90 results on '"Simon J, Handley"'
Search Results
2. The Suppression of Cand Selections in Waton's Selection Task: Evidence that Inference Plays a Role
- Author
-
Aidan Feeney and Simon J. Handley
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Investigating reasoning with multiple integrated neuroscientific methods
- Author
-
Matt eRoser, Jonathan St B T Evans, Nicolas S McNair, Giorgio eFuggetta, Simon J Handley, Lauren S Carroll, and Dries eTrippas
- Subjects
reasoning ,brain connectivity ,neuroscience methods ,Methodological Integration ,neuronavigated TMS ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Alleviating the concerns with the SDT approach to reasoning: Reply to Singman and Kellen (2014)
- Author
-
Dries eTrippas, Michael F. Verde, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Response bias ,reasoning ,model fitting ,Signal detection theory ,model identifiability ,Belief Bias ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Fluency and belief bias in deductive reasoning: New indices for old effects
- Author
-
Dries eTrippas, Simon J Handley, and Michael F Verde
- Subjects
Memory ,individual differences ,reasoning ,Signal detection theory ,Belief Bias ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
Models based on signal detection theory (SDT) have occupied a prominent role in domains such as perception, categorisation, and memory. Recent work by Dube et al. (2010) suggests that the framework may also offer important insights in the domain of deductive reasoning. Belief bias in reasoning has traditionally been examined using indices based on raw endorsement rates – indices that critics have claimed are highly problematic. We discuss a new set of SDT indices fit for the investigation belief bias and apply them to new data examining the effect of perceptual disfluency on belief bias in syllogisms. In contrast to the traditional approach, the SDT indices do not violate important statistical assumptions, resulting in a decreased Type 1 error rate. Based on analyses using these novel indices we demonstrate that disfluency leads to decreased reasoning accuracy, contrary to predictions. Disfluency also appears to eliminate the typical link found between cognitive ability and the effect of beliefs on accuracy. Finally, replicating previous work, we demonstrate that cognitive ability leads to an increase in reasoning accuracy and a decrease in the response bias component of belief bias.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Modeling causal conditional reasoning data using SDT: caveats and new insights
- Author
-
Dries eTrippas, Simon J Handley, and Michael F Verde
- Subjects
reasoning ,Signal detection theory ,normative models ,Belief Bias ,causal conditionals ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. The Design Stance, Intentional Stance, and Teleological Beliefs about Biological and Non-Biological Natural Entities
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Andrew J. Roberts, and Vince Polito
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Religion and Psychology ,Adolescent ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Intention ,PsycINFO ,Religiosity ,Young Adult ,Religion and Science ,Humans ,Natural (music) ,Function (engineering) ,Problem Solving ,media_common ,Dual process theory ,Middle Aged ,Epistemology ,Intentional stance ,Teleology ,Female ,Psychological Theory ,Psychology ,Cognitive science of religion ,Social psychology ,Intuition - Abstract
Teleology involves an appeal to function to explain why things are the way they are. Among scientists and philosophers, teleological explanations are widely accepted for human-made artifacts and biological traits, yet controversial for biological and nonbiological natural entities. Prior research shows a positive relationship between religiosity and acceptance of such controversial teleological explanations. Across three large online studies, we show that the relationship between religiosity and teleological acceptance cannot be explained by acceptance of objectively false explanations. Furthermore, we show that anthropomorphism and a belief in supernatural agents each independently predict teleological acceptance. In contrast, the tendency to inhibit intuitively appealing, yet incorrect responses to simple reasoning problems was associated with lower teleological acceptance. These results provide strong support for an intention-based account of teleology, and further contribute to the existing literature which situates teleological reasoning within a dual-process framework. Several avenues of future research are discussed, including the need to dissociate implicit and explicit measures of teleological belief, and the need for a greater focus on cross-cultural variation in teleological beliefs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Belief Bias, Logical Reasoning and Presentation Order on the Syllogistic Evaluation Task
- Author
-
Nicola J. Lambell, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Presentation ,Argument ,Logical reasoning ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Phenomenon ,Syllogism ,Contrast (statistics) ,Belief bias ,Psychology ,humanities ,media_common ,Task (project management) ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Evans, Barston and Pollard, (1983) found that on the syllogistic evaluation task participants tended to endorse believable conclusions as being valid but reject unbelievable conclusions as invalid. A phenomenon known as “Belief Bias”. Additionally, they collected verbal protocols from participants and established that this influence of belief was primarily associated with initial reference to the conclusions of these syllogistic arguments. In contrast, better logical reasoning was associated with initial reference to the premises. This experiment was designed to try to direct participants’ attention to either the conclusion or the premises of a syllogistic argument with the intention of manipulating participants’ logical reasoning ability and susceptibility to belief. The results reflected an inability to alter the influence of beliefs, but in one condition where the conclusion was presented prior to the premises, there was a successful reduction in participants’ reasoning ability. The results are discussed with respect to the current theories of belief bias.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. The logic sense: exploring the role of executive functioning in belief and logic-based judgments
- Author
-
Stephanie Howarth, Simon J. Handley, and Clare R. Walsh
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Computer science ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Dual process theory ,Conditional reasoning ,050105 experimental psychology ,Philosophy ,Parallel processing (DSP implementation) ,Belief bias ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Logical inference ,AND gate - Abstract
The Default Interventionist account suggests that by default, we often generate belief-based responses when reasoning and find it difficult to draw the logical inference. Recent research, h...
