250 results on '"Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition"'
Search Results
2. Chimpanzees use social information to acquire a skill they fail to innovate
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, EJC, Detroy, SE, Haun, DBM, Call, J, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, EJC, Detroy, SE, Haun, DBM, and Call, J
- Published
- 2024
3. The ability to inhibit impulses is related to social behavior in long‐tailed macaques
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Development and Treatment of Psychosocial Problems, Leerstoel Baar, Leerstoel Heijden, Methodology and statistics for the behavioural and social sciences, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Vries, Anne M. Overduin‐de, Vermande, Marjolijn M., Hessen, David J., Sterck, Elisabeth H. M., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Development and Treatment of Psychosocial Problems, Leerstoel Baar, Leerstoel Heijden, Methodology and statistics for the behavioural and social sciences, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Vries, Anne M. Overduin‐de, Vermande, Marjolijn M., Hessen, David J., and Sterck, Elisabeth H. M.
- Published
- 2024
4. FSC-certified forest management benefits large mammals compared to non-FSC
- Author
-
Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Energy System Analysis, Ecology and Biodiversity, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Copernicus Institute of Sustainable Development, Zwerts, Joeri A., Sterck, E. H.M., Verweij, Pita A., Maisels, Fiona, van der Waarde, Jaap, Geelen, Emma A.M., Tchoumba, Georges Belmond, Donfouet Zebaze, Hermann Frankie, van Kuijk, Marijke, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Energy System Analysis, Ecology and Biodiversity, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Copernicus Institute of Sustainable Development, Zwerts, Joeri A., Sterck, E. H.M., Verweij, Pita A., Maisels, Fiona, van der Waarde, Jaap, Geelen, Emma A.M., Tchoumba, Georges Belmond, Donfouet Zebaze, Hermann Frankie, and van Kuijk, Marijke
- Published
- 2024
5. Evolution of Humanity: English Translation of the Original Essay by Kinji Imanishi. Including Commentaries by Contemporary Scholars
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, DeTroy, Sarah E., Hirata, Satoshi, Nakamura, Michio, Haun, Daniel B. M., van Leeuwen, Edwin J. C., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, DeTroy, Sarah E., Hirata, Satoshi, Nakamura, Michio, Haun, Daniel B. M., and van Leeuwen, Edwin J. C.
- Published
- 2024
6. Personality heterophily and friendship as drivers for successful cooperation
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Waasdorp, Sophie, Middelburg, Esmee, Sterck, Elisabeth H.M., Massen, Jorg J.M., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Waasdorp, Sophie, Middelburg, Esmee, Sterck, Elisabeth H.M., and Massen, Jorg J.M.
- Published
- 2024
7. Behavioral, physiological, and genetic drivers of coping in a non-human primate
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, D, Guojonsdóttir, AR, Chova, PE, Middelburg, Esmee, Jäckels, J, de Groot, NG, Wallner, B, Massen, JJM, Pflüger, LS, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, D, Guojonsdóttir, AR, Chova, PE, Middelburg, Esmee, Jäckels, J, de Groot, NG, Wallner, B, Massen, JJM, and Pflüger, LS
- Published
- 2024
8. What animal cultures may beget: Comment on 'Blind alleys and fruitful pathways in the comparative study of cultural cognition' by Andrew Whiten
- Author
-
van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Goldsborough, Zoë, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Artificial Intelligence ,General Physics and Astronomy ,General Agricultural and Biological Sciences - Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Individual attractiveness preferences differentially modulate immediate and voluntary attention
- Author
-
Roth, Tom, Samara, Iliana, Perea-Garcia, Juan Olvido, Kret, Mariska E., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Attentional Bias ,Male ,Beauty ,Young Adult ,Multidisciplinary ,Individuality ,Humans ,Female ,Heterosexuality ,General ,Choice Behavior - Abstract
Physical attractiveness plays a crucial role in mate choice for both men and women. This is reflected in visual attention: people immediately attend towards and look longer at attractive faces, especially when they are motivated to find a partner. However, previous studies did not incorporate real-life dating decisions. Here, we aimed to combine attentional tasks with individual attractiveness ratings and a real-life mate choice context, namely a speed-dating paradigm. We investigated whether heterosexual non-committed young adults showed biases in immediate and voluntary attention towards attractive faces and preferred dating partners. In line with previous research, we found considerable individual differences in individual attractiveness preferences. Furthermore, our results showed that men had a bias towards attractive faces and preferred dating partners in the immediate attention task, while results for women were mixed. In the voluntary attention task, however, both men and women had an attentional bias towards attractive faces and preferred dating partners. Our results suggest that individual attractiveness preferences are good predictors of especially voluntary attention. We discuss these findings from an evolutionary perspective and suggest directions for future research.
- Published
- 2023
10. A comparative study of mirror self-recognition in three corvid species
- Author
-
Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, Szabó, Anita, Bugnyar, Thomas, Massen, Jorg J M, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Mirror-mark test ,Ecology ,Behavior and Systematics ,Evolution ,Common raven ,Azure-winged magpies ,Mirror response ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Carrion crow ,Self-awareness ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Abstract
Mirror self-recognition (MSR) assessed by the Mark Test has been the staple test for the study of animal self-awareness. When tested in this paradigm, corvid species return discrepant results, with only the Eurasian magpies and the Indian house crow successfully passing the test so far, whereas multiple other corvid species fail. The lack of replicability of these positive results and the large divergence in applied methodologies calls into question whether the observed differences are in fact phylogenetic or methodological, and, if so, which factors facilitate the expression of MSR in some corvids. In this study, we (1) present new results on the self-recognition abilities of common ravens, (2) replicate results of azure-winged magpies, and (3) compare the mirror responses and performances in the mark test of these two corvid species with a third corvid species: carrion crows, previously tested following the same experimental procedure. Our results show interspecies differences in the approach of and the response to the mirror during the mirror exposure phase of the experiment as well as in the subsequent mark test. However, the performances of these species in the Mark Test do not provide any evidence for their ability of self-recognition. Our results add to the ongoing discussion about the convergent evolution of MSR and we advocate for consistent methodologies and procedures in comparing this ability across species to advance this discussion.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Early trauma leaves no social signature in sanctuary-housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
- Author
-
van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Bruinstroop, Bernadette M C, Haun, Daniel B M, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
coping ,welfare ,chimpanzees ,trauma ,sanctuary ,General Veterinary ,social deprivation ,Animal Science and Zoology - Abstract
Negative early experiences can have detrimental effects on social functioning in later life, both in humans as well as in other socially-living animals. In zoo-housed chimpanzees, recent evidence suggests that there may be a lingering signature of early trauma on individuals’ social interaction tendencies as measured by social proximity and grooming. Here, we address whether a similar effect would be observable in chimpanzees living under semi-wild conditions in an African sanctuary. By analysing party size, close proximity and social grooming, we show that in this specific sanctuary, chimpanzees that suffered early trauma (n = 42) were socially indistinguishable from chimpanzees who were born and raised by their mothers in the sanctuary (n = 36). Our findings indicate that chimpanzees may not be irreversibly affected by early social trauma, possibly owing to rehabilitation in stable social groups in a semi-natural environment. Beyond identifying sanctuaries as valuable rehabilitation centres for orphaned chimpanzees, this study demonstrates a remarkable social flexibility in one of our closest living relatives.
