156 results on '"Sayigh, Laela S."'
Search Results
2. Automatic Deep-Learning-Based Classification of Bottlenose Dolphin Signature Whistles
- Author
-
Jensen, Frants Havmand, Wolters, Piper, van Zeeland, Louisa, Morrison, Evan, Ermi, Gracie, Smith, Scott, Tyack, Peter L., Wells, Randall S., McKennoch, Sam, Janik, Vincent M., Sayigh, Laela S., Vigness-Raposa, Kathleen, Section editor, Popper, Arthur N., editor, Sisneros, Joseph A., editor, Hawkins, Anthony D., editor, and Thomsen, Frank, editor
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Temporal and spatial distributions of delphinid species in Massachusetts Bay (USA) using passive acoustics from ocean gliders
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., and Baumgartner, Mark F.
- Published
- 2019
4. Signature-Whistle Production in Undisturbed Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S. and Wells, Randall S.
- Published
- 2004
5. Bottlenose dolphin mothers modify signature whistles in the presence of their own calves
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., El Haddad, Nicole, Tyack, Peter L., Janik, Vincent M., Wells, Randall S., Jensen, Frants H., University of St Andrews. School of Biology, University of St Andrews. Institute of Behavioural and Neural Sciences, University of St Andrews. Sea Mammal Research Unit, University of St Andrews. Scottish Oceans Institute, University of St Andrews. Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolution, University of St Andrews. Bioacoustics group, and University of St Andrews. Marine Alliance for Science & Technology Scotland
- Subjects
MCC ,QL ,bottlenose dolphin ,motherese ,signature whistle ,vocal learning ,DAS ,QL Zoology ,animal communication - Abstract
Funding: PLT received support from ONR grants N00014-18-1-2062 and N00014-20-1-2709. Financial support for the whistle database project has come from the Protect Wild Dolphins fund at Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute, Vulcan Machine Learning Center for Impact, Allen Institute for Artificial Intelligence, Adelaide M. & Charles B. Link Foundation, and Dolphin Quest, Inc. Human caregivers interacting with children typically modify their speech in ways that promote attention, bonding, and language acquisition. Although this “motherese,” or child-directed communication (CDC), occurs in a variety of human cultures, evidence among nonhuman species is very rare. We looked for its occurrence in a nonhuman mammalian species with long-term mother–offspring bonds that is capable of vocal production learning, the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Dolphin signature whistles provide a unique opportunity to test for CDC in nonhuman animals, because we are able to quantify changes in the same vocalizations produced in the presence or absence of calves. We analyzed recordings made during brief catch-and-release events of wild bottlenose dolphins in waters near Sarasota Bay, Florida, United States, and found that females produced signature whistles with significantly higher maximum frequencies and wider frequency ranges when they were recorded with their own dependent calves vs. not with them. These differences align with the higher fundamental frequencies and wider pitch ranges seen in human CDC. Our results provide evidence in a nonhuman mammal for changes in the same vocalizations when produced in the presence vs. absence of offspring, and thus strongly support convergent evolution of motherese, or CDC, in bottlenose dolphins. CDC may function to enhance attention, bonding, and vocal learning in dolphin calves, as it does in human children. Our data add to the growing body of evidence that dolphins provide a powerful animal model for studying the evolution of vocal learning and language. Publisher PDF
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Individual Signatures in Animal Groups: Cetaceans
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., primary and Janik, Vincent M., additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Probable signature whistle production in Atlantic white-sided (Lagenorhynchus acutus) and short-beaked common (Delphinus delphis) dolphins near Cape Cod, Massachusetts
- Author
-
Cones, Seth, Dent, Molly, Walkes, Sam, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, DeWind, Christianna, Arjasbi, Kayla, Rose, Kathryn S., Silva, Tammy L., Sayigh, Laela S., Cones, Seth, Dent, Molly, Walkes, Sam, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, DeWind, Christianna, Arjasbi, Kayla, Rose, Kathryn S., Silva, Tammy L., and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2022. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Cones, S., Dent, M., Walkes, S., Bocconcelli, A., DeWind, C., Arjasbi, K., Rose, K., Silva, T., & Sayigh, L. Probable signature whistle production in Atlantic white-sided (Lagenorhynchus acutus) and short-beaked common (Delphinus delphis) dolphins near Cape Cod, Massachusetts. Marine Mammal Science, 39(1), (2022): 338-344, https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12976., Some delphinids produce a learned, individually specific tonal whistle that conveys identity information to conspecifics (Janik & Sayigh, 2013). These whistles, termed signature whistles, were first described by Caldwell and Caldwell (1965) and have been studied intensively over the past several decades (Janik & Sayigh, 2013). In common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and potentially other species, signature whistles facilitate many ecologically-important behaviors, including individual recognition and maintenance of group cohesion (Janik & Slater, 1998). Additionally, signature whistle contours, or patterns of frequency change over time, can remain stable for several decades, aiding in long-term social bonds (Sayigh et al., 1990). Signature whistles account for approximately 38%–70% of all whistle production in free-swimming animals (Buckstaff, 2004; Cook et al., 2004; Watwood et al., 2005); this percentage can be up to 100% for isolated individuals in captivity (Caldwell et al., 1990). Most of our knowledge on the function and use of signature whistles stems from Tursiops spp., and their use and presence in other delphinid taxa is less understood. Nonetheless, seven additional delphinid species have been reported to produce signature whistles: Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus; Gridley et al., 2014), common dolphins (D. delphis; Caldwell & Caldwell 1968; Fearey et al., 2019), Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella plagiodon; Caldwell et al., 1970), Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens; Caldwell & Caldwell, 1973), Pacific humpback dolphins (Sousa chinensis; Van Parijs & Corkeron, 2001), and Guiana dolphins (Sotalia guianensis; Duarte de Figueiredo & Simão, 2009)., Woods Hole Sea Grant, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, Grant/Award Number: NA14OAR4170074
- Published
- 2023
8. The Sarasota Dolphin whistle database: a unique long-term resource for understanding dolphin communication.
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., Janik, Vincent M., Jensen, Frants H., Scott, Michael D., Tyack, Peter L., Wells, Randall S., Sayigh, Laela S., Janik, Vincent M., Jensen, Frants H., Scott, Michael D., Tyack, Peter L., and Wells, Randall S.