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. When fast logic meets slow belief: Evidence for a parallel-processing model of belief bias
- Author
-
Dries Trippas, Simon J. Handley, and Valerie A. Thompson
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Conflict detection ,Deductive reasoning ,Adolescent ,Logic ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Models, Psychological ,Article ,050105 experimental psychology ,Thinking ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Humans ,Belief bias ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Modus ponens ,Belief structure ,05 social sciences ,Syllogism ,Dual process theory ,16. Peace & justice ,Modus tollens ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Belief ,If and only if ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Two experiments pitted the default-interventionist account of belief bias against a parallel-processing model. According to the former, belief bias occurs because a fast, belief-based evaluation of the conclusion pre-empts a working-memory demanding logical analysis. In contrast, according to the latter both belief-based and logic-based responding occur in parallel. Participants were given deductive reasoning problems of variable complexity and instructed to decide whether the conclusion was valid on half the trials or to decide whether the conclusion was believable on the other half. When belief and logic conflict, the default-interventionist view predicts that it should take less time to respond on the basis of belief than logic, and that the believability of a conclusion should interfere with judgments of validity, but not the reverse. The parallel-processing view predicts that beliefs should interfere with logic judgments only if the processing required to evaluate the logical structure exceeds that required to evaluate the knowledge necessary to make a belief-based judgment, and vice versa otherwise. Consistent with this latter view, for the simplest reasoning problems (modus ponens), judgments of belief resulted in lower accuracy than judgments of validity, and believability interfered more with judgments of validity than the converse. For problems of moderate complexity (modus tollens and single-model syllogisms), the interference was symmetrical, in that validity interfered with belief judgments to the same degree that believability interfered with validity judgments. For the most complex (three-term multiple-model syllogisms), conclusion believability interfered more with judgments of validity than vice versa, in spite of the significant interference from conclusion validity on judgments of belief. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13421-016-0680-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. The goldilocks placebo effect:Placebo effects are stronger when people select a treatment from an optimal number of choices
- Author
-
Rebecca J. Hafner, Mathew P. White, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Bach's Flower Essences ,Satisfaction ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Placebo ,050105 experimental psychology ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,0502 economics and business ,medicine ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Choice overload ,Placebo effect ,05 social sciences ,Choice optimization ,Goldilocks effect for choice ,Health ,Goldilocks principle ,Physical therapy ,050211 marketing ,Expectation disconfirmation ,Psychology ,Decision making - Abstract
People are often more satisfied with a choice (e.g., chocolates, pens) when the number of options in the choice set is “just right” (e.g., 10–12), neither too few (e.g., 2–4) nor too many (e.g., 30–40). We investigated this “Goldilocks effect” in the context of a placebo treatment. Participants reporting nonspecific complaints (e.g., headaches) chose one of Bach’s 38 Flower Essences from a choice set of 2 (low choice), 12 (optimal choice), or 38 (full choice) options to use for a 2-week period. Replicating earlier findings in the novel context of a health-related choice, participants were initially more satisfied with the essence they selected when presented with 12 versus either 2 or 38 options. More importantly, self-reported symptoms were significantly lower 2 weeks later in the optimal (12) versus nonoptimal choice conditions (2 and 38). Because there is no known active ingredient in Bach’s Flower Essences, we refer to this as the Goldilocks placebo effect. Supporting a counterfactual thinking account of the Goldilocks effect, and despite significantly fewer symptoms after 2 weeks, those in the optimal choice set condition were no longer significantly more satisfied with their choice at the end of testing. Implications for medical practice, especially patient choice, are discussed.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. The Parallel Processing Model of Belief Bias
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley and Dries Trippas
- Subjects
Parallel processing (DSP implementation) ,Computer science ,Belief bias ,Parallel computing - Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Using forced choice to test belief bias in syllogistic reasoning
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Dries Trippas, and Michael F. Verde
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Signal Detection, Psychological ,Deductive reasoning ,Adolescent ,Logic ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Culture ,Individuality ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Choice Behavior ,Language and Linguistics ,Thinking ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Argument ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Belief bias ,Set (psychology) ,Problem Solving ,Two-alternative forced choice ,Syllogism ,Response bias ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Cognitive style - Abstract
In deductive reasoning, believable conclusions are more likely to be accepted regardless of their validity. Although many theories argue that this belief bias reflects a change in the quality of reasoning, distinguishing qualitative changes from simple response biases can be difficult (Dube, Rotello, & Heit, 2010). We introduced a novel procedure that controls for response bias. In Experiments 1 and 2, the task required judging which of two simultaneously presented syllogisms was valid. Surprisingly, there was no evidence for belief bias with this forced choice procedure. In Experiment 3, the procedure was modified so that only one set of premises was viewable at a time. An effect of beliefs emerged: unbelievable conclusions were judged more accurately, supporting the claim that beliefs affect the quality of reasoning. Experiments 4 and 5 replicated and extended this finding, showing that the effect was mediated by individual differences in cognitive ability and analytic cognitive style. Although the positive findings of Experiments 3–5 are most relevant to the debate about the mechanisms underlying belief bias, the null findings of Experiments 1 and 2 offer insight into how the presentation of an argument influences the manner in which people reason.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Reasoning and Dyslexia: is Visual Memory a Compensatory Resource?
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley and Alison M. Bacon
- Subjects
Visual perception ,Visual thinking ,Recall ,Working memory ,Dyslexia ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,General Medicine ,Verbal reasoning ,medicine.disease ,Education ,Visual memory ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Effective reasoning is fundamental to problem solving and achievement in education and employment. Protocol studies have previously suggested that people with dyslexia use reasoning strategies based on visual mental representations, whereas non-dyslexics use abstract verbal strategies. This research presents converging evidence from experimental and individual differences perspectives. In Experiment 1, dyslexic and non-dyslexic participants were similarly accurate on reasoning problems, but scores on a measure of visual memory ability only predicted reasoning accuracy for dyslexics. In Experiment 2, a secondary task loaded visual memory resources during concurrent reasoning. Dyslexics were significantly less accurate when reasoning under conditions of high memory load and showed reduced ability to subsequently recall the visual stimuli, suggesting that the memory and reasoning tasks were competing for the same visual cognitive resource. The results are consistent with an explanation based on limitations in the verbal and executive components of working memory in dyslexia and the use of compensatory visual strategies for reasoning. There are implications for cognitive activities that do not readily support visual thinking, whether in education, employment or less formal everyday settings.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Base rates: Both neglected and intuitive
- Author
-
Valerie A. Thompson, Dries Trippas, Gordon Pennycook, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Diagnostic information ,Adolescent ,Concept Formation ,media_common.quotation_subject ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Neglect ,Conflict, Psychological ,Young Adult ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Problem Solving ,media_common ,Analysis of Variance ,05 social sciences ,Bayes Theorem ,Cognition ,Middle Aged ,Time limit ,Salient ,Task analysis ,Female ,Probability Learning ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Intuition - Abstract