- Published
- 2023
12. Individual behavioral correlates of tail biting in pre-finishing piglets
- Author
-
Bagaria, Marc, Kuiper, Laura, Meijer, Ellen, Sterck, Elisabeth H M, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS Animal Welfare, Behaviour & Welfare, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS Human Animal Relations./One Welfare, Producció Animal, Benestar Animal, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS Animal Welfare, Behaviour & Welfare, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and AISS Human Animal Relations./One Welfare
- Subjects
General Veterinary ,tail docking ,sudden-forceful ,behavior ,tail biting ,pigs ,social ,two-stage - Abstract
IntroductionTail biting is a widespread problem in pig production systems and has a negative impact on both animal welfare and farm income. This explorative study aims to validate how tail biting is related to general behaviors at the individual level and explore whether these behaviors are related to a particular type of tail biting: two-stage, sudden-forceful, obsessive, or epidemic.MethodsThis research was conducted in a standard commercial setting where 89 tail-docked pre-finishing piglets divided into 8 groups were observed 4 days per week from 5 to 8 weeks of age. Each piglet was observed for a total of 160 min using continuous focal sampling. Ten individual behaviors were recorded based on the general behaviors expected to be linked to giving tail biting (PCA1), receiving tail biting (PCA2), and tail biting damage (PCA3). These PCAs were assembled and related to tail biting given, tail biting received, and tail biting lesions.ResultsTail biting did not lead to major damage on the piglets' tail at 8 weeks of age but was observed 420 times, where most of the individuals (72%) were categorized as “biters and victims.” When relating PCA1 with tail biting given, piglets that gave more tail biting showed more “active exploration.” When relating PCA2 with tail biting received, piglets receiving more tail biting were more “explored while active” and “attacked and explored.” When relating PCA2 with tail biting lesions, piglets presenting lesions showed more “agonism.” Surprisingly, tail biting lesions were not significantly related to PCA3. The relationship between explorative behaviors and tail biting indicates that the pre-damage stage of two-stage tail biting was the predominant tail biting type, while the damaging stage was likely incipient. The relationship between tail biting and aggression, as well as the minor tail lesions observed suggest that sudden-forceful tail biting was probably present even though it was rarely seen. Obsessive and epidemic tail biting were not observed.DiscussionThis study demonstrates that studying tail biting at the individual level helps to identify the type of tail biting present. This gives directions to farmers for applying appropriate measures to prevent the development of tail biting behavior in piglets.
- Published
- 2022
13. Personality and social environment predict cognitive performance in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)
- Author
-
Šlipogor, Vedrana, Graf, Christina, Massen, Jorg J M, Bugnyar, Thomas, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Male ,Behavior ,Multidisciplinary ,Behavior, Animal ,Animal ,Biological anthropology ,Callithrix ,Animal behaviour ,Social Environment ,Cognition ,Animals ,Callithrix/psychology ,Female ,Personality - Abstract
Consistent inter-individual variation in cognition has been increasingly explored in recent years in terms of its patterns, causes and consequences. One of its possible causes are consistent inter-individual differences in behaviour, also referred to as animal personalities, which are shaped by both the physical and the social environment. The latter is particularly relevant for group-living species like common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), apt learners that display substantial variation in both their personality and cognitive performance, yet no study to date has interlinked these with marmosets’ social environment. Here we investigated (i) consistency of learning speed, and (ii) whether the PCA-derived personality traits Exploration-Avoidance and Boldness-Shyness as well as the social environment (i.e., family group membership) are linked with marmosets’ speed of learning. We tested 22 individuals in series of personality and learning-focused cognitive tests, including simple motor tasks and discrimination learning tasks. We found that these marmosets showed significant inter-individual consistency in learning across the different tasks, and that females learned faster than males. Further, bolder individuals, and particularly those belonging to certain family groups, learned faster. These findings indicate that both personality and social environment affect learning speed in marmosets and could be important factors driving individual variation in cognition.
- Published
- 2022
14. Prosociality in a despotic society
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Cousin, Eythan, Pflüger, Lena S., Massen, Jorg J.M., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Cousin, Eythan, Pflüger, Lena S., and Massen, Jorg J.M.
- Published
- 2023
15. Conservation and monitoring of wildlife in logged tropical forests: A study in Western Equatorial Africa
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sterck, Liesbeth, Verweij, Pita, van Kuijk, Marijke, Maisels, Fiona, Zwerts, Joeri Alexander, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sterck, Liesbeth, Verweij, Pita, van Kuijk, Marijke, Maisels, Fiona, and Zwerts, Joeri Alexander
- Published
- 2023
16. Fat or fit: correlates, consequences and control of overweight in group-housed primates
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sterck, Liesbeth, Langermans, Jan, Zijlmans, Dian Gerdina Maria, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sterck, Liesbeth, Langermans, Jan, and Zijlmans, Dian Gerdina Maria
- Published
- 2023
17. Does tolerance allow bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task? A conceptual replication of Hare et al., 2007
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin, Sterck, Liesbeth, Nolte, Suska, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin, Sterck, Liesbeth, and Nolte, Suska
- Published
- 2023
18. Individual attractiveness preferences differentially modulate immediate and voluntary attention
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Roth, Tom, Samara, Iliana, Perea-Garcia, Juan Olvido, Kret, Mariska E., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Roth, Tom, Samara, Iliana, Perea-Garcia, Juan Olvido, and Kret, Mariska E.
- Published
- 2023
19. A novel methodological approach for group classification during fission of a semi-free-ranging group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Hammer, Roy, Stribos, Mathieu S., Boehm, Pia M., Pink, Katharina E., Herzele, Julia, Wallner, Bernard, Huffman, Michael A., Massen, Jorg J.M., Pflüger, Lena S., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Hammer, Roy, Stribos, Mathieu S., Boehm, Pia M., Pink, Katharina E., Herzele, Julia, Wallner, Bernard, Huffman, Michael A., Massen, Jorg J.M., and Pflüger, Lena S.