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2022. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Sayigh, L., Janik, V., Jensen, F., Scott, M., Tyack, P., & Wells, R. The Sarasota Dolphin whistle database: a unique long-term resource for understanding dolphin communication. Frontiers in Marine Science, 9, (2022): 923046, https://doi.org/10.3389/fmars.2022.923046., Common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) produce individually distinctive signature whistles that are learned early in life and that help animals recognize and maintain contact with conspecifics. Signature whistles are the predominant whistle type produced when animals are isolated from conspecifics. Health assessments of dolphins in Sarasota, Florida (USA) provide a unique opportunity to record signature whistles, as dolphins are briefly separated from conspecifics. Recordings were first made in the mid 1970’s, and then nearly annually since 1984. The Sarasota Dolphin Whistle Database (SDWD) now contains 926 recording sessions of 293 individual dolphins, most of known age, sex, and matrilineal relatedness. The longest time span over which an individual has been recorded is 43 years, and 85 individuals have been recorded over a decade or more. Here we describe insights about signature whistle structure revealed by this unique and expansive dataset. Signature whistles of different dolphins show great variety in their fundamental frequency contours. Signature whistle types (with ‘whistle type’ defined as all whistles visually categorized as sharing a particular frequency modulation pattern) can consist of a single stereotyped element, or loop (single-loop whistles), or of multiple stereotyped loops with or without gaps (multi-loop whistles). Multi-loop signature whistle types can also show extensive variation in both number and contour of loops. In addition, fundamental frequency contours of all signature whistle types can be truncated (deletions) or embellished (additions), and other features are also occasionally incorporated. However, even with these variable features, signature whistle types tend to be highly stereotyped and easily distinguishable due to the extensive variability in contours among individuals. In an effort to quantify this individual distinctiveness, and to compare it to other species, we calculated Beecher’s Information Statistic and found, Funding for data collection and analysis over the years has been provided by the National Science Foundation, The Royal Society of London, Dolphin Quest, Adelaide M. and Charles B. Link Foundation, Marine Mammal Commission, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, Earthwatch Institute, Protect Wild Dolphins Fund of the Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute, Grossman Family Foundation, WHOI Ocean Life Institute, Vulcan Machine Learning Center for Impact, and the Allen Institute for Artificial Intelligence. Current support for PT’s involvement is provided by the Office of Naval Research Grants N00014-18-1-2062 and N00014-20-1-2709 through a subaward from Carnegie Mellon University. Current support for LS’s involvement is provided by the Adelaide M. & Charles B. Link Foundation and Dolphin Quest.
- Published
- 2023
9. Bottlenose dolphin mothers modify signature whistles in the presence of their own calves
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., primary, El Haddad, Nicole, additional, Tyack, Peter L., additional, Janik, Vincent M., additional, Wells, Randall S., additional, and Jensen, Frants H., additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. What’s in a voice? Dolphins do not use voice cues for individual recognition
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., Wells, Randall S., and Janik, Vincent M.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Cetacean Acoustic Communication
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S. and Witzany, Guenther, editor
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Signature Whistles
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., primary and Janik, Vincent M., additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Contributors
- Author
-
Acevedo-Gutiérrez, Alejandro, primary, Adam, Peter J., additional, Aguilar, Alex, additional, Amano, Masao, additional, Anderson, Paul K., additional, Archer, Frederick I., additional, Arnould, John P.Y., additional, Atkinson, Shannon, additional, Au, Whitlow W.L., additional, Aurioles-Gamboa, David, additional, Javier Aznar, F., additional, Baird, Robin W., additional, Baker, C. Scott, additional, Ballance, Lisa T., additional, Balbuena, Juan A., additional, Bannister, John L., additional, Barlow, Jay, additional, Barton, Sheri L., additional, Bearzi, Giovanni, additional, Beasley, Isabel, additional, Bekoff, Marc, additional, Ben-David, M., additional, Bengtson, John L., additional, Berta, Annalisa, additional, Bérubé, Martine, additional, Bester, Marthán N., additional, Bianucci, Giovanni, additional, Bjørge, Arne, additional, Black, Nancy A., additional, Bodkin, J.L., additional, Bonde, Robert K., additional, Borger, Jill, additional, Borrell, Asuncion, additional, Boveng, Peter, additional, Bowen, W.D., additional, Boyd, Ian L., additional, Braulik, Gillian T., additional, Brown, Alexander M., additional, Brownell, Robert L., additional, Buckland, Stephen T., additional, Burns, John J., additional, Cabrera, Andrea A., additional, Campagna, Claudio, additional, Cantor, Mauricio, additional, Cárdenas-Alayza, Susana, additional, Cárdenas-Hinojosa, Gustavo, additional, Castellini, Michael, additional, Cerchio, Salvatore, additional, Champagne, Cory D., additional, Chilvers, B. Louise, additional, Chivers, Susan J., additional, Cipriano, Frank, additional, Clapham, Phillip J., additional, Constantine, Rochelle, additional, Cooper, Lisa N., additional, Corkeron, Peter, additional, Costa, Daniel P., additional, Costidis, Alexander M., additional, Cowan, Daniel F., additional, Cranford, Ted, additional, Crespo, Enrique A., additional, Crocker, Daniel E., additional, Croll, Donald A., additional, da Silva, Vera M.F., additional, Danil, Kerri, additional, Darling, Jim, additional, Dawson, Stephen M., additional, de Muizon, Christian, additional, de Vos, Asha, additional, Dehnhardt, Guido, additional, DeMaster, Douglas P., additional, Deméré, Thomas A., additional, Dendrinos, Panagiotis, additional, Dill, Lawrence M., additional, Dizon, Andrew E., additional, L. Dolar, M. Louella, additional, Domning, Daryl P., additional, Donovan, G.P., additional, Dudzinski, Kathleen M., additional, Duffield, Deborah A., additional, Dyer, Michael P., additional, Ellis, Richard, additional, Eskelinen, Holli, additional, Estes, James A., additional, Evans, Peter G.H., additional, Fernández, Mercedes, additional, Fertl, Dagmar, additional, Fettuccia, Daniela de Castro, additional, Fiedler, Paul C., additional, Fish, Frank E., additional, Flores, Paulo A.C., additional, Forcada, Jaume, additional, Ford, John K.B., additional, Fordyce, R. Ewan, additional, Forestell, Paul H., additional, Forney, Karin A., additional, Fowler, Charles W., additional, Frankel, Adam S., additional, Friedlaender, Ari S., additional, Frohoff, Toni, additional, Frost, Kathryn J., additional, Galatius, Anders, additional, García-Vernet, Raquel, additional, Geisler, Jonathan H., additional, Gelatt, Thomas S., additional, Gentry, Roger, additional, George, J. Craig, additional, Gerrodette, Tim, additional, Goldbogen, Jeremy A., additional, Goldsworthy, Simon D., additional, P. Goodall, R. Natalie, additional, Goodman, Simon J., additional, Gregg, Justin D., additional, Hall, Ailsa J., additional, Hammill, Mike O., additional, Hammond, Philip S., additional, Hanke, Frederike D., additional, Hartman, Karin L., additional, Hazen, Elliott, additional, Heide-Jørgensen, M.P., additional, Heithaus, Michael R., additional, Herman, Louis M., additional, Herzing, Denise L., additional, Hewitt, Roger P., additional, Hindell, Mark A., additional, Hoelzel, A. Rus, additional, Hofmeyr, G. J. Greg, additional, Hohn, Aleta