Base-rate neglect refers to the tendency for people to underweight base-rate probabilities in favor of diagnostic information. It is commonly held that base-rate neglect occurs because effortful (Type 2) reasoning is required to process base-rate information, whereas diagnostic information is accessible to fast, intuitive (Type 1) processing (e.g., Kahneman & Frederick, 2002). To test this account, we instructed participants to respond to base-rate problems on the basis of "beliefs" or "statistics," both in free time (Experiments 1 and 3) and under a time limit (Experiment 2). Participants were given problems with salient stereotypes (e.g., "Jake lives in a beautiful home in a posh suburb") that either conflicted or coincided with base-rate probabilities (e.g., "Jake was randomly selected from a sample of 5 doctors and 995 nurses for conflict; 995 doctors and 5 nurses for nonconflict"). If utilizing base-rates requires Type 2 processing, they should not interfere with the processing of the presumably faster belief-based judgments, whereas belief-based judgments should always interfere with statistics judgments. However, base-rates interfered with belief judgments to the same extent as the stereotypes interfered with statistical judgments, as indexed by increased response time and decreased confidence for conflict problems relative to nonconflict. These data suggest that base-rates, while typically underweighted or neglected, do not require Type 2 processing and may, in fact, be accessible to Type 1 processing.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. The SDT model of belief bias: Complexity, time, and cognitive ability mediate the effects of believability
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Michael F. Verde, and Dries Trippas
- Subjects
Adult ,Linguistics and Language ,Logic ,Logical reasoning ,Syllogism ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Response bias ,Language and Linguistics ,Cognitive bias ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Attitude ,ROC Curve ,Humans ,Belief bias ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive load - Abstract
When people evaluate conclusions, they are often influenced by prior beliefs. Prevalent theories claim that belief bias affects the quality of syllogistic reasoning. However, recent work by Dube, Rotello, and Heit (2010) has suggested that belief bias may be a simple response bias. In Experiment 1, receiver operating characteristic analysis revealed that believability affected accuracy for complex but not for simple syllogisms. In Experiment 2, the effect of believability on accuracy disappeared when judgments were made under time pressure and with participants low in cognitive capacity. The observed effects on reasoning accuracy indicate that beliefs influence more than response bias when conditions are conducive to the use of certain reasoning strategies. The findings also underscore the need to consider individual differences in reasoning.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Reasoning on the Basis of Fantasy Content: Two Studies with High-Functioning Autistic Adolescents
- Author
-
Kinga Morsanyi and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,Logical reasoning ,Concept Formation ,Intelligence ,Context (language use) ,Fantasy ,Developmental psychology ,Executive Function ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Cognitive development ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Autistic Disorder ,Child ,Problem Solving ,Knowledge level ,Syllogism ,Cognition ,medicine.disease ,Autism ,Female ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Reasoning about problems with empirically false content can be hard, as the inferences that people draw are heavily influenced by their background knowledge. However, presenting empirically false premises in a fantasy context helps children and adolescents to disregard their beliefs, and to reason on the basis of the premises. The aim of the present experiments was to see if high-functioning adolescents with autism are able to utilize fantasy context to the same extent as typically developing adolescents when they reason about empirically false premises. The results indicate that problems with engaging in pretence in autism persist into adolescence, and this hinders the ability of autistic individuals to disregard their beliefs when empirical knowledge is irrelevant.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Making heads or tails of probability: An experiment with random generators
- Author
-
Sylvie Serpell, Simon J. Handley, and Kinga Morsanyi
- Subjects
education ,Probabilistic logic ,Sample (statistics) ,Cognition ,Representativeness heuristic ,Session (web analytics) ,Education ,Equiprobability ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Heuristics ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Randomness ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Background. The equiprobability bias is a tendency for individuals to think of probabilistic events as 'equiprobable' by nature, and to judge outcomes that occur with different probabilities as equally likely. The equiprobability bias has been repeatedly found to be related to formal education in statistics, and it is claimed to be based on a misunderstanding of the concept of randomness. Aims. The aim of the present study was to examine whether experimenting with random generators would decrease the equiprobability bias. Sample. The participants were 108 psychology students whose performance was measured either immediately after taking part in a training session ( n= 55), or without doing any training exercises ( n= 53). Method. The training session consisted of four activities. These included generating random sequences of events, and learning about the law of large numbers. Subsequently, the participants were tested on a series of equiprobability problems, and a number of other problems with similar structure and content. Results. The results indicated that the training successfully decreased the equiprobability bias. However, this effect was moderated by participants' cognitive ability (i.e., higher ability participants benefitted from the training more than participants with lower cognitive ability). Finally, the training session had the unexpected side effect of increasing students' susceptibility to the representativeness heuristic. Conclusions. Experimenting with random generators has a positive effect on students' general understanding of probability, but the same time it might increase their susceptibility to certain biases (especially, to the representativeness heuristic). These findings have important implications for using training methods to improve probabilistic reasoning performance.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Spoilt for choice: The role of counterfactual thinking in the excess choice and reversibility paradoxes
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Mathew P. White, and Rebecca J. Hafner
- Subjects
Counterfactual thinking ,Choice set ,Counterfactual conditional ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Selection (linguistics) ,Low load ,High load ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive load ,Task (project management) - Abstract
Contrary to popular belief many choice options and the ability to reverse one's initial choice are sometimes associated with decreased chooser satisfaction. Two studies investigated the role of counterfactual thinking in explaining these paradoxes. Participants chose drawing implements from either a limited (6) or extensive (24) choice set (Study 1), or an expected reversible/non-reversible selection (Study 2). Following a drawing task, satisfaction with their chosen implement was rated under either high or low cognitive load to manipulate the availability of counterfactual alternatives. In Study 1 satisfaction was higher with limited vs. extensive choice under low load. The number of counterfactuals generated mediated this effect. Under high load the pattern was reversed. Participants in Study 2 generated more counterfactuals when reversibility was expected under low but not high load and this partially mediated the impact of expected reversibility on revealed satisfaction. Implications for theoretical understanding of these paradoxes are discussed.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Logic feels so good—I like it! Evidence for intuitive detection of logicality in syllogistic reasoning
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley and Kinga Morsanyi
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Adolescent ,Logic ,Logical reasoning ,Concept Formation ,Culture ,Emotions ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Mindset ,Language and Linguistics ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Concept learning ,Credibility ,Humans ,Problem Solving ,Cognitive science ,Analysis of Variance ,Subliminal stimuli ,Syllogism ,Reproducibility of Results ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Female ,Psychology ,Intuition ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive load ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
When people evaluate syllogisms, their judgments of validity are often biased by the believability of the conclusions of the problems. Thus, it has been suggested that syllogistic reasoning performance is based on an interplay between a conscious and effortful evaluation of logicality and an intuitive appreciation of the believability of the conclusions (e.g., Evans, Newstead, Allen, & Pollard, 1994). However, logic effects in syllogistic reasoning emerge even when participants are unlikely to carry out a full logical analysis of the problems (e.g., Shynkaruk & Thompson, 2006). There is also evidence that people can implicitly detect the conflict between their beliefs and the validity of the problems, even if they are unable to consciously produce a logical response (e.g., De Neys, Moyens, & Vansteenwegen, 2010). In 4 experiments we demonstrate that people intuitively detect the logicality of syllogisms, and this effect emerges independently of participants' conscious mindset and their cognitive capacity. This logic effect is also unrelated to the superficial structure of the problems. Additionally, we provide evidence that the logicality of the syllogisms is detected through slight changes in participants' affective states. In fact, subliminal affective priming had an effect on participants' subjective evaluations of the problems. Finally, when participants misattributed their emotional reactions to background music, this significantly reduced the logic effect.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Are systemizing and autistic traits related to talent and interest in mathematics and engineering? Testing some of the central claims of the empathizing-systemizing theory
