensively reviewed. There emerges a fundamental contrast between two groups of tasks: on the one hand, tasks which require the subject to indicate whether a test item was present in the memorised sequence (item recognition) or to judge the relative recency with which items have occurred; on the other,tasks which require the subject to base his response on the order or precise position of items in the sequence. To account for the nature of the contrast I put forward a general hypothesis which is an amended and elaborated version of parts of a general model of memory described by Morton (1970). It is argued that subsequent to identification of an item, representations of it may temporarily be held in active memory by two distinct storage mechanisms. Thefirst is as a decaying trace at the neural unit, called (following Morton) a 'logogen', responsible for the identification of that item. This trace may be treated as possessing a unidimensional 'strength' whose magnitude is dependent on how recently the item has occurred. Item recognition and judgements of recency are held to be mediated by assessment of the test item's trace strength with respect to a decision criterion located on the strength continuum. Secondly, the item may also be represented as one of several maintained in a serially- organised limited-capacity storage mechanism called (following Morton) the 'response buffer' . This holds a small number of the verbal responses recently made available by the logogens, coded as a string of articulatory descriptions or commands. It has two crucial properties. Firstly, items represented in it as potential responses may be fed back in sequence to the logogens for re-identification (the process of sub-vocal rehearsal). Secondly, since the serial order in which items enter the response buffer is retained as an intrinsic property of its structure, retrieval from it provides a ready mode of access to the order or position of items in the memorised sequence, which item-traces at logogens do not. The thesis falls into two parts. The first three chapters contain a theoretical review of the literature. In Ch.1, the active/ inactive distinction is introduced with some reference to the historical background (1.1). Some points of technique are raised (1.2), the aims, of the present work are outlined (1.3) and the twostore hypothesis is described (1.4) In Ch.2, evidence is reviewed from short-term memory experiments in which accuracy is the main dependent variable, beginning with experiments in which either serial recall of the whole sequence or probed recall of a single item is required. The hypothesis that the order of items is retained in active memory as an intrinsic property of the memory structure is contrasted with theories (e.g. Wickelgren, 1972) emphasising the formation of temporary associations between the representations of items adjacent in the list and/or between item and position representations. It is concluded that inter-item associations play no major role in active memory, and that item-position associations cannot account both for the partial independence of order and item errors and for the relationship between order errors and phonemic similarity. Conrad's (1965) model is introduced as a precursor of my own. It is concluded that items are recalled primarily from the (ordered) response buffer, but that traces at logogens influence the availability of responses as guesses when items are wholly or partly lost from the response buffer (2.11). Some evidence is then described which suggests that variables may be identified which differentially influence retention of information about the order of items and about their occurrence (2.12). Experiments comparing the effectiveness of position, context or both,as cues for recall are argued to imply that order and position are coded by the same mechanism, but that access to items retained by it may be more direct given a position rather than a contextual cue (2.13). We then turn to experiments on short-term recognition and judgements of recency, which are discussed in relation to the trace strength theory of Wickelgren and Norman (1966) (2.21). It is concluded that an item's recent identification is represented as an exponentially-decaying trace located at a unique permanent representation of that item, to which access is direct (2.22). But order-recognition experiments provide little evidence that associations between items are represented in active memory in the same way (2.23). The two-store hypothesis is then applied to some general problems of the functions of active memory: in speech comprehension (2.31) as an 'address register' for inactive memory (cf. Broadbent, 1971) (2.32) and as a working memory. Finally, the logogen system is discussed in relation to Sperling's (1967) 'recognition buffer' (2.34) and evidence for articulatory as opposed to acoustic coding in immediate memory is reviewed (2.35). In Ch.3, I introduce the experimental paradigms pioneered by Sternberg (1966), which involve presentation on each trial of a different sub-span list for memorisation, followed by a probe to which reaction-time (RT) is measured. Several models are outlined of the nature, dynamics and format of the representations mediating performance in Sternberg's item-recognition paradigm (IRn), in which the subject must indicate whether the probe was or was not a member of the list. Predictions are derived from Sternberg's two scanning models and two versions of a trace strength hypothesis and compared to data available in the literature. The evidence favours the hypothesis that subjects perform the IRn task by judging the strength of the trace at the amodal representation (logogen) to which the probe provides direct access (3.1). Evidence from a version of the IRn paradigm in which the memory set remains constant from trial to trial is then reviewed; it is argued that the results obtained are necessarily equivocal with respect to active memory; nevertheless, the concept of trace strength may be of use in this context also (3.2). Next, we see that Sternberg's own experiments on contextual recall (CR), in which the subject must respond