A., additional, Hooker, Sascha K., additional, Horstmann, Lara, additional, Horwood, Joseph, additional, Hoyt, Erich, additional, Hückstädt, Luis A., additional, Ivashchenko, Yulia V., additional, Iverson, Sara J., additional, Janik, Vincent M., additional, Jaramillo-Legorreta, Armando M., additional, Jefferson, Thomas A., additional, Jensen, Anne M., additional, Karamanlidis, Alexandros A., additional, Kasuya, Toshio, additional, Kato, Hidehiro, additional, Keith Diagne, Lucy W., additional, Kemp, Christopher, additional, Kemper, Catherine M., additional, Kenney, Robert D., additional, Kinze, Carl C., additional, Kirkman, Stephen P., additional, Kiszka, Jeremy J., additional, Koopman, Heather N., additional, Kooyman, Gerald L., additional, Kovacs, Kit M., additional, Kraus, Scott D., additional, Krysl, Petr, additional, Laidre, Kristin L., additional, Laitman, Jeffrey T., additional, Lambert, Olivier, additional, Landry, André M., additional, Lavigne, David M., additional, LeDuc, Rick, additional, Lipsky, Jessica D., additional, Littnan, Charles, additional, Loughlin, Thomas R., additional, Lowry, Lloyd, additional, Lowther, Andrew D., additional, Lydersen, Christian, additional, Maas, Mary C., additional, MacLean, Stephen A., additional, MacLeod, Colin D., additional, Mallette, Sarah D., additional, Mann, Janet, additional, Maresh, Jennifer L., additional, Marsh, Helene, additional, Marshall, Christopher D., additional, Martin, Anthony R., additional, Mass, Alla M., additional, McAlpine, Donald F., additional, Chris McKnight, J., additional, McLellan, William A., additional, Mead, James G., additional, Melin, Sharon R., additional, Merrick, Richard, additional, Mesnick, Sarah L., additional, Miller, Edward H., additional, Miller, Lance J., additional, Miller, Patrick J.O., additional, Miyazaki, Nobuyuki, additional, Moore, Jeffrey E., additional, Moore, Kathleen M., additional, Moore, Michael, additional, Moore, Sue E., additional, Moors-Murphy, Hilary B., additional, Morin, Phillip A., additional, Newman, William A., additional, Newton, Kelly M., additional, Nieto-García, Edwyna, additional, Northridge, Simon, additional, Nummela, Sirpa, additional, O'Brien, Justine K., additional, O'Corry-Crowe, Gregory M., additional, Olsen, Morten T., additional, Olson, Paula A., additional, Oppenheimer, Jonas, additional, Orbach, Dara N., additional, Ortiz, Rudy M., additional, Pabst, D. Ann, additional, Palsbøll, Per J., additional, Parra, Guido J., additional, Patterson, Eric, additional, Paves-Hernández, Héctor, additional, Perrin, William F., additional, Perryman, Wayne L., additional, Pitman, Robert, additional, Pomeroy, Patrick P., additional, Ponganis, Paul J., additional, Powell, James A., additional, Pyenson, Nicholas D., additional, Racicot, Rachel, additional, Raga, J. Antonio, additional, Ralls, Katherine, additional, Raverty, Stephen, additional, Read, Andrew J., additional, Reeves, Randall R., additional, Regehr, Eric V., additional, Reggente, Melissa A.L., additional, Reidenberg, Joy S., additional, Reijnders, Peter J.H., additional, Reyes, Julio C., additional, Reynolds, John E., additional, Robeck, Todd R., additional, Robinson, Kelly J., additional, Rode, Karyn, additional, Rogers, Tracey, additional, Rojas-Bracho, Lorenzo, additional, Roman, Joe, additional, Rommel, Sentiel A., additional, Roos, Marjoleine M.H., additional, Rosel, Patricia E., additional, Rowntree, Victoria J., additional, Rugh, David, additional, Russell, Debbie J.F., additional, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, Scolardi, Kerri M., additional, Scott, Michael D., additional, Sears, Richard, additional, Seger, Jon, additional, Sharp, Sarah, additional, Sheehan, Glenn W., additional, Silber, Gregory K., additional, Simeone, Claire A., additional, Smith, Brian D., additional, Southall, Brandon L., additional, Spitz, Jérôme, additional, Spoor, Fred, additional, Stacey, Rita, additional, Staniland, Iain J., additional, Steel, Debbie, additional, Stern, S. Jonathan, additional, Stewart, Brent S., additional, Supin, Alexander Y., additional, Suydam, R., additional, Swartz, Steven L., additional, Teilmann, Jonas, additional, Tershy, Bernie R., additional, Thewissen, J.G.M., additional, Tinker, M.T., additional, Tolley, Krystal A., additional, Trillmich, Fritz, additional, Trites, Andrew W., additional, Turner, Ted, additional, Twiss, Sean D., additional, Tyack, Peter L., additional, Uhen, Mark D., additional, Van Franeker, Jan A., additional, Van Waerebeek, Koen, additional, Wade, Paul R., additional, Wang, John Y., additional, Weller, David W., additional, Wells, Randall S., additional, Werth, Alexander J., additional, Whitehead, Hal, additional, Williams, Terrie M., additional, Würsig, Bernd, additional, Yablokov, Alexey V., additional, Yamada, Tadasu K., additional, Yamato, Maya, additional, Yochem, Pamela K., additional, York, Anne E., additional, and Zhou, Kaiya, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Vocal copying of individually distinctive signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins
- Author
-
King, Stephanie L., Sayigh, Laela S., Wells, Randall S., Fellner, Wendi, and Janik, Vincent M.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. The Sarasota Dolphin Whistle Database: A unique long-term resource for understanding dolphin communication
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., primary, Janik, Vincent M., additional, Jensen, Frants H., additional, Scott, Michael D., additional, Tyack, Peter L., additional, and Wells, Randall S., additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Whistles as Potential Indicators of Stress in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Esch, H. Carter, Sayigh, Laela S., Blum, James E., and Wells, Randall S.
- Published
- 2009
17. Seasonal trends and diel patterns of downsweep and SEP calls in Chilean blue whales
- Author
-
Redaelli, Laura, Mangia Woods, Sari, Landea Briones, Rafaela, Sayigh, Laela S., Redaelli, Laura, Mangia Woods, Sari, Landea Briones, Rafaela, and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2022. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Redaelli, L., Mangia Woods, S., Landea, R., & Sayigh, L. Seasonal trends and diel patterns of downsweep and SEP calls in Chilean blue whales. Journal of Marine Science and Engineering, 10(3), (2022): 316, https://doi.org/10.3390/jmse10030316., To learn more about the occurrence and behaviour of a recently discovered population of blue whales, passive acoustic data were collected between January 2012 and April 2013 in the Chiloense ecoregion of southern Chile. Automatic detectors and manual auditing were used to detect blue whale songs (SEP calls) and D calls, which were then analysed to gain insights into temporal calling patterns. We found that D call rates were extremely low during winter (June–August) but gradually increased in spring and summer, decreasing again later during fall. SEP calls were absent for most winter and spring months (July–November) but increased in summer and fall, peaking between March and April. Thus, our results support previous studies documenting the austral summer residency of blue whales in this region, while suggesting that some individuals stay longer, highlighting the importance of this area as a blue whale habitat. We also investigated the daily occurrence of each call type and found that D calls occurred more frequently during dusk and night hours compared to dawn and day periods, whereas SEP calls did not show any significant diel patterns. Overall, these findings help to understand the occurrence and behaviour of endangered Chilean blue whales, enhancing our ability to develop conservation strategies in this important Southern Hemisphere habitat., Financial support for expeditions, deployments, and retrievals of MARUs, and for some of the data analysis, was provided by Fundacion MERI, Av. Pdte. Kennedy 5682, Vitacure, Región Metropolitana, Chile. The data analysis for this study was carried out without external funding.