- Author
-
Caterina Primi, Silvia Galli, Francesca Chiesi, Kinga Morsanyi, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Autism-spectrum quotient ,Self-efficacy ,Empathizing–systemizing theory ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Cognition ,Empathy ,Developmental psychology ,Theory of mind ,Aptitude ,Psychological testing ,Psychology ,General Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
In two experiments, we tested some of the central claims of the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Experiment 1 showed that the systemizing quotient (SQ) was unrelated to performance on a mathematics test, although it was correlated with statistics-related attitudes, self-efficacy, and anxiety. In Experiment 2, systemizing skills, and gender differences in these skills, were more strongly related to spatial thinking styles than to SQ. In fact, when we partialled the effect of spatial thinking styles, SQ was no longer related to systemizing skills. Additionally, there was no relationship between the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the SQ, or skills and interest in mathematics and mechanical reasoning. We discuss the implications of our findings for the E-S theory, and for understanding the autistic cognitive profile.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Autism and performance on the suppression task: Reasoning, context and complexity
- Author
-
Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, Rebecca McKenzie, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Elementary cognitive task ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Context (language use) ,Conditional reasoning ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Task (project management) ,Developmental psychology ,Philosophy ,Typically developing ,Autism spectrum disorder ,medicine ,Autism ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Psychology ,Counterexample ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
In this study both adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and typically developing controls were presented with conditional reasoning problems using familiar content. In this task both valid and fallacious conditional inferences that would otherwise be drawn can be suppressed if counterexample cases are brought to mind. Such suppression occurs when additional premises are presented, whose effect is to suggest such counterexample cases. In this study we predicted and observed that this suppression effect was substantially and significantly weaker for autistic participants. We take this as evidence that autistics are less contextualised in their reasoning, a finding that can be linked to research on autism on a variety of other cognitive tasks.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Logic, beliefs, and instruction: A test of the default interventionist account of belief bias
- Author
-
Stephen E. Newstead, Dries Trippas, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Universities ,Logic ,Logical reasoning ,Culture ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Language and Linguistics ,Thinking ,Judgment ,Bias ,Predictive Value of Tests ,Credibility ,Reaction Time ,Content validity ,Humans ,Belief bias ,Students ,Set (psychology) ,Analysis of Variance ,Reproducibility of Results ,Cognition ,Response bias ,Cognitive bias ,Female ,Cues ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
According to dual-process accounts of thinking, belief-based responses on reasoning tasks are generated as default but can be intervened upon in favor of logical responding, given sufficient time, effort, or cognitive resource. In this article, we present the results of 5 experiments in which participants were instructed to evaluate the conclusions of logical arguments on the basis of either their logical validity or their believability. Contrary to the predictions arising from these accounts, the logical status of the presented conclusion had a greater impact on judgments concerning its believability than did the believability of the conclusion on judgments about whether it followed logically. This finding was observed when instructional set was presented as a between-participants factor (Experiment 1), when instruction was indicated prior to problem presentation by a cue (Experiment 2), and when the cue appeared simultaneously with conclusion presentation (Experiments 3 and 4). The finding also extended to a range of simple and more complex argument forms (Experiment 5). In these latter experiments, belief-based judgments took significantly longer than those made under logical instructions. We discuss the implications of these findings for default interventionist accounts of belief bias.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Cognitive Psychological Support for the ADC Model of Moral Judgment
- Author
-
Michał Białek, Sylvia Terbeck, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
General Neuroscience ,Psychological support ,Cognition ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
“It is important to provide a common theoretical framework that would link the neuroscience and cognitive science literature on decision making and moral judgment, and the ADC approach with the REA...
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Dyslexia and reasoning: The importance of visual processes
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley and Alison M. Bacon
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Cognitive science ,Analysis of Variance ,Chi-Square Distribution ,Visual perception ,Visual comparison ,Syllogism ,Dyslexia ,Cognition ,Verbal reasoning ,medicine.disease ,Thinking ,Visual memory ,Mental Recall ,Imagination ,Visual Perception ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,Verbal memory ,Psychology ,General Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Recent research has suggested that individuals with dyslexia rely on explicit visuospatial representations for syllogistic reasoning while most non-dyslexics opt for an abstract verbal strategy. This paper investigates the role of visual processes in relational reasoning amongst dyslexic reasoners. Expt 1 presents written and verbal protocol evidence to suggest that reasoners with dyslexia generate detailed representations of relational properties and use these to make a visual comparison of objects. Non-dyslexics use a linear array of objects to make a simple transitive inference. Expt 2 examined evidence for the visual-impedance effect which suggests that visual information detracts from reasoning leading to longer latencies and reduced accuracy. While non-dyslexics showed the impedance effects predicted, dyslexics showed only reduced accuracy on problems designed specifically to elicit imagery. Expt 3 presented problems with less semantically and visually rich content. The non-dyslexic group again showed impedance effects, but dyslexics did not. Furthermore, in both studies, visual memory predicted reasoning accuracy for dyslexic participants, but not for non-dyslexics, particularly on problems with highly visual content. The findings are discussed in terms of the importance of visual and semantic processes in reasoning for individuals with dyslexia, and we argue that these processes play a compensatory role, offsetting phonological and verbal memory deficits.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. The influence of cognitive ability and instructional set on causal conditional inference
- Author
-
David E. Over, Simon J. Handley, Helen Neilens, and Jonathan St. B. T. Evans
- Subjects
Male ,Universities ,Pragmatic reasoning ,Physiology ,Statement (logic) ,Culture ,Inference ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Thinking ,Cognition ,Bias ,Physiology (medical) ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Belief bias ,Attention ,Students ,Set (psychology) ,General Psychology ,Analysis of Variance ,Statistical model ,Dual process theory ,General Medicine ,Semantics ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Female ,Probability Learning ,Psychological Theory ,Psychology ,Social psychology - Abstract
We report a large study in which participants are invited to draw inferences from causal conditional sentences with varying degrees of believability. General intelligence was measured, and participants were split into groups of high and low ability. Under strict deductive-reasoning instructions, it was observed that higher ability participants were significantly less influenced by prior belief than were those of lower ability. This effect disappeared, however, when pragmatic reasoning instructions were employed in a separate group. These findings are in accord with dual-process theories of reasoning. We also took detailed measures of beliefs in the conditional sentences used for the reasoning tasks. Statistical modelling showed that it is not belief in the conditional statement per se that is the causal factor, but rather correlates of it. Two different models of belief-based reasoning were found to fit the data according to the kind of instructions and the type of inference under consideration.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Decontextualised Minds: Adolescents with Autism are Less Susceptible to the Conjunction Fallacy than Typically Developing Adolescents
- Author
-
Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, Kinga Morsanyi, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,Decision Making ,Context (language use) ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Choice Behavior ,Developmental psychology ,Executive Function ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Autistic Disorder ,Child ,Set (psychology) ,Emotional Intelligence ,Cognition ,medicine.disease ,Cognitive bias ,Conjunction (grammar) ,Developmental disorder ,Inhibition, Psychological ,Case-Control Studies ,Autism ,Female ,Conjunction fallacy ,Psychology - Abstract