- Published
- 2023
20. A comparative study of mirror self-recognition in three corvid species
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, Szabó, Anita, Bugnyar, Thomas, Massen, Jorg J M, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Vanhooland, Lisa-Claire, Szabó, Anita, Bugnyar, Thomas, and Massen, Jorg J M
- Published
- 2023
21. Group-specific expressions of co-feeding tolerance in bonobos and chimpanzees preclude dichotomous species generalizations
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J.C., Staes, Nicky, Brooker, Jake S., Kordon, Stephanie, Nolte, Suska, Clay, Zanna, Eens, Marcel, Stevens, Jeroen M.G., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J.C., Staes, Nicky, Brooker, Jake S., Kordon, Stephanie, Nolte, Suska, Clay, Zanna, Eens, Marcel, and Stevens, Jeroen M.G.
- Published
- 2023
22. Chimpanzee communities differ in their inter- and intrasexual social relationships
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Rawlings, Bruce S, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Davila-Ross, Marina, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Rawlings, Bruce S, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, and Davila-Ross, Marina
- Published
- 2023
23. Food sharing with friends and acquaintances: A study in preschool boys and girls
- Author
-
Leerstoel Baar, Development and Treatment of Psychosocial Problems, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Hallers-Haalboom, Elizabeth T, Vermande, Marjolijn M, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Sterck, Elisabeth H M, Leerstoel Baar, Development and Treatment of Psychosocial Problems, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Hallers-Haalboom, Elizabeth T, Vermande, Marjolijn M, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, and Sterck, Elisabeth H M
- Published
- 2023
24. Biased cultural transmission of a social custom in chimpanzees
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Hoppitt, William, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, and Hoppitt, William
- Published
- 2023
25. Real‐time alerts from AI‐enabled camera traps using the Iridium satellite network: A case‐study in Gabon, Central Africa
- Author
-
Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ecology and Biodiversity, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Whytock, Robin, Suijten, Thijs, Deursen, Tim van, Świeżewski, Jędrzej, Mermiaghe, Hervé, Madamba, Nazaire, Mouckoumou, Narcisse, Zwerts, Joeri A., Pambo, Aurélie Flore Koumba, Bahaa‐el‐din, Laila, Brittain, Stephanie, Cardoso, Anabelle Williamson, Henschel, Philipp, Lehmann, David, Momboua, Brice Roxan, Makaga, Loïc, Orbell, Christopher, White, Lee J. T., Iponga, Donald Midoko, Abernethy, Katharine, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ecology and Biodiversity, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Whytock, Robin, Suijten, Thijs, Deursen, Tim van, Świeżewski, Jędrzej, Mermiaghe, Hervé, Madamba, Nazaire, Mouckoumou, Narcisse, Zwerts, Joeri A., Pambo, Aurélie Flore Koumba, Bahaa‐el‐din, Laila, Brittain, Stephanie, Cardoso, Anabelle Williamson, Henschel, Philipp, Lehmann, David, Momboua, Brice Roxan, Makaga, Loïc, Orbell, Christopher, White, Lee J. T., Iponga, Donald Midoko, and Abernethy, Katharine
- Published
- 2023
26. The evidence does not support long-term oxygenation as a functional explanation for the evolution of yawning
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Massen, Jorg J M, Gallup, Andrew C, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Massen, Jorg J M, and Gallup, Andrew C
- Published
- 2023
27. Hand preference predicts behavioral responses to threats in Barbary macaques
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Dijk, Eva S. J., Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Belli, Elena, Massen, Jorg J. M., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Dijk, Eva S. J., Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Belli, Elena, and Massen, Jorg J. M.
- Published
- 2023
28. Reference Intervals and Percentiles for Hematologic and Serum Biochemical Values in Captive Bred Rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Cynomolgus Macaques (Macaca fascicularis)
- Author
-
AISS Animal Welfare, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS – Animal Welfare, Bakker, Jaco, Maaskant, Annemiek, Wegman, Merel, Zijlmans, Dian G. M., Hage, Patrice, Langermans, Jan A. M., Remarque, Edmond J., AISS Animal Welfare, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS – Animal Welfare, Bakker, Jaco, Maaskant, Annemiek, Wegman, Merel, Zijlmans, Dian G. M., Hage, Patrice, Langermans, Jan A. M., and Remarque, Edmond J.
- Published
- 2023
29. Conservation and monitoring of wildlife in logged tropical forests: A study in Western Equatorial Africa
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ecology and Biodiversity, Sterck, Liesbeth, Verweij, Pita, van Kuijk, Marijke, Maisels, Fiona, Zwerts, Joeri Alexander, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ecology and Biodiversity, Sterck, Liesbeth, Verweij, Pita, van Kuijk, Marijke, Maisels, Fiona, and Zwerts, Joeri Alexander
- Published
- 2023
30. Does tolerance allow bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task? A conceptual replication of Hare et al., 2007
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Nolte, Suska, Sterck, Liesbeth, van Leeuwen, Edwin, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Nolte, Suska, Sterck, Liesbeth, and van Leeuwen, Edwin
- Published
- 2023
31. What animal cultures may beget: Comment on 'Blind alleys and fruitful pathways in the comparative study of cultural cognition' by Andrew Whiten
- Author
-
Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Goldsborough, Zoë, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, and Goldsborough, Zoë
- Published
- 2023
32. Chimpanzees communicate to coordinate a cultural practice
- Author
-
Sub Animal Ecology, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Goldsborough, Zoë, Schel, Anne Marijke, Van Leeuwen, Edwin J.C., Sub Animal Ecology, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Goldsborough, Zoë, Schel, Anne Marijke, and Van Leeuwen, Edwin J.C.
- Published
- 2023
33. Call combinations and compositional processing in wild chimpanzees
- Author
-
Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Leroux, Maël, Schel, Anne Marijke, Wilke, Claudia, Chandia, Bosco, Zuberbühler, Klaus, Slocombe, Katie E., Townsend, Simon W., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Leroux, Maël, Schel, Anne Marijke, Wilke, Claudia, Chandia, Bosco, Zuberbühler, Klaus, Slocombe, Katie E., and Townsend, Simon W.
- Published
- 2023
34. Intentional gestural communication amongst red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus)
- Author
-
Schel, Anne Marijke, Bono, Axelle, Aychet, Juliette, Pika, Simone, Lemasson, Alban, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Utrecht University [Utrecht], Ethologie animale et humaine (EthoS), Université de Caen Normandie (UNICAEN), Normandie Université (NU)-Normandie Université (NU)-Université de Rennes (UR)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Universität Osnabrück - Osnabrück University, and This study was funded by Fondation Fyssen to AS, the Dr. J. L. Dobberke Stichting voor Vergelijkende Psychologie of the Koninklijke Academie van Wetenschappen to AS, and a grant of the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (Grant agreement no. 772000) to SP.