with the item following the probed item in the list, yield a pattern of results very different from that obtained in IRn. His own explanation is in terms of different strategies of search within a single store. In view of the failure of the exhaustive scanning model of IRn, the differences may be better accounted for by the two-store model. Since these RT paradigms provide a relatively pure and sensitive way of probing the retrieval of different attributes from active memory, a strategy is suggested of looking for factors which differentially influence performance in IRn, CR and related tasks. Evidence already available suggests that these factors may include serial position, phonemic similarity and the availability of a position cue. The second part of the thesis describes my own experiments. These employ Sternberg's varied-set IEn and CR paradigms and a task in which the subject must indicate the location of the probe in the list presented on that trial (Ln). In Ch.4, I raise some methodological issues inherent in the use of reaction time as a measure and give details of the apparatus and general procedure common to the experiments. In Ch.5 are described three experiments on the effects of phonemic similarity between items of the memory set on performance in the three tasks. Five-consonant lists were presented visually at a fast rate; the probe followed immediately and subjects were instructed not to rehearse; vocabulary and other factors were held constant across tasks. In Exp.I (CR) and Exp.III (Ln) phonemic similarity increased both RT and error frequency. The relationship between serial position and the effects of similarity in both tasks suggested a slow forward serial search through a temporally-ordered phonemically-coded store, save perhaps for terminal items of the sequence. The effects of similarity were in both tasks diminished when the probed item was phonemically distinct from the list items and, in CR, when the response item was similarly isolated. In Exp. II, no comparable effects upon IRn performance were found. Moreover, whereas marked increases with serial position probed were obtained in both CR and Ln, in the IRn task RT showed an accelerating decrease with serial position probed. The contrast between the two patterns of results is argued to be incompatible both with Sternberg's account in terms of two retrieval strategies and with Wickelgren's associative theory. The first experiment described in Ch.6 (Exp.IV) demonstrated that IRn was not facilitated by a cue presented along with the probe which informed the subject which half (in terms of ordinal position) of the four digit memory set was relevant to his decision. This result is compatible with the proposal that access to the active memory representations involved in IRn is direct, and does not require a search through an ordered store. Exp.V compared the effects upon IRn and CR of giving with the probe a cue telling the subject in which of four spatio-temporal positions the probed item had been presented (if, in IRn, it was a positive probe). Experimental parameters and subjects were the same for both tasks. Information provided by the position cue had a marked facilitatory effect on CR, none on IRn (as in Exp.IV). The difference is held to reflect the fact that location in the list of the probed item and retrieval of its successor, in CR, requires a serial search of the response buffer, a search which may however be abbreviated if the subject knows at what position to begin the search. In Ch.7 I give an account of a single experiment (Exp.Vl) intended to test predictions critically distinguishing the models of IRn outlined in Section 3.1. Memory sets of 1-4 consonants were visually presented at a fast rate; subjects were told not to rehearse, Two variables were manipulated: 1) the recency of occurrence, prior to the present trial, of items presented as negative probes; 2) the recency of items in the memory set. The latter was varied by presenting the probe either immediately or following a short delay during which rehearsal was prevented by a vowel-naming task. It was found that the more recently a negative probe had occurred, the slower and less accurately it was rejected; the effect increased with set size. The introduction of a filled delay between list and probe reduced the slopes of the serial position and set-size functions. The former showed monotonic recency effects. These and other aspects of tho data are incompatible with any scanning model and favour the trace strength inspection hypothesis. In Ch.8, two early exploratory experiments are described in which the task was again IRn. Mixed memory sets of digits and letters were presented; the probe was randomly either a letter or a digit. The question was whether the subject could use the semantic class of the probe to restrict a search to the appropriate subset of the list; such a strategy would not be possible according to a pure trace strength hypothesis. While Exp. VII provided no evidence for such a 'directed search' strategy, the data from Exp.VIII, in which the conditions were changed so as to maximise the possibility of such a strategy, showed an increase in RT with the size of the subset of the same class as the probe. However, it transpires that given the conditions of the experiment, this result does not rule out an explanation in terms of the trace strength inspection model. Changes are suggested which would make the experiment more decisive. The last chapter is devoted to concluding comments,speculations and plans. The necessity for exploring the adequacy of the two-store model in relation to other areas of research is acknowledged; these include speech production perception and comprehension, attention, non-verbal memory and the study of patients with memory disorders due to brain damage. Further experiments are suggested to compare the effects in the various short-term memory tasks of semantic similarity, rate of presentation, and interpolated material. In conclusion it is suggested that a greater emphasis on the representation of motor as well as perceptual events does much tc clarify the nature of active memory.