The conjunction fallacy has been cited as a classic example of the automatic contextualisation of problems. In two experiments we compared the performance of autistic and typically developing adolescents on a set of conjunction fallacy tasks. Participants with autism were less susceptible to the conjunction fallacy. Experiment 2 also demonstrated that the difference between the groups did not result from increased sensitivity to the conjunction rule, or from impaired processing of social materials amongst the autistic participants. Although adolescents with autism showed less bias in their reasoning they were not more logical than the control group in a normative sense. The findings are discussed in the light of accounts which emphasise differences in contextual processing between typical and autistic populations.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Conditional reasoning in autism: Activation and integration of knowledge and belief
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, and Rebecca McKenzie
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,Concept Formation ,Culture ,Inference ,Context (language use) ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Severity of Illness Index ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Developmental psychology ,Judgment ,Concept learning ,Conditioning, Psychological ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Autistic Disorder ,Child ,Life-span and Life-course Studies ,Probability ,Demography ,Analysis of Variance ,Cognition ,Adolescent Development ,medicine.disease ,Weak central coherence theory ,humanities ,Developmental disorder ,Memory, Short-Term ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Autism ,Female ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Everyday conditional reasoning is typically influenced by prior knowledge and belief in the form of specific exceptions known as counterexamples. This study explored whether adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; N = 26) were less influenced by background knowledge than typically developing adolescents (N = 38) when engaged in conditional reasoning. Participants were presented with pretested valid and invalid conditional inferences with varying available counterexamples. The group with ASD showed significantly less influence of prior knowledge on valid inferences (p = .01) and invalid inferences (p = .01) compared with the typical group. In a secondary probability judgment task, no significant group differences were found in probabilistic judgments of the believability of the premises. Further experiments found that results could not be explained by differences between the groups in the ability to generate counterexamples or any tendency among adolescents with ASD to exhibit a "yes" response pattern. It was concluded that adolescents with ASD tend not to spontaneously contextualize presented material when engaged in everyday reasoning. These findings are discussed with reference to weak central coherence theory and the conditional reasoning literature.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. The effects and side-effects of statistics education: Psychology students’ (mis-)conceptions of probability
- Author
-
Caterina Primi, Kinga Morsanyi, Francesca Chiesi, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Need for cognition ,Equiprobability ,Logical reasoning ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Contrast (statistics) ,Cognition ,Statistics education ,Psychology ,Representativeness heuristic ,Social psychology ,Cognitive bias ,Education ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
In three studies we looked at two typical misconceptions of probability: the representativeness heuristic, and the equiprobability bias. The literature on statistics education predicts that some typical errors and biases (e.g., the equiprobability bias) increase with education, whereas others decrease. This is in contrast with reasoning theorists’ prediction who propose that education reduces misconceptions in general. They also predict that students with higher cognitive ability and higher need for cognition are less susceptible to biases. In Experiments 1 and 2 we found that the equiprobability bias increased with statistics education, and it was negatively correlated with students’ cognitive abilities. The representativeness heuristic was mostly unaffected by education, and it was also unrelated to cognitive abilities. In Experiment 3 we demonstrated through an instruction manipulation (by asking participants to think logically vs. rely on their intuitions) that the reason for these differences was that these biases originated in different cognitive processes.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Reasoning Under Time Pressure
- Author
-
Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, Alison M. Bacon, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Male ,Logic ,Culture ,Inference ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Association ,Thinking ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Reaction Time ,Content validity ,Humans ,Belief bias ,Attention ,Association (psychology) ,Problem Solving ,General Psychology ,Syllogism ,General Medicine ,Causality ,Cognitive bias ,Semantics ,Inhibition, Psychological ,Memory, Short-Term ,Reading ,Causal inference ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Stress, Psychological - Abstract
In this study, we examine the role of beliefs in conditional inference in two experiments, demonstrating a robust tendency for people to make fewer inferences from statements they disbelieve, regardless of logical validity. The main purpose of this study was to test whether participants are able to inhibit this belief effect where it constitutes a bias. This is the case when participants are specifically instructed to assume the truth of the premises. However, Experiment 1 showed that the effect is no less marked than when this instruction is given, than when it is not, although higher ability participants did show slightly less influence of belief (Experiment 2). Contrary to the findings with syllogistic reasoning, use of speeded tasks had no effect on the extent of the belief bias (both experiments), although it did considerably reduce the numbers of inferences that were drawn overall. These findings suggest that the belief bias in conditional inference is less open to volitional control than that associated with syllogistic reasoning.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Understanding causal conditionals: A study of individual differences
- Author
-
David E. Over, Helen Neilens, Simon J. Handley, and Jonathan St. B. T. Evans
- Subjects
Interpretation (logic) ,Physiology ,Truth table ,Linguistics ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,General Medicine ,Conditional reasoning ,Causality ,Large sample ,Task (project management) ,Thinking ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Physiology (medical) ,Converse ,Humans ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Problem Solving ,General Psychology - Abstract
Previous studies have suggested that a minority of university students, of lower cognitive ability, are inclined to interpret abstract conditional statements, if p then q, as if they were conjunctions: p and q. In the present study we administered the conditional truth table task to a large sample of students ( n = 160), but using realistic, everyday causal conditionals. We also measured their general intelligence. While individual differences were found, these were not consistent with some participants adopting a conjunctive interpretation of such statements. Rather, it appears that students of lower cognitive ability are rather likely to assume that a conditional implies its converse, so that it means also if q then p. The results are discussed with reference to the suppositional theory of conditionals and our more general account of hypothetical thinking.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Counterintuitive and alternative moves choice in the Water Jug task
- Author
-
Hassina P. Carder, Simon J. Handley, and Timothy J. Perfect
- Subjects
Time Factors ,Experimental psychology ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Context (language use) ,Intention ,Cognitive neuroscience ,Choice Behavior ,Task (project management) ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Perception ,Reaction Time ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Problem Solving ,media_common ,Cognitive science ,Analysis of Variance ,Counterintuitive ,Cognition ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Task analysis ,Probability Learning ,Psychology ,Forecasting ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
MOVE problems, like the Tower of London (TOL) or the Water Jug (WJ) task, are planning tasks that appear structurally similar and are assumed to involve similar cognitive processes. Carder et al. [Carder, H.P., Handley, S.J., & Perfect, T.J. ( 2004). Deconstructing the Tower of London: Alternative moves and conflict resolution as predictors of task performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 57a, 8, 1459-1483] showed that one predictor of TOL performance was the number of alternative move choices there were at a given point in the solution. In two experiments an individual move experienced on the WJ task was manipulated (perceptually consistent/counterintuitive) along with the number of alternative moves there were to choose between. A verification paradigm was employed in which participants made speeded judgements about the correctness of a move. Results showed performance was consistent with the application of a perceptual strategy accompanied by a process involving the evaluation of non-redundant alternative moves. These are discussed in the context of recent research that has examined the impact of executive dysfunction on Water Jug performance [Colvin, M.K., Dunbar, K., & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the Water Jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1129-1147].