- Subjects
Primates ,Gestures ,Ecology ,Cercocebus ,Evolution ,Manual ,Hominidae ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Animal Communication ,Monkey ,[SCCO]Cognitive science ,Behavior and Systematics ,Intentionality ,Animals ,Attention ,Flexibility ,Gestural communication ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Abstract
Apes, human’s closest living relatives, are renowned for their intentional and highly flexible use of gestural communication. In stark contrast, evidence for flexible and intentional gestural communication in monkeys is scarce. Here, we investigated the intentionality and flexibility of spontaneous gesture use in red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). We applied established methods used in ape gesture research to analyse whether the body acts produced by a total of 17 individuals living in three different groups in captivity qualified as intentionally produced gesture instances. Results showed that signallers showed all hallmarks of intentionality during the production of 20 out of a total of 21 different types of body acts. These were only produced in the presence of other individuals, and the monkeys showed audience checking, sensitivity to the attentional states of recipients, adjustment of signal modality, and response waiting relative to their production. Moreover, in case of communication failure, the monkeys showed goal persistence, and regarding the production contexts they showed some signs of means–ends dissociation. Therefore, these monkeys are capable of flexible and intentional gestural communication and use this to communicate with conspecifics. Our results corroborate recent findings showing that intentional gestural communication was already present in the monkey lineage of catarrhine primates. We discuss our results in light of the comparative approach towards human language evolution and highlight our finding that these monkeys also showed flexible and intentional use of four ‘free’ manual gesture types.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Water table depth modulates productivity and biomass across Amazonian forests
- Author
-
Sousa, Thaiane R., Schietti, Juliana, Ribeiro, Igor O., Emílio, Thaise, Fernández, Rafael Herrera, ter Steege, Hans, Castilho, Carolina V., Esquivel-Muelbert, Adriane, Baker, Timothy, Pontes-Lopes, Aline, Silva, Camila V.J., Silveira, Juliana M., Derroire, Géraldine, Castro, Wendeson, Mendoza, Abel Monteagudo, Ruschel, Ademir, Prieto, Adriana, Lima, Adriano José Nogueira, Rudas, Agustín, Araujo-Murakami, Alejandro, Gutierrez, Alexander Parada, Andrade, Ana, Roopsind, Anand, Manzatto, Angelo Gilberto, Di Fiore, Anthony, Torres-Lezama, Armando, Dourdain, Aurélie, Marimon, Beatriz, Marimon, Ben Hur, Burban, Benoit, van Ulft, Bert, Herault, Bruno, Quesada, Carlos, Mendoza, Casimiro, Stahl, Clement, Bonal, Damien, Galbraith, David, Neill, David, de Oliveira, Edmar A., Hase, Eduardo, Jimenez-Rojas, Eliana, Vilanova, Emilio, Arets, Eric, Berenguer, Erika, Alvarez-Davila, Esteban, Honorio Coronado, Eurídice N., Almeida, Everton, Coelho, Fernanda, Valverde, Fernando Cornejo, Elias, Fernando, Brown, Foster, Bongers, Frans, Arevalo, Freddy Ramirez, Lopez-Gonzalez, Gabriela, van der Heijden, Geertje, Aymard C., Gerardo A., Llampazo, Gerardo Flores, Pardo, Guido, Ramírez-Angulo, Hirma, do Amaral, Iêda Leão, Vieira, Ima Célia Guimarães, Huamantupa-Chuquimaco, Isau, Comiskey, James A., Singh, James, Espejo, Javier Silva, del Aguila-Pasquel, Jhon, Zwerts, Joeri Alexander, Talbot, Joey, Terborgh, John, Ferreira, Joice, Barroso, Jorcely G., Barlow, Jos, Camargo, José Luís, Stropp, Juliana, Peacock, Julie, Serrano, Julio, Melgaço, Karina, Ferreira, Leandro V., Blanc, Lilian, Poorter, Lourens, Gamarra, Luis Valenzuela, Aragão, Luiz, Arroyo, Luzmila, Silveira, Marcos, Peñuela-Mora, Maria Cristina, Vargas, Mario Percy Núñez, Toledo, Marisol, Disney, Mat, Réjou-Méchain, Maxime, Baisie, Michel, Kalamandeen, Michelle, Camacho, Nadir Pallqui, Cardozo, Nállarett Dávila, Silva, Natalino, Pitman, Nigel, Higuchi, Niro, Banki, Olaf, Loayza, Patricia Alvarez, Graça, Paulo M.L.A., Morandi, Paulo S., van der Meer, Peter J., van der Hout, Peter, Naisso, Pétrus, Camargo, Plínio Barbosa, Salomão, Rafael, Thomas, Raquel, Boot, Rene, Umetsu, Ricardo Keichi, da Costa Silva, Richarlly, Burnham, Robyn, Zagt, Roderick, Martinez, Rodolfo Vasquez, Brienen, Roel, Ribeiro, Sabina Cerruto, Lewis, Simon L., Vieira, Simone Aparecida, de Almeida Reis, Simone Matias, Fauset, Sophie, Laurance, Susan, Feldpausch, Ted, Erwin, Terry, Killeen, Timothy, Wortel, Verginia, Moscoso, Victor Chama, Vos, Vincent, Huasco, Walter Huaraca, Laurance, William, Malhi, Yadvinder, Magnusson, William E., Phillips, Oliver L., Costa, Flávia R.C., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Ecology and Biodiversity, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Ecology and Biodiversity, and University of St Andrews. School of Geography & Sustainable Development
- Subjects
forest dynamics ,Evolution ,Bos- en Landschapsecologie ,tropical ecology ,Behavior and Systematics ,groundwater ,SDG 13 - Climate Action ,Forest and Landscape Ecology ,Bosecologie en Bosbeheer ,Vegetatie ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,above-ground biomass ,MCC ,Global and Planetary Change ,GE ,Vegetation ,Ecology ,seasonality ,carbon ,DAS ,PE&RC ,AC ,Forest Ecology and Forest Management ,Vegetatie, Bos- en Landschapsecologie ,Vegetation, Forest and Landscape Ecology ,GE Environmental Sciences - Abstract
Funding: This work was part of the PhD thesis of the first author, developed at the Graduate Program in Ecology of the National Institute of Amazonian Research (INPA), with a fellowship funded by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior – Brasil (CAPES), Finance Code 001, (88887.141433/2017-00). The authors are also grateful for the financial and research support of the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq), Amazonas State Research Foundation (FAPEAM), the Newton Fund via the Natural Environment Research Council (NE/M022021/1 to O.L.P. and F.R.C.C.), PPBio Manaus, Centro de Estudos Integrados da Biodiversidade Amazônica and RAINFOR. We also thank Karina Melgaço, Aurora Levesley and Gabriela Lopez-Gonzalez for curating and managing ForestPlots.net. This was ForestPlots.net Project 26 led by Thaiane Sousa. This is publication number 832 of the Technical Series of the Biological Dynamics of Forest Fragments Project (BDFFP, INPA/STRI). Aim : Water availability is the major driver of tropical forest structure and dynamics. Most research has focused on the impacts of climatic water availability, whereas remarkably little is known about the influence of water table depth and excess soil water on forest processes. Nevertheless, given that plants take up water from the soil, the impacts of climatic water supply on plants are likely to be modulated by soil water conditions. Location : Lowland Amazonian forests. Time period : 1971–2019. Methods : We used 344 long-term inventory plots distributed across Amazonia to analyse the effects of long-term climatic and edaphic water supply on forest functioning. We modelled forest structure and dynamics as a function of climatic, soil-water and edaphic properties. Results : Water supplied by both precipitation and groundwater affects forest structure and dynamics, but in different ways. Forests with a shallow water table (depth
- Published
- 2022
36. Density dependence of daily activity in three ungulate species
- Author
-
Ramirez, Juan Ignacio, Zwerts, Joeri A., van Kuijk, Marijke, Iacobelli, Palma, Li, Xuqing, Herdoiza, Natalie, Jansen, Patrick A., Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Environmental Sciences, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ecology and Biodiversity, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Ecology and Biodiversity, Environmental Sciences, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
0106 biological sciences ,Ungulate ,hunting ,Evolution ,Zoology ,safety-in-numbers ,010603 evolutionary biology ,01 natural sciences ,Population density ,Intraspecific competition ,03 medical and health sciences ,landscape of fear ,Capreolus ,Wild boar ,Behavior and Systematics ,camera traps ,biology.animal ,safety‐in‐numbers ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,QH540-549.5 ,030304 developmental biology ,Nature and Landscape Conservation ,Original Research ,Ekologi ,0303 health sciences ,Fish and Wildlife Management ,biology ,Ecology ,behavior ,Population size ,food availability ,PE&RC ,biology.organism_classification ,Roe deer ,Vilt- och fiskeförvaltning ,Density dependence ,Wildlife Ecology and Conservation ,temperate forest - Abstract