- Published
- 2022
18. Temporal trends and effects of noise on upsweep calls of Eastern South Pacific southern right whales
- Author
-
Jacobs, Ellen, Landea Briones, Rafaela, Sayigh, Laela S., Jacobs, Ellen, Landea Briones, Rafaela, and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
Eastern South Pacific southern right whales (ESPSRW) are a subpopulation of southern right whales (Eubalaena australis) off the coasts of Peru and Chile recognized by the International Union for the Conservation of Nature (IUCN) as critically endangered as a result of heavy whaling efforts in the late 18th to 20th centuries. Most recent population estimates put their numbers around 50 individuals. To test for the efficacy of passive acoustic monitoring of this population, we recorded five months of continuous acoustic data (January 2012-June 2012) off the southwestern tip of Isla de Chiloé. To test for trends in occurrence, we identified a total of 11,313 individual ESPSRW upsweep calls, which have been associated with maintaining contact with conspecifics. Calls increased over the course of the deployment and peaked between April and June, indicating an increase in use of the habitat consistent with the concurrent blue whale migration in the area. A clear diel pattern in which upsweep calls were predominately detected during dusk and night hours was identified, indicating that ESPSRW are likely foraging during daylight hours, as upsweep calls are known to be inversely related to foraging behavior. We also quantified noise levels in the frequency range of their communication (100 Hz third octave) to understand the change in active space whales may be experiencing. We measured noise levels from 90 dB re 1 µPa to 111 dB re 1 µPa (5th and 95th percentile), a 21 dB fluctuation that results in an order-of-magnitude decrease in active space area. We identified sources of high noise at or above the 75th percentile as predominately blue and humpback whale calls (occurring in 71.6% of total sampled minutes) and ship noise (occurring in 69.4% of total sampled minutes). Ship noise was responsible for outliers in excess of 140 dB re 1 µPa. In a population as diminished as ESPSRW, such disruptions of their communication range could result in significant barriers to maintaining c, Support from a private foundation in Chile, Fundacion MERI
- Published
- 2022
19. Sounding the call for a global library of underwater biological sounds
- Author
-
Parsons, Miles J. G., Lin, Tzu-Hao, Mooney, T. Aran, Erbe, Christine, Juanes, Francis, Lammers, Marc O., Li, Songhai, Linke, Simon, Looby, Audrey, Nedelec, Sophie L., Van Opzeeland, Ilse, Radford, Craig A., Rice, Aaron N., Sayigh, Laela S., Stanley, Jenni A., Urban, Edward, Di Iorio, Lucia, Parsons, Miles J. G., Lin, Tzu-Hao, Mooney, T. Aran, Erbe, Christine, Juanes, Francis, Lammers, Marc O., Li, Songhai, Linke, Simon, Looby, Audrey, Nedelec, Sophie L., Van Opzeeland, Ilse, Radford, Craig A., Rice, Aaron N., Sayigh, Laela S., Stanley, Jenni A., Urban, Edward, and Di Iorio, Lucia
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2022. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Parsons, M., Lin, T.-H., Mooney, T., Erbe, C., Juanes, F., Lammers, M., Li, S., Linke, S., Looby, A., Nedelec, S., Van Opzeeland, I., Radford, C., Rice, A., Sayigh, L., Stanley, J., Urban, E., & Di Iorio, L. Sounding the call for a global library of underwater biological sounds. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 10, (2022): 810156, https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.810156., Aquatic environments encompass the world’s most extensive habitats, rich with sounds produced by a diversity of animals. Passive acoustic monitoring (PAM) is an increasingly accessible remote sensing technology that uses hydrophones to listen to the underwater world and represents an unprecedented, non-invasive method to monitor underwater environments. This information can assist in the delineation of biologically important areas via detection of sound-producing species or characterization of ecosystem type and condition, inferred from the acoustic properties of the local soundscape. At a time when worldwide biodiversity is in significant decline and underwater soundscapes are being altered as a result of anthropogenic impacts, there is a need to document, quantify, and understand biotic sound sources–potentially before they disappear. A significant step toward these goals is the development of a web-based, open-access platform that provides: (1) a reference library of known and unknown biological sound sources (by integrating and expanding existing libraries around the world); (2) a data repository portal for annotated and unannotated audio recordings of single sources and of soundscapes; (3) a training platform for artificial intelligence algorithms for signal detection and classification; and (4) a citizen science-based application for public users. Although individually, these resources are often met on regional and taxa-specific scales, many are not sustained and, collectively, an enduring global database with an integrated platform has not been realized. We discuss the benefits such a program can provide, previous calls for global data-sharing and reference libraries, and the challenges that need to be overcome to bring together bio- and ecoacousticians, bioinformaticians, propagation experts, web engineers, and signal processing specialists (e.g., artificial intelligence) with the necessary support and funding to build a sustainable and scalable platform tha, Support for the initial author group to meet, discuss, and build consensus on the issues within this manuscript was provided by the Scientific Committee on Oceanic Research, Monmouth University Urban Coast Institute, and Rockefeller Program for the Human Environment. The U.S. National Science Foundation supported the publication of this article through Grant OCE-1840868 to the Scientific Committee on Oceanic Research.
- Published
- 2022
20. Seasonal trends and diel patterns of downsweep and SEP calls in Chilean blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus)
- Author
-
Redaelli, Laura, Mangia Woods, Sari, Landea Briones, Rafaela, Sayigh, Laela S., Redaelli, Laura, Mangia Woods, Sari, Landea Briones, Rafaela, and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
To learn more about occurrence and behavior of a recently discovered population of blue whales, passive acoustic data were collected for 15 consecutive months (January 2012 – April 2013) in the Chiloense ecoregion of southern Chile. Automatic detectors and manual auditing were used to detect blue whale songs (SEP calls) and D calls, which were then analyzed to gain insights into temporal calling patterns. We found a year-round acoustic presence of D calls, with the majority occurring during austral summer (December to April), with several sub-monthly peaks. On the other hand, no SEP calls were found during austral winter. Thus, our results support previous studies documenting austral summer residency of blue whales in the Chiloense ecoregion, although they suggest that some individuals remain in the area year-round, highlighting the importance of the Chiloense ecoregion as blue whale habitat. We also investigated daily occurrence of each call type and found that D calls occurred more frequently during dusk and night hours compared to dawn and day periods, whereas SEP calls did not show any significant diel differences. Overall, these findings contribute to a better understanding of occurrence and behavior of endangered Chilean blue whales, which can enhance our ability to develop conservation strategies in this important southern hemisphere habitat.
- Published
- 2022
21. Fine-scale habitat selection of foraging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus near Clearwater, Florida
- Author
-
Allen, Mark C., Read, Andrew J., Gaudet, Jocelyn, and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Published
- 2001
22. Demonstration of Cryptic Incompatibility in Distylous Amsinckia douglasiana
- Author
-
Casper, Brenda B., Sayigh, Laela S., and Lee, Stanley S.
- Published
- 1988
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Sex Difference in Signature Whistle Production of Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., Tyack, Peter L., Wells, Randall S., Scott, Michael D., and Irvine, A. Blair
- Published
- 1995
24. Signature Whistles of Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins Tursiops truncatus: Stability and Mother-Offspring Comparisons
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., Tyack, Peter L., Wells, Randall S., and Scott, Michael D.