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Logic Brightens My Day: Evidence for Implicit Sensitivity to Logical Validity
- Author
-
Michael F. Verde, Kinga Morsanyi, Simon J. Handley, and Dries Trippas
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Adolescent ,Logical reasoning ,Logic ,Culture ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,PsycINFO ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Thinking ,03 medical and health sciences ,Fluency ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Content validity ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Misattribution of memory ,Cognitive science ,Analysis of Variance ,05 social sciences ,Awareness ,Semantics ,Comprehension ,Philosophy of logic ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
A key assumption of dual process theory is that reasoning is an explicit, effortful, deliberative process. The present study offers evidence for an implicit, possibly intuitive component of reasoning. Participants were shown sentences embedded in logically valid or invalid arguments. Participants were not asked to reason but instead rated the sentences for liking (Experiment 1) and physical brightness (Experiments 2-3). Sentences that followed logically from preceding sentences were judged to be more likable and brighter. Two other factors thought to be linked to implicit processing-sentence believability and facial expression-had similar effects on liking and brightness ratings. The authors conclude that sensitivity to logical structure was implicit, occurring potentially automatically and outside of awareness. They discuss the results within a fluency misattribution framework and make reference to the literature on discourse comprehension. (PsycINFO Database Record
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Iffy beliefs: Conditional thinking and belief change
- Author
-
Rosemary J. Stevenson, Constantinos Hadjichristidis, Simon J. Handley, David E. Over, Steven A. Sloman, and Jonathan St. B. T. Evans
- Subjects
Property (philosophy) ,Logic ,Human intelligence ,Statement (logic) ,Antecedent (logic) ,Culture ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Context (language use) ,Cognition ,Thinking ,Judgment ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Reading ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Set, Psychology ,Humans ,Probability Learning ,Belief change ,Comprehension ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Problem Solving ,Cognitive psychology ,Event (probability theory) - Abstract
The ability to entertain possibilities and draw inferences about them is essential to human intelligence. We examine the hypothesis that conditional if-then statements trigger a mental simulation process in which people suppose the antecedent (if statement) to be true and evaluate the consequent (then statement) in that context. On the assumption that supposing an event to be true increases belief that the event has occurred or will occur, this hypothesis is consistent with the claim that evaluating a conditional will heighten belief in its antecedent more than in its consequent. Two experiments, employing conditionals of the form If animal A has property X, then animal B will have property X, in which X was a property that people could not readily relate to the animals, supported this claim. The effect was stronger following the evaluation of conditionals with dissimilar animal categories.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. The role of prior task experience in temporal misestimation
- Author
-
Kevin Thomas, Stephen E. Newstead, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Physiology ,Anchoring ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Cognition ,Professional Competence ,Physiology (medical) ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Duration (project management) ,General Psychology ,Planning fallacy ,05 social sciences ,General Medicine ,Time perception ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Time Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The effect of experience with a preceding task on the accuracy of predictions of duration was examined in three experiments, where two tasks comprising similar or different mental operators were performed consecutively. Results supported an anchoring account of misestimation, which states that misestimation occurs because predictions are anchored to the duration of the preceding task. Preceding performance of a longer task led to overestimation on a shorter task with similar mental operators (Experiment 1), whereas preceding performance of a shorter task comprising similar or different mental operators led to underestimation on a longer task (Experiments 1 to 3). Contrary to the planning fallacy account (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), these findings indicate that preceding task performance is considered when predicting duration, but that using such information does not necessarily improve accuracy.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Reasoning and dyslexia: A spatial strategy may impede reasoning with visually rich information
- Author
-
Alison M. Bacon, Simon J. Handley, and Emma L. McDonald
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Symbolism ,Logic ,Concept Formation ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Discrimination Learning ,Dyslexia ,Orientation ,Concept learning ,Perception ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Discrimination learning ,Students ,Problem Solving ,General Psychology ,media_common ,Cognitive science ,Syllogism ,Cognition ,Spatial cognition ,medicine.disease ,Verbal reasoning ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Imagination ,Female ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Bacon, Handley, and Newstead (2003, 2004), have presented evidence for individual differences in reasoning strategies, with most people seeming to represent and manipulate problem information using either a verbal or a spatial strategy. There is also evidence that individuals with dyslexia are inclined to conceptualise information in a visuo-spatial, rather than a verbal, way (e.g. von Károlyi et al., 2003). If so, we might expect a higher proportion of individuals with dyslexia to be spatial reasoners, compared with individuals who do not have dyslexia. The study reported here directly compared strategies reported by these two groups of participants on a syllogistic reasoning task. Moreover, problem content was manipulated so that reasoning across concrete and abstract materials could be compared. The findings suggest that whilst most individuals without dyslexia use a verbal strategy, reasoners with dyslexia do tend to adopt a spatial approach, though their performance is impaired with visually concrete materials. However, when reasoning with more abstract content, they perform comparably with non-dyslexic controls. The paper discusses these results in the light of recent research which has suggested that visual images may impede reasoning, and considers how individuals with dyslexia may differ from other reasoners.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. The logic-bias effect: The role of effortful processing in the resolution of belief-logic conflict
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Stephanie Howarth, and Clare R. Walsh
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Deductive reasoning ,Experimental psychology ,Logic ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Conflict, Psychological ,Thinking ,03 medical and health sciences ,Executive Function ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Argument ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Cognitive science ,Interpretation (logic) ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,Cognition ,Resolution (logic) ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
According to the default interventionist dual-process account of reasoning, belief-based responses to reasoning tasks are based on Type 1 processes generated by default, which must be inhibited in order to produce an effortful, Type 2 output based on the validity of an argument. However, recent research has indicated that reasoning on the basis of beliefs may not be as fast and automatic as this account claims. In three experiments, we presented participants with a reasoning task that was to be completed while they were generating random numbers (RNG). We used the novel methodology introduced by Handley, Newstead & Trippas (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 28–43, 2011), which required participants to make judgments based upon either the validity of a conditional argument or the believability of its conclusion. The results showed that belief-based judgments produced lower rates of accuracy overall and were influenced to a greater extent than validity judgments by the presence of a conflict between belief and logic for both simple and complex arguments. These findings were replicated in Experiment 3, in which we controlled for switching demands in a blocked design. Across all three experiments, we found a main effect of RNG, implying that both instructional sets require some effortful processing. However, in the blocked design RNG had its greatest impact on logic judgments, suggesting that distinct executive resources may be required for each type of judgment. We discuss the implications of our findings for the default interventionist account and offer a parallel competitive model as an alternative interpretation for our findings.
- Published
- 2015
38. Predicting the difficulty of complex logical reasoning problems
- Author
-
Ian Dennis, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, Peter Bradon, Stephen E. Newstead, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Predictive validity ,Philosophy ,Logical reasoning ,business.industry ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Rule-based system ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Artificial intelligence ,Type (model theory) ,business ,Psychology ,Variety (cybernetics) - Abstract
The aim of the present research was to develop a difficulty model for logical reasoning problems involving complex ordered arrays used in the Graduate Record Examination. The approach used involved breaking down the problems into their basic cognitive elements such as the complexity of the rules used, the number of mental models required to represent the problem, and question type. Weightings for these different elements were derived from two experimental studies and from the reasoning literature. Based on these weights, difficulty models were developed which were then tested against new data. The models had excellent predictive validity and showed the relative influence of rule based factors and factors relating to the number of underlying models. Different difficulty models were needed for different question types, suggesting that people used a variety of approaches and, at a wider level, that both mental models and mental rules may be used in reasoning.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. The negated conditional: A litmus test for the suppositional conditional?