Daily activity in herbivores reflects a balance between finding food and safety. The safety‐in‐numbers theory predicts that living in higher population densities increases safety, which should affect this balance. High‐density populations are thus expected to show a more even distribution of activity—that is, spread—and higher activity levels across the day. We tested these predictions for three ungulate species; red deer (Cervus elaphus), roe deer (Capreolus capreolus), and wild boar (Sus scrofa). We used camera traps to measure the level and spread of activity across ten forest sites at the Veluwe, the Netherlands, that widely range in ungulate density. Food availability and hunting levels were included as covariates. Daily activity was more evenly distributed when population density was higher for all three species. Both deer species showed relatively more feeding activity in broad daylight and wild boar during dusk. Activity level increased with population density only for wild boar. Food availability and hunting showed no correlation with activity patterns. These findings indicate that ungulate activity is to some degree density dependent. However, while these patterns might result from larger populations feeling safer as the safety‐in‐numbers theory states, we cannot rule out that they are the outcome of greater intraspecific competition for food, forcing animals to forage during suboptimal times of the day. Overall, this study demonstrates that wild ungulates adjust their activity spread and level based on their population size., Daily activity of three ungulate species is density dependent across ten temperate forest sites. Activity spread and level increases with ungulate population size. Suggesting that future research should account for population size as a driver of ungulate activity.
- Published
- 2021
37. External environmental conditions impact nocturnal activity levels in proboscis monkeys ( Nasalis larvatus ) living in Sabah, Malaysia
- Author
-
Kooros, Sophie J., Goossens, Benoit, Sterck, Elisabeth H.M., Kenderdine, Richard, Malim, Peter T., Ramirez Saldivar, Diana A., Stark, Danica J., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
nocturnal activity ,Male ,Ecology ,Evolution ,Presbytini ,Malaysia ,Temperature ,activity pattern ,diurnal primates ,proboscis monkeys ,Haplorhini ,Circadian Rhythm ,accelerometer ,masking effects ,Behavior and Systematics ,Animals ,Humans ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Moon ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Abstract
Recently, several diurnal nonhuman anthropoids have been identified displaying varying degrees of nocturnal activity, which can be influenced by activity “masking effects”—external events or conditions that suppress or trigger activity, temporarily altering normal activity patterns. Environmental masking characteristics include nocturnal temperature, rainfall, cloud cover, and moon brightness. Similarly, other ecological characteristics, including proximity to humans and predators and daytime activity, may also trigger or suppress nocturnal activity. Understanding the effects of external conditions on activity patterns is pertinent to effective species conservation. We investigated the presence of nocturnal activity and the influence of masking effects on the level of nocturnal activity displayed by wild proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus) in Sabah, Malaysian Borneo. Dual-axis accelerometers were attached by collar to six male proboscis monkeys from different one-male, multi-female groups to record activity continuously (165–401 days each). We measured the monkeys' nocturnal and diurnal activity levels and investigated the effects of seven potential masking effects. Nocturnal activity was much lower than diurnal activity. Still, proboscis monkeys did display varying levels of nocturnal activity. Generalized linear mixed models identified higher nocturnal activity in the study individuals during nights with cooler temperatures, higher rainfall, and after higher diurnal activity. These three masking effects affected nocturnal activity levels during the observation period that informed our model, although they did not predict nocturnal activity outside of this period. While the generalizability of these results remains uncertain, this study highlights the utility of accelerometers in identifying activity patterns and masking effects that create variability in these patterns.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Multigenerational Social Housing and Group-Rearing Enhance Female Reproductive Success in Captive Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)
- Author
-
Rox, Astrid, Waasdorp, Sophie, Sterck, Elisabeth H.M., Langermans, Jan A.M., Louwerse, Annet L., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Agricultural and Biological Sciences(all) ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,macaques ,Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) ,Biochemistry ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,animal welfare ,reproductive success ,housing ,rearing history ,Immunology and Microbiology(all) ,General Agricultural and Biological Sciences ,Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) - Abstract
To optimize costs and reproductive success, rhesus macaques in biomedical primate research facilities are often peer-reared. Older, dependent infants are typically removed from their natal group to enhance female reproduction. The minimal husbandry age-norm of infant removal is ten months. These practices deviate from species-specific behavior and may reduce welfare, suggesting a trade-off between female reproduction and welfare. However, the effect of breeding group type and rearing history on female reproductive success (i.e., birth rate; inter-birth interval (IBI); offspring survival) is unclear. This retrospective study investigated whether group type (i.e., peer groups versus multigenerational groups) and rearing history (i.e., peer- or hand-reared; group-reared with peer- or hand-reared mother; group-reared) affected female reproductive success in captive rhesus macaques. Data on female reproduction between 1996 and 2019 were collected at the Biomedical Primate Research Centre, Rijswijk. Birth rates were higher in multigenerational breeding groups than in peer groups. Moreover, group-reared females had higher offspring survival than peer- or hand-reared females. IBI was not affected by breeding group type or female rearing history. However, females in both peer and multigenerational breeding groups typically conceived earlier after giving birth than the husbandry infant removal age-norm of ten months. Thus, infant removal at an age of ten months does not enhance a female’s reproduction. Altogether, female reproduction and non-human primate welfare can simultaneously be optimized through multigenerational breeding groups and group-rearing.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Overweight Management through Mild Caloric Restriction in Multigenerational Long-Tailed Macaque Breeding Groups
- Author
-
Zijlmans, Dian G. M., Maaskant, Annemiek, Louwerse, Annet L., Sterck, Elisabeth H. M., Langermans, Jan A. M., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
reproduction ,dietary restriction ,nutrition ,welfare ,obesity ,group-living ,General Veterinary - Abstract
Caloric restriction (CR) is an effective method to reduce overweight in captive non-human primates (NHPs). CR has been applied to individually- and pair-housed NHPs, but whether applying CR can be effective and safe in group-housed NHPs has not yet been assessed. This study investigates the effect of mild (20%) CR on adult overweight and biochemical parameters, immature growth, veterinary consultations, and reproductive success in multigenerational long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis) breeding groups. Data were derived from anthropometric measurements and blood samples during yearly health checks, complemented with retrospective data on veterinary consultations and reproductive success. Adult body measures decreased after CR, with heavier individuals and females losing more weight compared to leaner individuals and males. CR lowered cholesterol levels in adults but had no overall effect on other biochemical parameters. Yet, biochemical parameters of individuals with high baseline values were reduced more compared to individuals with low baseline values. Immature growth, veterinary consultations and reproductive success were not influenced by CR. Thus, CR targeted the right individuals, i.e., overweight adults, and had no adverse effects on the variables examined in this study. This implies that mild CR can be a valuable overweight management strategy in group-housed NHPs.