- Published
- 1990
25. Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years of signature whistle research
- Author
-
Janik, Vincent M. and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Facts about signature whistles of bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., Esch, H. Carter, Wells, Randall S., and Janik, Vincent M.
- Subjects
Delphinidae ,Dolphins ,Zoology and wildlife conservation - Abstract
To link to full-text access for this article, visit this link: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.018 Byline: Laela S. Sayigh, H. Carter Esch, Randall S. Wells, Vincent M. Janik Abstract: Data collected from wild and captive bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus, over the past five decades indicate that they produce individually distinctive signature whistles that function in individual recognition and in maintaining group cohesion. However, a recent study by McCowan & Reiss (2001, Animal Behaviour, 62, 1151-1162) failed to detect signature whistles in nine of 12 captive bottlenose dolphins. These authors suggested that previous studies of signature whistles had used biased data sets, and claimed that the visual classification methods used by most researchers were inferior to their computerized technique. To evaluate their claims, we randomly selected 20 whistles from each of 20 bottlenose dolphins from recordings made during brief capture-release events in Sarasota Bay, FL, U.S.A., and asked 10 judges to visually group spectrograms based on similarity of their contours. Judges consistently grouped whistles according to the identity of the vocalizer; the mean number of whistles of a single dolphin in the judges' groups was 18.9[+ or -]1.6 out of 20 possible; resampling simulations indicate that it is highly unlikely (P Author Affiliation: (a) Department of Biology and Marine Biology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, U.S.A. (a ) Biology Department, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution (a ) Chicago Zoological Society, Chicago, IL, U.S.A. (As.) Sea Mammal Research Unit & Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, U.K. Article History: Received 24 December 2006; Revised 10 January 2007; Accepted 14 February 2007 Article Note: (miscellaneous) MS. number: A10656R
- Published
- 2007
27. Signature Whistles
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S., primary and Janik, Vincent M., additional
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Temporal and spatial distributions of delphinid species in Massachusetts Bay (USA) using passive acoustics from ocean gliders
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Baumgartner, Mark F., Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., and Baumgartner, Mark F.
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2020. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in Silva, T. L., Mooney, T. A., Sayigh, L. S., & Baumgartner, M. F. Temporal and spatial distributions of delphinid species in Massachusetts Bay (USA) using passive acoustics from ocean gliders. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 631, (2019): 1-17, doi:10.3354/meps13180., Knowledge about marine mammal habitat use is necessary for informing ecosystem-based management and mitigating human impacts. Massachusetts Bay is an important marine mammal foraging area in the Gulf of Maine and an area of substantial human activity, but delphinid habitat use is poorly understood. The goals of this work were to (1) document temporal and spatial occurrence of delphinid species in Massachusetts Bay using passive acoustic monitoring from ocean gliders and (2) explore the potential influences of environmental conditions on delphinid distributions. Gliders were deployed in late fall and early winter of 2014 and 2015-2016 and were equipped with a digital acoustic recorder and conductivity-temperature-depth instrument. Gliders surveyed an area of approximately 1000 km2. Delphinid whistles were detected on 93 of 128 (73%) deployment days. Animals were detected more often at night. Presence was consistent over 2 years, although detection rates showed annual and monthly variability. Spatial distribution differed between years, but most detections occurred close to Stellwagen Bank. Visual assessment of spectrograms suggests the presence of 2 species, Atlantic white-sided dolphins and common dolphins. The reoccurrence of 2 probable signature whistles over several weeks and consecutive winter seasons suggests prolonged occupancy during winter and possible annual site fidelity. These data show a consistent and frequent presence of delphinids near a known marine mammal foraging area (Stellwagen Bank) during late fall and winter and are a first step towards understanding both how odontocetes influence the Massachusetts Bay/Gulf of Maine ecosystem and how they may be impacted by human activities., We gratefully acknowledge the NOAA Northeast Fisheries Science Center, Stellwagen Bank National Marine Sanctuary, The Nature Conservancy, Massachusetts Division of Marine Fisheries, and the University of Massachusetts Dartmouth for their collaboration and support for this project. We thank Susan Parks, Julie Oswald, Sofie Van Parijs, and Danielle Cholewiak for helpful discussionsand sharing acoustic recordings for species comparisons. We are grateful to Ben Hodges for critical assistance with preparing, deploying, and recovering gliders. Thanks to Michael Thompson for assistance with spatial analysis and Dave Wiley for support and insights into the Stellwagen Bank ecosystem. The WHOI Marine Mammal Center provided additional funding for this work. Funding support for T.L.S. was provided by the NOAA Dr. Nancy Foster Scholarship. Finally, we thank the 3 anonymous re viewers for their comments and suggestions that improved this manuscript.
- Published
- 2020
29. Signal-specific amplitude adjustment to noise in common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Kragh, Ida M., McHugh, Katherine, Wells, Randall S., Sayigh, Laela S., Janik, Vincent M., Tyack, Peter L., Jensen, Frants H., Kragh, Ida M., McHugh, Katherine, Wells, Randall S., Sayigh, Laela S., Janik, Vincent M., Tyack, Peter L., and Jensen, Frants H.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © Company of Biologists, 2019. This article is posted here by permission of Company of Biologists for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Journal of Experimental Biology 222 (2019): jeb.216606, doi: 10.1242/jeb.216606, Anthropogenic underwater noise has increased over the past century, raising concern about the impact on cetaceans that rely on sound for communication, navigation and locating prey and predators. Many terrestrial animals increase the amplitude of their acoustic signals to partially compensate for the masking effect of noise (the Lombard response), but it has been suggested that cetaceans almost fully compensate with amplitude adjustments for increasing noise levels. Here, we used sound-recording DTAGs on pairs of free-ranging common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) to test (i) whether dolphins increase signal amplitude to compensate for increasing ambient noise and (ii) whether adjustments are identical for different signal types. We present evidence of a Lombard response in the range 0.1–0.3 dB per 1 dB increase in ambient noise, which is similar to that of terrestrial animals, but much lower than the response reported for other cetaceans. We found that signature whistles tended to be louder and with a lower degree of amplitude adjustment to noise compared with non-signature whistles, suggesting that signature whistles may be selected for higher output levels and may have a smaller scope for amplitude adjustment to noise. The consequence of the limited degree of vocal amplitude compensation is a loss of active space during periods of increased noise, with potential consequences for group cohesion, conspecific encounter rates and mate attraction., Fieldwork in Sarasota was funded by the Grossman Foundation, the Office of Naval Research, and Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution. Health assessments were funded by Dolphin Quest, Inc. I.M.K. received support from the Danish Acoustical Society (Dansk Akustisk Selskab). P.L.T. received funding from the University of St Andrews, the Office of Naval Research (N00014-19-1-2560) and the MASTS pooling initiative (The Marine Alliance for Science and Technology for Scotland). F.H.J. was supported by the Office of Naval Research (N00014-1410410) and an AIAS-COFUND fellowship from Aarhus Institute of Advanced Studies under the FP7-PEOPLE programme of the EU (agreement no. 609033). All support is gratefully acknowledged., 2020-11-08
- Published
- 2020
30. Vocal learning in cetaceans
- Author
-
Tyack, Peter L., primary and Sayigh, Laela S., additional
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Eavesdropping on Marine Mammal Conversations: An Activity Suitable for the Visually Impaired
- Author
-
Curran, Mary Carla, primary, Patterson, Kathleen, additional, and Sayigh, Laela S., additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Acoustic recordings of rough-toothed dolphin (Steno bredanensis) offshore Eastern Sicily (Mediterranean Sea)
- Author
-