- Author
-
Valerie A. Thompson, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Adult ,Linguistics and Language ,Computers ,Logic ,Assertion ,Conditional probability ,Contrast (statistics) ,Inference ,Linguistics ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,State of affairs ,Language and Linguistics ,Semantics ,Strict conditional ,Cognition ,Mental Processes ,Negation ,Statistics ,Humans ,Learning ,Conjunction fallacy ,Psychology ,Mathematical economics ,Problem Solving ,Language - Abstract
Under the suppositional account of conditionals, when people think about a conditional assertion, "if p then q," they engage in a mental simulation in which they imagine p holds and evaluate the probability that q holds under this supposition. One implication of this account is that belief in a conditional equates to conditional probability [P(q/p)]. In this paper, the authors examine a further implication of this analysis with respect to the wide-scope negation of conditional assertions, "it is not the case that if p then q." Under the suppositional account, nothing categorically follows from the negation of a conditional, other than a second conditional, "if p then not-q." In contrast, according to the mental model theory, a negated conditional is consistent only with the determinate state of affairs, p and not-q. In 4 experiments, the authors compare the contrasting predictions that arise from each of these accounts. The findings are consistent with the suppositional theory but are incongruent with the mental model theory of conditionals.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Persuading and dissuading by conditional argument☆
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, and Valerie A. Thompson
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Persuasion ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Informal logic ,Inference ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Dual process theory ,Language and Linguistics ,Argumentation theory ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Action (philosophy) ,Artificial Intelligence ,Argument ,Relevance (law) ,Positive economics ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Informal reasoning typically draws on a wider range of inferential behaviour than is measured by traditional inference tasks. In this paper, we developed several tasks to study informal reasoning with two novel types of conditional statements: Persuasions (e.g., if the Kyoto accord is ratified, greenhouse gas emissions will be reduced) and dissuasions (e.g., if the Kyoto accord is ratified, there will be a downturn in the economy). For these statements, the consequent event, q , is offered as an incentive or disincentive for undertaking action p . Forty-eight university students reasoned about the statements from the point of view of the writer; another forty-eight reasoned from their own perspective. We found that reasoning about these statements involves a sophisticated chain of implicit inferences (e.g., a downturn in the economy should be avoided; ratifying the accord will produce a downturn; in order to avoid a downturn the accord should not be ratified) in support of an implicit conclusion (i.e., the accord should not be ratified). When generating arguments to either support or refute a position, reasoners relied on two main strategies: addressing the truth of the conditional or arguing the merits of undertaking action p (i.e., ratifying the Kyoto accord). Finally, using a traditional conditional arguments task, we found that reasoners were more likely to adopt a deductive strategy when reasoning from the writer’s point of view than their own point of view, even though we did not include any instructions to reason logically. We discuss the relevance of these findings for formal and informal models of reasoning, the writer’s theory of mind, and the role of pragmatic implicatures in reasoning.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Suppositions, extensionality, and conditionals: A critique of the mental model theory of Johnson-Laird and Byrne (2002)
- Author
-
David E. Over, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Counterfactual conditional ,Logic ,Semantics (computer science) ,Inference ,Epistemology ,Meaning (philosophy of language) ,Cognition ,Philosophy of logic ,Psychological Theory ,Extensionality ,Humans ,General Psychology ,Ramsey RESET test ,Language ,Mathematics - Abstract
P. N. Johnson-Laird and R. M. J. Byrne proposed an influential theory of conditionals in which mental models represent logical possibilities and inferences are drawn from the extensions of possibilities that are used to represent conditionals. In this article, the authors argue that the extensional semantics underlying this theory is equivalent to that of the material, truth-functional conditional, at least for what they term "basic" conditionals, concerning arbitrary problem content. On the basis of both logical argument and psychological evidence, the authors propose that this approach is fundamentally mistaken and that conditionals must be viewed within a suppositional theory based on what philosophical logicians call the Ramsey test. The Johnson-Laird and Byrne theory is critically examined with respect to its account of basic conditionals, nonbasic conditionals, and counterfactuals.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. The story of some : Everyday pragmatic inference by children and adults
- Author
-
Aidan Feeney, Susan Scrafton, Simon J. Handley, and Amber Duckworth
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Analysis of Variance ,Adolescent ,Logic ,Statement (logic) ,Human Development ,Informal logic ,Age Factors ,Inference ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,Middle Aged ,Scalar implicature ,Pragmatics ,Thinking ,Meaning (philosophy of language) ,Cognitive development ,Humans ,Female ,Child ,Psychology ,Implicature ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The statement, some elephants have trunks, is logically true but pragmatically infelicitous. Whilst some is logically consistent with all, it is often pragmatically interpreted as precluding all. In Experiments 1 and 2, we show that with pragmatically impoverished materials, sensitivity to the pragmatic implicature associated with some is apparent earlier in development than has previously been found. Amongst 8-year-old children, we observed much greater sensitivity to the implicature in pragmatically enriched contexts. Finally, in Experiment 3, we found that amongst adults, logical responses to infelicitous some statements take longer to produce than do logical responses to felicitous some statements, and that working memory capacity predicts the tendency to give logical responses to the former kind of statement. These results suggest that some adults develop the ability to inhibit a pragmatic response in favour of a logical answer. We discuss the implications of these findings for theories of pragmatic inference.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children's reasoning
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, J. St B. T. Evans, Ian Dennis, M. Beveridge, and Alison Capon
- Subjects
Childhood development ,School age child ,Logical reasoning ,Working memory ,Short-term memory ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Developmental psychology ,Philosophy ,Inhibitory control ,Cognitive development ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The ability to reason independently from one's own goals or beliefs has long been recognised as a key characteristic of the development of formal operational thought. In this article we present the...