- Published
- 2022
40. Adjustment in the point-following behaviour of free-ranging dogs - roles of social petting and informative-deceptive nature of cues
- Author
-
Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Bhadra, Anindita, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Evolution ,Behavioural sciences ,Context (language use) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Dogs ,Reward ,Behavior and Systematics ,Interspecific communication ,Socio-cognitive skills ,Animals ,Social Behavior ,Human social petting ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Gestures ,Point (typography) ,Free ranging ,Ecology ,Mechanism (biology) ,Point-following ,Psychological research ,Recognition, Psychology ,Social cue ,Cues ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
Animals of different taxa can read and respond to various human communicative signals. Such a mechanism facilitates animals to acquire social information and helps them react in a context-dependent manner. Dogs have garnered extensive attention owing to their socio-cognitive skills and remarkable sensitivity to human social cues. For example, dogs readily respond to different human pointing gestures to locate hidden food rewards. However, a general inclination towards testing highly socialized pet dogs has resulted in a dearth of information on other sub-populations of dogs. Free-ranging dogs are one of the least socialized dog populations yet exhibit point-following behaviour flexibly. As a consequence of frequent negative interspecific interactions, they are typically wary of unfamiliar humans; thus, contextual recognition of human actions is paramount for these dogs to avoid potential conflict. However, the mechanisms influencing their point-following behaviour remain unidentified. We asked to what extent the informative-deceptive nature of cues and positive human interactions influence the interspecific communicative behaviour of these minimally socialized dogs. Using a point-following experiment with a 2 × 2 design, we focused on adult free-ranging dogs' behavioural adjustments. Dogs were randomly divided into two groups, with only one receiving brief social petting. Further, informative and deceptive cues were given to separate subsets within each group. Our findings suggest that brief social petting strongly affects the likelihood of free-ranging dogs' point-following tendencies. Dogs who received petting followed the pointing cues regardless of their informative or deceptive nature, whereas dogs who did not receive petting discriminated between informative and deceptive pointing. This study highlights the contribution of positive human interaction and informative-deceptive quality of cues in modulating the behavioural responses of free-ranging dogs in an interspecific communicative context.
- Published
- 2022
41. No Evidence for Contagious Yawning in Juvenile Ravens (Corvus corax): An Observational Study
- Author
-
Gallup, Andrew C, Schild, Anja B, Ühlein, Markus A, Bugnyar, Thomas, Massen, Jorg J M, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
collective behavior ,General Veterinary ,comparative cognition ,state change ,Animal Science and Zoology ,motor synchrony ,social behavior - Abstract
The overt and reflexive matching of behaviors among conspecifics has been observed in a growing number of social vertebrates, including avian species. In general, behavioral contagion—such as the spread of yawning—may serve important functions in group synchronization and vigilance behavior. Here, we performed an exploratory study to investigate yawn contagion among 10 captive juvenile ravens (Corvus corax), across two groups. Using observational methods, we also examined the contagiousness of three other distinct behaviors: stretching, scratching, and shaking. A total of 44 20 min observations were made across both groups, including 28 in the morning and 16 in the afternoon. The time and occurrence of all the behaviors from each bird were coded, and the temporal pattern of each behavior across both groups was then analyzed to assess the degree of social contagion. Overall, we found no evidence for contagious yawning, stretching, scratching, or shaking. However, yawns were relatively infrequent per observation (0.052 ± 0.076 yawns/bird) and thus experimental methods should be used to support this finding.
- Published
- 2022
42. Personality as a Predictor of Time-Activity Budget in Lion-Tailed Macaques (Macaca silenus)
- Author
-
Kluiver, Charlotte E, de Jong, Jolanda A, Massen, Jorg J M, Bhattacharjee, Debottam, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
sociability ,General Veterinary ,individual variation ,activity ,food‐related behaviour ,resting ,Animal Science and Zoology ,persistence ,affiliation ,anxiety ,food-related behaviour ,veterinary(all) - Abstract
Time-activity budget, i.e., how a population or an individual divides their day into various behaviours and activities, is an important ecological aspect. Existing research primarily focused on group-level time-activity budgets, while individual variations have only been reported recently. However, little is known about how consistent inter-individual differences or personalities influence time-activity budgets. We examined the personalities of lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) and investigated their influence on individual time-activity budgets. The resulting personality traits, namely persistence, sociability, affiliation, and anxiety, were used to predict the three broad categories of the time-activity budget—food-related, active, and resting behaviours. We found that persistence and sociability positively predicted the time spent being active. Food-related behaviours were positively predicted by persistence, while anxiety was found to influence them negatively. The time spent resting was negatively predicted by persistence. We did not find an effect of affiliation on the time-activity budgets. We discuss these findings in light of the ecology of lion-tailed macaques. Our study highlights the importance of a novel approach that uses animal personality traits as predictors of individual time-activity budgets and offers insights regarding the use of personality assessments in conservation and welfare activities.