Caruso, Francesco, Sciacca, Virginia, Parisi, Ignazio, Viola, Salvatore, de Vincenzi, Giovanni, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Li, Songhai, Filiciotto, Francesco, Moulins, Aurelie, Tepsich, Paola, Rosso, Massimiliano, Caruso, Francesco, Sciacca, Virginia, Parisi, Ignazio, Viola, Salvatore, de Vincenzi, Giovanni, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Li, Songhai, Filiciotto, Francesco, Moulins, Aurelie, Tepsich, Paola, and Rosso, Massimiliano
- Abstract
Author Posting. © Acoustical Society of America, 2019. This article is posted here by permission of Acoustical Society of America for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 146(3), (2019): EL286-EL292, doi:10.1121/1.5126118., ough-toothed dolphin's abundance and distribution is largely unknown worldwide and evaluation of its conservation status in the Mediterranean Sea is necessary. A rough-toothed dolphin was sighted offshore Eastern Sicily (Mediterranean Sea) in July 2017 and acoustic data were acquired in the same area of Watkins, Tyack, Moore, and Notarbartolo di Sciara [(1987). Mar. Mamm. Sci. 3, 78–82]. An automatic detection algorithm was developed to identify the echolocation clicks recorded within both datasets and a recurrent inter-click interval value was identified during the new encounter. Distinctive whistle classes were also identified with similar contour shapes within both datasets., The research has been partially funded by ONR Award (No. N00014-16-1-3017) and National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 41422604 and 41306169). The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. They wish to thank Professor Gianni Pavan for the software seapro. F.C. wants to thank the President's International Fellowship Initiative (PIFI) of the Chinese Academy of Sciences. F.C. and V.S. contributed equally to this work., 2020-03-25
- Published
- 2019
33. Individual Signatures in Animal Groups: Cetaceans
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S. and Janik, Vincent M.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Signal-specific amplitude adjustment to noise in common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Kragh, Ida M., primary, McHugh, Katherine, additional, Wells, Randall S., additional, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, Janik, Vincent M., additional, Tyack, Peter L., additional, and Jensen, Frants H., additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Individual recognition in wild bottlenose dolphins: a field test using playback experiments
- Author
-
SAYIGH, LAELA S., TYACK, PETER L., WELLS, RANDALL S., SOLOW, ANDREW R., SCOTT, MICHAEL D., and IRVINE, A.B.
- Published
- 1999
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Characterizing Chilean blue whale vocalizations with DTAGs : a test of using tag accelerometers for caller identification
- Author
-
Saddler, Mark R., Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Hickmott, Leigh S., Chiang, Gustavo, Landea Briones, Rafaela, Bahamonde, Paulina A., Howes, Gloria, Segre, Paolo S., Sayigh, Laela S., Saddler, Mark R., Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Hickmott, Leigh S., Chiang, Gustavo, Landea Briones, Rafaela, Bahamonde, Paulina A., Howes, Gloria, Segre, Paolo S., and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © Company of Biologists, 2017. This article is posted here by permission of Company of Biologists for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Journal of Experimental Biology 220 (2017): 4119-4129, doi: 10.1242/jeb.151498., Vocal behavior of blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) in the Gulf of Corcovado, Chile, was analysed using both audio and accelerometer data from digital acoustic recording tags (DTAGs). Over the course of three austral summers (2014, 2015 and 2016), seventeen tags were deployed, yielding 124 h of data. We report the occurrence of Southeast Pacific type 2 (SEP2) calls, which exhibit peak frequencies, durations and timing consistent with previous recordings made using towed and moored hydrophones. We also describe tonal downswept (D) calls, which have not been previously described for this population. As being able to accurately assign vocalizations to individual whales is fundamental for studying communication and for estimating population densities from call rates, we further examine the feasibility of using high-resolution DTAG accelerometers to identify low-frequency calls produced by tagged blue whales. We cross-correlated acoustic signals with simultaneous tri-axial accelerometer readings in order to analyse the phase match as well as the amplitude of accelerometer signals associated with low-frequency calls, which provides a quantitative method of determining if a call is associated with a detectable acceleration signal. Our results suggest that vocalizations from nearby individuals are also capable of registering accelerometer signals in the tagged whale's DTAG record. We cross-correlate acceleration vectors between calls to explore the possibility of using signature acceleration patterns associated with sounds produced within the tagged whale as a new method of identifying which accelerometer-detectable calls originate from the tagged animal., Fieldwork for this project was funded by the Melimoyu Ecosystem Research Institute Foundation and the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution. Analysis was supported by a Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution Summer Student Fellowship., 2018-09-07
- Published
- 2017
37. Successful suction-cup tagging of a small delphinid species, Stenella attenuata : insights into whistle characteristics
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Baird, Robin W., Tyack, Peter L., Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Baird, Robin W., and Tyack, Peter L.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © The Author(s), 2016. This is the author's version of the work. It is posted here by permission of Society for Marine Mammalogy for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Marine Mammal Science 33 (2017): 653–668, doi:10.1111/mms.12376., The Delphinidae is the most diverse family of cetaceans, with 38 species recognized. Small pelagic delphinids are also the most abundant cetaceans world-wide, yet their communication and behavior remain poorly understood. Many populations live in relatively remote habitats, which creates challenges in accessing study animals. Small odontocete species often face numerous anthropogenic stressors. For example, many pelagic delphinids incur significant interactions with fisheries (Gerrodette and Forcada 2005, Geijer and Read 2013). With a wide distribution, many delphinid populations utilize habitats that also are important for human seagoing activities that produce intense sound, such as seismic surveys or naval sonar exercises that may disturb or harm them. Many U.S. naval sonar exercises take place on naval training ranges such as those in in Hawai‘i (Baird et al. 2013), California (Carretta et al. 1995, Henderson et al. 2014), and the Bahamas (DeRuiter et al. 2013). At least one delphinid stranding event involving melon-headed whales (Peponocephala electra) was correlated with military activities (Southall et al. 2006); a mass stranding of melon-headed whales has also been associated with multi-beam echosounder operations as part of a seismic survey (Southall et al. 2013). Because many of these delphinid groups can number in the 100s to 1,000s, fisheries or sonar exposures can account for the highest estimates of marine mammal “takes” in related Environmental Impact Assessments (Department of the Navy 2013). Given the potential for anthropogenic interactions with large numbers of individual delphinids, improved methods of studying small delphinids are invaluable to understand, reduce, or mitigate potential human influences on these animals., This project was funded by the Office of Naval Research (award number: N000141110612; WHOI Marine Mammal Center, and the Sawyer and Penzance Endowed Funds, with additional field time funded by grants through Cascadia Research Collective by the National Oceanographic Partnership Program (through the Alaska SeaLife Center) and the Pacific Islands Fisheries Science Center. P.L.T. acknowledges the support of the MASTS pooling initiative (The Marine Alliance for Science and Technology for 355 Scotland). MASTS is funded by the Scottish Funding Council (grant reference HR09011) and contributing institutions., 2017-12-12
- Published
- 2017
38. Characterizing Chilean blue whale vocalizations with DTAGs: a test of using tag accelerometers for caller identification
- Author
-