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Belief Bias and Figural Bias in Syllogistic Reasoning
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Nicola J. Morley, and Jonathan St. B. T. Evans
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Logic ,Culture ,05 social sciences ,Syllogism ,Reproducibility of Results ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Context (language use) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Cognitive bias ,Task (project management) ,Random Allocation ,Humans ,Belief bias ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychological Theory ,Psychology ,Prejudice ,Problem Solving ,General Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Belief bias is the tendency to be influenced by the believability of the conclusion when attempting to solve a syllogistic reasoning problem. Figural bias is the tendency to be influenced by the order in which the information is presented in the premises when attempting to solve a syllogistic reasoning problem. When studied simultaneously they enable an investigation of whether participants’ reasoning on the syllogistic reasoning task is guided by the conclusion (backward reasoning) or the premises (forward reasoning). Experiments 1 and 2 found evidence of belief bias but not figural bias on the syllogistic evaluation task paradigm. Experiments 3 and 4 found evidence of figural bias but not belief bias on the syllogistic production task paradigm. The findings highlight that different task characteristics influence performance dependent upon the nature of task presentation. These findings are discussed in the context of current theories of belief bias in syllogistic reasoning.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. In Search of Counter-Examples: Deductive Rationality in Human Reasoning
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley, Walter Schroyens, and Walter Schaeken
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Logic ,Decision Making ,Inference ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Rationality ,050105 experimental psychology ,Association ,Content validity ,Humans ,Belief bias ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Competence (human resources) ,General Psychology ,Associative property ,Cognitive science ,Computers ,05 social sciences ,Reproducibility of Results ,Cognition ,Female ,Psychological Theory ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Counterexample - Abstract
Dual-process theories come in many forms. They draw on the distinction between associative, heuristic, tacit, intuitive, or implicit processes (System 1) and rule-based, analytic, explicit processes (System 2). We present the results of contextual manipulations that have a bearing on the supposed primacy of System 1 (Stanovich & West, 2000). Experiment 1 showed that people who evaluated logically valid or invalid conditional inferences under a timing constraint ( N=56), showed a smaller effect of logical validity than did people who were not placed under a timing constraint ( N= 44). Experiment 2 similarly showed that stressing the logical constraint that only inferences that follow necessarily are to be endorsed ( N= 36) increased the size of the validity effect, as compared to that of participants ( N=33) given the standard instruction to make “logical” inferences. These findings concur with the thesis in dual-processing frameworks that “Rationality-2 processes” (Evans & Over, 1996), “test procedures” (Chater & Oaksford, 1999), or “conclusion validation processes” (Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 1991; Schroyens, Schaeken, & d'Ydewalle, 2001) serve to override the results of System 1 processes.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Working memory and reasoning: An individual differences perspective
- Author
-
Ian Dennis, Alison Capon, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Philosophy ,Working memory ,Short-term memory ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Spatial intelligence ,Context (language use) ,Cognition ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Verbal memory ,Psychology ,Verbal reasoning ,Spatial memory ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
This article reports three experiments that investigated the relationship between working memory capacity and syllogistic and five-term series spatial inference. A series of complex and simple verbal and spatial working memory measures were employed. Correlational analyses showed that verbal and spatial working memory span tasks consistently predicted syllogistic and spatial reasoning performance. A confirmatory factor analysis showed that three factors best accounted for the data—a verbal, a spatial, and a general factor. Syllogistic reasoning performance loaded all three factors, whilst spatial reasoning loaded only the general factor. The implications of these findings are discussed in the context of reasoning theories and contemporary accounts of the structure of working memory.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Individual differences in strategies for syllogistic reasoning
- Author
-
Stephen E. Newstead, Simon J. Handley, and Alison M. Bacon
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Philosophy ,Deductive reasoning ,Syllogism ,Psychology of reasoning ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Causal reasoning ,Non-monotonic logic ,Psychology ,Verbal reasoning ,Analytic reasoning - Abstract
Current theories of reasoning such as mental models or mental logic assume a universal cognitive mechanism that underlies human reasoning performance. However, there is evidence that this is not the case, for example, the work of Ford (1995), who found that some people adopted predominantly spatial and some verbal strategies in a syllogistic reasoning task. Using written and think-aloud protocols, the present study confirmed the existence of these individual differences. However, in sharp contrast to Ford, the present study found few differences in reasoning performance between the two groups, in terms of accuracy or type of conclusion generated. Hence, reasoners present an outward appearance of ubiquity, despite underlying differences in reasoning processes. These findings have implications for theoretical accounts of reasoning, and for attempts to model reasoning data. Any comprehensive account needs to account for strategic differences and how these may develop in logically untrained individuals.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Exploring the time prediction process: the effects of task experience and complexity on prediction accuracy
- Author
-
Stephen E. Newstead, Kevin Thomas, and Simon J. Handley
- Subjects
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Process (engineering) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Task completion ,Time perception ,Duration (project management) ,Everyday life ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Task (project management) - Abstract
Whilst considerable research shows that people tend to underestimate their task completion times, there is little research concerning factors that mediate the time prediction process. In Experiments 1 to 3 a simple, well-structured task, the 3-disk Tower of Hanoi, showed no evidence of underestimation; in fact, participants consistently overestimated the duration of this task. However, predictions were more accurate among participants who acquired some task experience beforehand. Task complexity was also found to be an important factor since the more cognitively complex 4- and 5-disk versions produced less biased predictions. Using a cognitively undemanding disk movement task, we found a general temporal overestimation in Experiment 4, thus suggesting that task duration might be responsible for the general lack of underestimation in the present studies. These results have implications for the planning of tasks in everyday life, and also suggest conditions under which time prediction accuracy can be improved. Copyright © 2003 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Investigating reasoning with multiple integrated neuroscientific methods
- Author
-
Nicolas A. McNair, Simon J. Handley, Jonathan St. B. T. Evans, Lauren Carroll, Giorgio Fuggetta, Dries Trippas, and Matthew E. Roser
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Working memory ,brain connectivity ,DUAL (cognitive architecture) ,Opinion Article ,Data science ,Methodological Integration ,neuronavigated TMS ,Social research ,lcsh:RC321-571 ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Neurology ,reasoning ,neuroscience methods ,Psychology ,lcsh:Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,Biological Psychiatry ,Neuroscience - Abstract
This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council Grant RES- 062-23-3285. Dual processes in reasoning: A neuropsychological study of the role of working memory
- Published
- 2015
50. Dual Processes and the Interplay between Knowledge and Structure: A New Parallel Processing Model
- Author
-
Simon J. Handley and Dries Trippas
- Subjects
Parallel processing (psychology) ,Structure (mathematical logic) ,Cognitive science ,Process (engineering) ,Heuristic ,DUAL (cognitive architecture) ,Non-monotonic logic ,Heuristics ,Psychology ,Sketch ,Epistemology - Abstract
How do reasoners resolve a conflict between two competing responses, one cued by beliefs and knowledge, and one based upon the problem's underlying logic and structure? The literature suggests that such conflicts are routinely resolved in favor of a belief-based heuristic (Type 1) response that is generated autonomously and by default. Given sufficient effort, time, and motivation, reasoners can and sometimes do engage in more deliberative (Type 2) processing and intervene on default responses, generating responses based upon the underlying structure of the problem. Such default interventionist accounts of reasoning biases have become increasingly popular in the reasoning and judgment domains. In this chapter we review recent evidence which suggests that reasoners show intuitive sensitivity to logical structure, which can interfere with belief or knowledge judgments. We further show that the tendency to respond on the basis of beliefs can be slow and effortful, can increase with development, and often depends upon working memory. We evaluate the implications of these findings for dual process accounts and conclude that existing dual process frameworks cannot account for the evidence. Finally, we sketch the principles of a new model which assumes that both structure and knowledge make intuitive and deliberative contributions to responses on reasoning and judgment tasks.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.