- Published
- 2022
43. Tolerant and intolerant macaques differ in the context specificity of their calls and how they 'comment' on the interactions of others
- Author
-
Rebout, Nancy, Marco, Arianna De, Sanna, Andrea, Micheletta, Jérôme, Lone, Jean-Christophe, Berg, Reinier F. van den, Sterck, Elisabeth H. M., Langermans, Jan A. M., Thierry, Bernard, Lemasson, Alban, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Fondazione Ethoikos, Physiologie de la reproduction et des comportements [Nouzilly] (PRC), Institut Français du Cheval et de l'Equitation [Saumur] (IFCE)-Université de Tours (UT)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), University of Portsmouth, Universiteit Utrecht, Ethologie animale et humaine (EthoS), Université de Caen Normandie (UNICAEN), Normandie Université (NU)-Normandie Université (NU)-Université de Rennes (UR)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Ministère de l'Education nationale, de l’Enseignement supérieur et de la Recherche (M.E.N.E.S.R.), The Fondazione Ethoikos, Fondation des Treilles, Departement d'Ecologie, Physiologie et Ethologie (CNRS IPHC), The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, French-Dutch Network, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, AISS LAS/3'R Centre ULS, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Primates ,Ecology ,Evolution ,Communication ,[SDV.BA]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Animal biology ,Uncertainty ,Social complexity ,Acoustics ,[SCCO]Cognitive science ,Behavior and Systematics ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Flexibility ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Abstract
International audience; The 'social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity' posits that living in a complex social system requires complex communication skills. Since the complexity of a system can be measured by the amount of uncertainty it produces, we tested this hypothesis by studying species of macaque that differ in social tolerance and uncertainty of social interactions. We studied vocal communication in groups of macaques belonging to four species: Japanese and rhesus macaques, which are characterized by low levels of social tolerance and low uncertainty in the outcome of social interactions, and Tonkean and crested macaques, which display high levels of tolerance and uncertainty in interactions. We recorded the vocalizations emitted by adult females in agonistic, affiliative and neutral contexts. We measured call duration, entropy and time and frequency energy quantiles and processed these variables using cluster analyses and permutational multivariate analyses of variance. We found that tolerant macaques had a weaker relationship between the acoustic structure of calls and their context of emission compared to intolerant macaques. The study of 'commenting calls', i.e. calls made by individuals attending interactions between groupmates, also showed that their acoustic structure was more differentiated from other calls in tolerant Tonkean and crested macaques than in intolerant rhesus macaques. The flexibility of vocal production therefore appears to be correlated with the level of uncertainty of social interactions. Species with more complex social interactions were also those with higher degree of freedom in the association between acoustic structure and social context, which supports the social complexity hypothesis. Significance statement Is there a relationship between the complexity of social systems and the complexity of communication skills? Animals living in complex social environments are expected to use a wide variety of messages related to different goals and contexts. The complexity of a system can be assessed by the amount of uncertainty it can produce. We investigated the complexity of vocal communication by comparing two species of macaque displaying low uncertainty in their social interactions, with two other macaque species displaying high levels of uncertainty in their interactions. The comparison showed that call flexibility was related to uncertainty levels. Species with higher levels of uncertainty had weaker associations between acoustic structure and social context. These results support the hypothesis of a link between social system complexity and communication complexity, which has important implications for our understanding of the evolution of social and communication systems.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. A novel methodological approach for group classification during fission of a semi‐free‐ranging group of Japanese macaques ( Macaca fuscata )
- Author
-
Hammer, Roy, Stribos, Mathieu S., Boehm, Pia M., Pink, Katharina E., Herzele, Julia, Wallner, Bernard, Huffman, Michael A., Massen, Jorg J.M., Pflüger, Lena S., Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
semi-free-ranging ,Ecology ,Behavior and Systematics ,Evolution ,group membership ,social dynamics ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,group fission ,Macaca fuscata - Abstract
The self-initiated split of a social group, known as fission, is a challenge faced by many group-living animals. The study of group fission and the social restructuring process in real time provides insights into the mechanism of this biologically important process. Previous studies on fission in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) assigned individuals to newly reorganized groups mainly using behavioral observations and group attendance records based on periods before or after fission itself. Here, we present a novel framework for group classification during the process of fission that uses quantifiable behavioral variables and statistical analyses. The framework was tested on a group fission process at Affenberg Landskron (Austria), a park that housed around 160 semi-free-ranging Japanese macaques. The behavioral data were collected for 26 days during fission. We analyzed three behavioral developments recurrent in fissions in Japanese macaques, that is, independence of behavior, participation in group movements, and separation of nomadic ranges. These analyses were combined to assign individuals to different groups. Our study resulted in one main group (N = 33), one subgroup (N = 36) and 56 individuals whose group membership was still undefined. The demographic characteristics of these newly formed groups were comparable with those of fissioned groups in wild populations. Furthermore, we found that these newly forming groups showed early social dynamics of fission five months before group level movements, that is: grouping based on spatial proximity and spatial withdrawal of the subgroup to the periphery. These results underline the validity of our novel framework to study social dynamics in Japanese macaques during the process of fission. It represents an important addition to existing methods, and we recommend testing its scope in other primate societies.