Saddler, Mark R., primary, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, additional, Hickmott, Leigh S., additional, Chiang, Gustavo, additional, Landea-Briones, Rafaela, additional, Bahamonde, Paulina A., additional, Howes, Gloria, additional, Segre, Paolo S., additional, and Sayigh, Laela S., additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. First observed wild birth and acoustic record of a possible infanticide attempt on a common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Perrtree, Robin M., Sayigh, Laela S., Williford, Allison, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Curran, Mary C., Cox, Tara M., Perrtree, Robin M., Sayigh, Laela S., Williford, Allison, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Curran, Mary C., and Cox, Tara M.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © The Author(s), 2015. This is the author's version of the work. It is posted here by permission of Society for Marine Mammalogy for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Marine Mammal Science 32 (2016): 376–385, doi:10.1111/mms.12248., We observed the birth of a common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) followed immediately by a possible infanticide attempt in the estuary near Savannah, Georgia. Our report is unique in several ways: first, we witnessed the birth of the calf; second, we observed infanticidal behavior almost immediately afterward; and third, we obtained acoustic recordings concurrent with the possible infanticidal behavior. Our observations provide insight into aggressive, possible infanticidal, behavior in bottlenose dolphins., Boat time and support was provided by Department of Education/Title VII Award P382G090003. Additional support was provided by EDGE (Enhancing Diversity in Geosciences Education through Costal Research in Port City) NSF award GEO-0194680., 2016-07-14
- Published
- 2016
40. Whistle characteristics and daytime dive behavior in pantropical spotted dolphins (Stenella attenuata) in Hawai‘i measured using digital acoustic recording tags (DTAGs)
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Tyack, Peter L., Baird, Robin W., Oswald, Julie N., Silva, Tammy L., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Tyack, Peter L., Baird, Robin W., and Oswald, Julie N.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © Acoustical Society of America, 2016. This article is posted here by permission of Acoustical Society of America for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 140 (2016): 421, doi:10.1121/1.4955081., This study characterizes daytime acoustic and dive behavior of pantropical spotted dolphins (Stenella attenuata) in Hawai‘i using 14.58 h of data collected from five deployments of digital acoustic recording tags (DTAG3) in 2013. For each tagged animal, the number of whistles, foraging buzzes, dive profiles, and dive statistics were calculated. Start, end, minimum, and maximum frequencies, number of inflection points and duration were measured from 746 whistles. Whistles ranged in frequency from 9.7 ± 2.8 to 19.8 ± 4.2 kHz, had a mean duration of 0.7 ± 0.5 s and a mean of 1.2 ± 1.2 inflection points. Thirteen foraging buzzes were recorded across all tags. Mean dive depth and duration were 16 ± 9 m and 1.9 ± 1.0 min, respectively. Tagged animals spent the majority of time in the upper 10 m (76.9% ± 16.1%) of the water column. Both whistle frequency characteristics and dive statistics measured here were similar to previously reported values for spotted dolphins in Hawai‘i. Shallow, short dive profiles combined with few foraging buzzes provide evidence that little spotted dolphin feeding behavior occurs during daytime hours. This work represents one of the first successful DTAG3 studies of small pelagic delphinids, providing rare insights into baseline bioacoustics and dive behavior., This project was funded by the Office of Naval Research (award number: N000141110612; Program Manager Michael J. Weise), the WHOI Marine Mammal Center, and the Sawyer and Penzance Endowed Funds, with additional field time funded by grants through Cascadia Research Collective by the National Oceanographic Partnership Program (through the Alaska SeaLife Center) and the Pacific Islands Fisheries Science Center. Funding to support P.L.T. was also received from the MASTS pooling initiative (The Marine Alliance for Science and Technology for Scotland) and their support is gratefully acknowledged. MASTS is funded by the Scottish Funding Council (grant reference HR09011) and contributing institutions.
- Published
- 2016
41. Successful suction‐cup tagging of a small delphinid species, Stenella attenuata : Insights into whistle characteristics
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., primary, Aran Mooney, T., additional, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, Baird, Robin W., additional, and Tyack, Peter L., additional
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Whistle characteristics and daytime dive behavior in pantropical spotted dolphins (Stenella attenuata) in Hawai‘i measured using digital acoustic recording tags (DTAGs)
- Author
-
Silva, Tammy L., primary, Mooney, T. Aran, additional, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, Tyack, Peter L., additional, Baird, Robin W., additional, and Oswald, Julie N., additional
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. A preliminary investigation into the ecology and behavior of blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) in the Gulf of Corcovado, Chile
- Author
-
Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Hickmott, Leigh S., Landea Briones, Rafaela, Howes, Gloria, Sayigh, Laela S., Bocconcelli, Alessandro, Hickmott, Leigh S., Landea Briones, Rafaela, Howes, Gloria, and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
A joint effort between WHOI and the Melimoyu Ecosystem Research Institute (MERI) sought to gain a better understanding of a population of blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus) in the Gulf of Corcovado, Chile. A cruise in March 2014 resulted in the deployment of 5 DTAGs, which are miniature sound and orientation recording tags that are attached via suction cups. A total of five tag deployments on four individual whales were achieved, totaling 21 hr 11 min. Dives were predominantly between 10 and 50 m in depth, with a maximum of 139 m. Sloughed skin found on the suction cups of recovered tags and fecal samples were preserved to be used for genetic, dietary and pollutant analyses. Acoustic data on the tags revealed numerous calls from distant blue whales, and an apparent call exchange was recorded between a tagged juvenile whale and a distant animal. Photo-identification images and acoustic recordings of all marine mammal species encountered were obtained whenever possible; these included humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae), Peale’s dolphins (Lagenorhynchus australis), Chilean dolphins (Cephalorhynchus eutropia), and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Continuation of this collaboration has great potential to provide information to policy makers regarding how to protect the unique habitats in this region., Funding was provided by the Melimoyu Ecosystem Research Institute
- Published
- 2015
44. First observed wild birth and acoustic record of a possible infanticide attempt on a common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Perrtree, Robin M., primary, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, Williford, Allison, additional, Bocconcelli, Alessandro, additional, Curran, Mary Carla, additional, and Cox, Tara M., additional
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Repeated call types in Hawaiian melon-headed whales (Peponocephala electra)
- Author
-
Kaplan, Maxwell B., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., Baird, Robin W., Kaplan, Maxwell B., Mooney, T. Aran, Sayigh, Laela S., and Baird, Robin W.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © Acoustical Society of America, 2014. This article is posted here by permission of Acoustical Society of America for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 136 (2014): 1394, doi:10.1121/1.4892759., Melon-headed whales are pantropical odontocetes that are often found near oceanic islands. While considered sound-sensitive, their bioacoustic characteristics are relatively poorly studied. The goal of this study was to characterize the vocal repertoire of melon-headed whales to determine whether they produce repeated calls that could assist in recognition of conspecifics. The first tag-based acoustic recordings of three melon-headed whales were analyzed. Tag records were visually and aurally inspected and all calls were individually extracted. Non-overlapping calls with sufficient signal-to-noise were then parameterized and visually grouped into categories of repeated call types. Thirty-six call categories emerged. Categories differed significantly in duration, peak and centroid frequency, and −3 dB bandwidth. Calls of a given type were more likely to follow each other than expected. These data suggest that repeated calls may function in individual, subgroup, or group recognition. Repeated call production could also serve to enhance signal detection in large groups with many individuals producing simultaneous calls. Results suggest that caution should be used in developing automatic classification algorithms for this species based on small sample sizes, as they may be dominated by repeated calls from a few individuals, and thus not representative of species- or population-specific acoustic parameters., This project was funded by the Office of Naval Research (award number: N000141110612; Program Manager Michael J. Weise), WHOI Marine Mammal Center, and the Sawyer and Penzance Endowed Funds, with additional field time funded by grants through Cascadia Research Collective by the National Oceanographic Partnership Program (through the Alaska SeaLife Center) and the Pacific Islands Fisheries Science Center.