- Published
- 2016
45. Mackintosh lecture—: Association and cognition: Two processes, one system.
- Author
-
McLaren, I. P. L., McAndrew, Amy, Angerer, Katharina, McLaren, Rossy, Forrest, Charlotte, Bowditch, Will, Monsell, Stephen, and Verbruggen, Frederick
- Subjects
LIFE expectancy ,MACHINE learning ,COGNITION ,SOCIAL responsibility of business ,ASSOCIATIVE learning - Abstract
This article argues that the dual-process position can be a useful first approximation when studying human mental life, but it cannot be the whole truth. Instead, we argue that cognition is built on association, in that associative processes provide the fundamental building blocks that enable propositional thought. One consequence of this position is to suggest that humans are able to learn associatively in a similar fashion to a rat or a pigeon, but another is that we must typically suppress the expression of basic associative learning in favour of rule-based computation. This stance conceptualises us as capable of symbolic computation but acknowledges that, given certain circumstances, we will learn associatively and, more importantly, be seen to do so. We present three types of evidence that support this position: The first is data on human Pavlovian conditioning that directly support this view. The second is data taken from task-switching experiments that provide convergent evidence for at least two modes of processing, one of which is automatic and carried out "in the background." And the last suggests that when the output of propositional processes is uncertain, the influence of associative processes on behaviour can manifest. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. The coupling between spatial attention and other components of task-set: A task-switching investigation
- Author
-
Longman, Cai S., primary, Lavric, Aureliu, additional, and Monsell, Stephen, additional
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. List of Contributors
- Author
-
Adams, Jack A., primary, Baron, Sheldon, additional, Diewert, Gordon L., additional, Fowler, Carol A., additional, Frank, James S., additional, Kantowitz, Barry H., additional, Scott Kelso, J.A., additional, Kerr, Beth, additional, Knight, James L., additional, Knoll, Ronald L., additional, Marteniuk, Ronald G., additional, Monsell, Stephen, additional, Newell, K.M., additional, Pew, Richard W., additional, Schmidt, Richard A., additional, Smith, Judith L., additional, Wyrick Spirduso, Waneen, additional, Stelmach, George E., additional, Sternberg, Saul, additional, Turvey, M.T., additional, Wallace, Stephen A., additional, Wing, Alan M., additional, Wright, Charles E., additional, and Zelaznik, Howard N., additional
- Published
- 1978
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. The Latency and Duration of Rapid Movement Sequences: Comparisons of Speech and Typewriting
- Author
-
Sternberg, Saul, primary, Monsell, Stephen, additional, Knoll, Ronald L., additional, and Wright, Charles E., additional
- Published
- 1978
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Contributors
- Author
-
Adams, Jack A., primary, Baron, Sheldon, additional, Diewert, Gordon L., additional, Fowler, Carol A., additional, Frank, James S., additional, Kantowitz, Barry H., additional, Scott Kelso, J.A., additional, Kerr, Beth, additional, Knight, James L., additional, Knoll, Ronald L., additional, Marteniuk, Ronald G., additional, Monsell, Stephen, additional, Newell, K.M., additional, Pew, Richard W., additional, Schmidt, Richard A., additional, Smith, Judith L., additional, Wyrick Spirduso, Waneen, additional, Stelmach, George E., additional, Sternberg, Saul, additional, Turvey, M.T., additional, Wallace, Stephen A., additional, Wing, Alan M., additional, Wright, Charles E., additional, and Zelaznik, Howard N., additional
- Published
- 1978
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Advance re-orientation and attentional inertia in task-switching: An eye-tracking study
- Author
-
Longman, Cai S., primary, Lavric, Aureliu, additional, and Monsell, Stephen, additional
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.