- Published
- 2014
46. Repeated call types in Hawaiian melon-headed whales (Peponocephala electra)
- Author
-
Kaplan, Maxwell B., primary, Aran Mooney, T., additional, Sayigh, Laela S., additional, and Baird, Robin W., additional
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Those dolphins aren't just whistling in the dark
- Author
-
Tyack, Peter L. and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Subjects
Marine mammals -- Research ,Dolphins ,Animal communication -- Research - Published
- 1989
48. Identifying signature whistles from recordings of groups of unrestrained bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
- Author
-
Janik, Vincent M., King, Stephanie L., Sayigh, Laela S., Wells, Randall S., Janik, Vincent M., King, Stephanie L., Sayigh, Laela S., and Wells, Randall S.
- Abstract
Author Posting. © The Author(s), 2011. This is the author's version of the work. It is posted here by permission of John Wiley & Sons for personal use, not for redistribution. The definitive version was published in Marine Mammal Science 29 (2013): 109–122, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00549.x., Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have individually-distinctive signature whistles. Each individual dolphin develops its own unique frequency modulation pattern and uses it to broadcast its identity. However, underwater sound localization is challenging, and researchers have had difficulties identifying signature whistles. The traditional method to identify them involved isolating individuals. In this context, the signature whistle is the most commonly produced whistle type of an animal. However, most studies on wild dolphins cannot isolate animals. We present a novel method, SIGID, that can identify signature whistles in recordings of groups of dolphins recorded via a single hydrophone. We found that signature whistles tend to be delivered in bouts with whistles of the same type occurring within 1-10 s of each other. Non-signature whistles occur over longer or shorter periods, and this distinction can be used to identify signature whistles in a recording. We tested this method on recordings from wild and captive bottlenose dolphins and show thresholds needed to identify signature whistles reliably. SIGID will facilitate the study of signature whistle use in the wild, signature whistle diversity between different populations, and potentially allow signature whistles to be used in mark-recapture studies., This work was supported by Dolphin Quest, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) Fisheries Service, Disney’s Animal Programs and Mote Marine Laboratory (R.S.W.), Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute (L.S.S. and R.S.W.), and a Royal Society University Research Fellowship (V.M.J.).
- Published
- 2013
49. The encoding of individual identity in dolphin signature whistles : how much information is needed?
- Author
-
Kershenbaum, Arik, Sayigh, Laela S., Janik, Vincent M., Kershenbaum, Arik, Sayigh, Laela S., and Janik, Vincent M.
- Abstract
© The Author(s), 2013. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License. The definitive version was published in PLoS ONE 8 (2013): e77671, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077671., Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) produce many vocalisations, including whistles that are unique to the individual producing them. Such “signature whistles” play a role in individual recognition and maintaining group integrity. Previous work has shown that humans can successfully group the spectrographic representations of signature whistles according to the individual dolphins that produced them. However, attempts at using mathematical algorithms to perform a similar task have been less successful. A greater understanding of the encoding of identity information in signature whistles is important for assessing similarity of whistles and thus social influences on the development of these learned calls. We re-examined 400 signature whistles from 20 individual dolphins used in a previous study, and tested the performance of new mathematical algorithms. We compared the measure used in the original study (correlation matrix of evenly sampled frequency measurements) to one used in several previous studies (similarity matrix of time-warped whistles), and to a new algorithm based on the Parsons code, used in music retrieval databases. The Parsons code records the direction of frequency change at each time step, and is effective at capturing human perception of music. We analysed similarity matrices from each of these three techniques, as well as a random control, by unsupervised clustering using three separate techniques: k-means clustering, hierarchical clustering, and an adaptive resonance theory neural network. For each of the three clustering techniques, a seven-level Parsons algorithm provided better clustering than the correlation and dynamic time warping algorithms, and was closer to the near-perfect visual categorisations of human judges. Thus, the Parsons code captures much of the individual identity information present in signature whistles, and may prove useful in studies requiring quantification of whistle similarity., Arik Kershenbaum is a Postdoctoral Fellow at the National Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis, an Institute sponsored by the National Science Foundation, the U.S. Department of Homeland Security, and the U.S. Department of Agriculture through NSF Award #EF-0832858, with additional support from The University of Tennessee, Knoxville. Part of this work was conducted while Arik Kershenbaum was provided with a doctoral scholarship by the University of Haifa. Funding for access to the dolphins for recordings was provided by Dolphin Quest and the Chicago Zoological Society.
- Published
- 2013
50. Development and functions of signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus
- Author
-
Sayigh, Laela S. and Sayigh, Laela S.
- Abstract
Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution September 1992, This thesis presents data on the development and functions of individually distinctive signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops tnmcatus. Research was conducted at a study site near Sarasota, Florida, where a resident community of bottlenose dolphins have been the focus of a long-term, ongoing study. Through observations and censuses, researchers have gained information on home ranges and association patterns among individuals. A temporary capture and release program has provided opportunities to collect basic information regarding age, sex, genetic relationships, and life history of individuals, as well as to record vocalizations of known individuals. During the periods 1975-1976 and 1984-1992, 134 different individuals were recorded during temporary capture. More than half of these were recorded on two or more (up to 10) different occasions. These recordings demonstrate that free-ranging dolphins produce individually distinctive signature whistles, as was previously documented for captive dolphins. Each dolphin produced a distinctive frequency contour, or pattern of frequency changes over time, and this whistle comprised a large portion of all whistles produced. Comparisons of whistles recorded from the same individuals over periods of more than a decade indicate that these signature whistle contours are markedly stable. This extensive database of recordings of signature whistles produced by known individuals formed the basis for much of the work described in this thesis. Playback experiments conducted during temporary capture-release projects indicated that free-ranging dolphins were able to discriminate among signature whistles of familiar individuals. When these results are taken in the context of what is known about dolphin societies, which are characterized by stable individual associations intermixed with fluid patterns of association among many individuals, it appears highly likely that dolphins use signature whistles to recognize on, This work was supported by the Education office of the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, two Ocean Ventures Fund awards, the American Cetacean Society, and National Science Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Research Grant No. BNS-9014545. Additional support was provided from NIH Grant No. 1R29NS25290 and ONR Grant No. N00014-87-K-0236, both to Peter Tyack.
- Published
- 2012
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.