- Published
- 2023
45. Does tolerance allow bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task? A conceptual replication of Hare et al ., 2007
- Author
-
van Leeuwen, Edwin, Sterck, Liesbeth, Nolte, Suska, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
replication ,chimpanzees ,Multidisciplinary ,cooperation ,co-feeding ,social tolerance ,bonobos - Abstract
Across various taxa, social tolerance is thought to facilitate cooperation, and many species are treated as having species-specific patterns of social tolerance. Yet studies that assess wild and captive bonobos and chimpanzees result in contrasting findings. By replicating a cornerstone experimental study on tolerance and cooperation in bonobos and chimpanzees (Hare et al . 2007 Cur. Biol. 17 , 619–623 ( doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.040 )), we aim to further our understanding of current discrepant findings. We tested bonobos and chimpanzees housed at the same facility in a co-feeding and cooperation task. Food was placed on dishes located on both ends or in the middle of a platform. In the co-feeding task, the tray was simply made available to the ape duos, whereas in the cooperation task the apes had to simultaneously pull at both ends of a rope attached to the platform to retrieve the food. By contrast to the published findings, bonobos and chimpanzees co-fed to a similar degree, indicating a similar level of tolerance. However, bonobos cooperated more than chimpanzees when the food was monopolizable, which replicates the original study. Our findings call into question the interpretation that at the species level bonobos cooperate to a higher degree because they are inherently more tolerant.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Chimpanzees organize their social relationships like humans
- Author
-
Escribano, Diego, Doldán-Martelli, Victoria, Cronin, Katherine A, Haun, Daniel B M, van Leeuwen, Edwin J C, Cuesta, José A, Sánchez, Angel, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (España), Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Antropología ,Multidisciplinary ,Pan troglodytes ,Matemáticas ,Reciprocity ,Emotions ,Complex networks ,Applied mathematics ,Grooming ,Pan troglodytes/psychology ,Size ,Animals ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,Networks ,General ,Social Behavior ,Sociología - Abstract
Human relationships are structured in a set of layers, ordered from higher (intimate relationships) to lower (acquaintances) emotional and cognitive intensity. This structure arises from the limits of our cognitive capacity and the different amounts of resources required by different relationships. However, it is unknown whether nonhuman primate species organize their affiliative relationships following the same pattern. We here show that the time chimpanzees devote to grooming other individuals is well described by the same model used for human relationships, supporting the existence of similar social signatures for both humans and chimpanzees. Furthermore, the relationship structure depends on group size as predicted by the model, the proportion of high-intensity connections being larger for smaller groups. We are thankful to Robin Dunbar and José L. Molina for discussions on our results. We also thank the Chimfunshi staff and the Zambia Wildlife Authority. DE, VD-M, JAC and AS acknowledge support from project BASIC (PGC2018-098186-B-I00) funded by MCIN/AEI/ 10.13039/501100011033 and by ERDF A way of making Europe. EJCvL acknowledges support from the Flanders Research Foundation (FWO) in the capacity of a postdoctoral fellowship. DBMH is supported by the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Dominance in a socially dynamic setting: hierarchical structure and conflict dynamics in ravens' foraging groups
- Author
-
Boucherie, Palmyre H., Gallego-Abenza, Mario, Massen, Jorg J.M., Bugnyar, Thomas, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Crows ,Male ,cognition ,fission–fusion dynamic ,Agricultural and Biological Sciences(all) ,Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) ,dominance structure ,Biochemistry ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,elo-rating ,Aggression ,Social Dominance ,Animals ,Humans ,Female ,General Agricultural and Biological Sciences ,Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) - Abstract
Dominance hierarchies typically emerge in systems where group members regularly encounter and compete for resources. In birds, the ‘open’ and dynamic structure of foraging groups may prevent the emergence of structured hierarchies, although this assumption have hardly been tested. We report on agonistic data for ravensCorvus corax, collected over two 18-month periods for 183 marked individuals of a wild (fluid) population and 51 birds from six captive (stable) groups. We show that the dominance structure (steep and transitive) in wild foraging groups is strikingly similar to that found in captivity. In the wild, we found that higher ranks are mainly occupied by males, older and more aggressive individuals that also tend to receive fewer aggressions. Exploring the mechanisms sustaining the wild dominance structure, we confirmed that males are more aggressive than females and, with age, tend to receive fewer aggressions than females. Males that are about to leave the foraging groups for some months are less aggressive than newcomers or locals, while newcomers are specifically targeted by aggressions in their first year (as juveniles). Taken together, our results indicate that the socially dynamic conditions ravens face during foraging do not hinder, but provide opportunities for, using (advanced) social cognition.This article is part of the theme issue ‘The centennial of the pecking order: current state and future prospects for the study of dominance hierarchies’.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. People that score high on psychopathic traits are less likely to yawn contagiously
- Author
-
Gallup, Andrew C, Kret, Mariska E, Eldakar, Omar Tonsi, Folz, Julia, Massen, Jorg J M, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Adult ,Data Analysis ,Male ,Science ,Emotions ,Global Health ,Article ,Young Adult ,Quantitative Trait, Heritable ,Human behaviour ,Odds Ratio ,Psychology ,Humans ,Geography, Medical ,Social Behavior ,General ,Emotion ,Multidisciplinary ,Mental Disorders ,Social behaviour ,Population Surveillance ,Medicine ,Female ,Yawning ,Disease Susceptibility ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Considerable variation exists in the contagiousness of yawning, and numerous studies have been conducted to investigate the proximate mechanisms involved in this response. Yet, findings within the psychological literature are mixed, with many studies conducted on relatively small and homogeneous samples. Here, we aimed to replicate and extend upon research suggesting a negative relationship between psychopathic traits and yawn contagion in community samples. In the largest study of contagious yawning to date (N = 458), which included both university students and community members from across 50 nationalities, participants completed an online study in which they self-reported on their yawn contagion to a video stimulus and completed four measures of psychopathy: the primary and secondary psychopathy scales from the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LSRPS), the psychopathy construct from the Dirty Dozen, and the Psychopathic Personality Traits Scale (PPTS). Results support previous findings in that participants that yawned contagiously tended to score lower on the combined and primary measures of psychopathy. That said, tiredness was the strongest predictor across all models. These findings align with functional accounts of spontaneous and contagious yawning and a generalized impairment in overall patterns of behavioral contagion and biobehavioral synchrony among people high in psychopathic traits.
- Published
- 2021
49. Brain size and neuron numbers drive differences in yawn duration across mammals and birds
- Author
-
Massen, Jorg J.M., Hartlieb, Margarita, Martin, Jordan S., Leitgeb, Elisabeth B., Hockl, Jasmin, Kocourek, Martin, Olkowicz, Seweryn, Zhang, Yicheng, Osadnik, Christin, Verkleij, Jorrit W., Bugnyar, Thomas, Němec, Pavel, Gallup, Andrew C., Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, University of Zurich, Massen, Jorg J M, Gallup, Andrew C, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, and Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
Behavioural ecology ,QH301-705.5 ,Zoology ,Medicine (miscellaneous) ,610 Medicine & health ,1100 General Agricultural and Biological Sciences ,Biology ,Biochemistry ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,Article ,Yawn ,Birds ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,1300 General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,medicine ,Premovement neuronal activity ,Animals ,Biology (General) ,030304 developmental biology ,Mammals ,Neurons ,0303 health sciences ,Agricultural and Biological Sciences(all) ,Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) ,Brain Mass ,Brain ,2701 Medicine (miscellaneous) ,Organ Size ,biology.organism_classification ,Phylogenetics ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Brain size ,11294 Institute of Evolutionary Medicine ,Neuronal physiology ,Amniote ,Yawning ,Neuron ,Allometry ,medicine.symptom ,Adaptation ,General Agricultural and Biological Sciences ,Biologie ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) - Abstract
Recent studies indicate that yawning evolved as a brain cooling mechanism. Given that larger brains have greater thermolytic needs and brain temperature is determined in part by heat production from neuronal activity, it was hypothesized that animals with larger brains and more neurons would yawn longer to produce comparable cooling effects. To test this, we performed the largest study on yawning ever conducted, analyzing 1291 yawns from 101 species (55 mammals; 46 birds). Phylogenetically controlled analyses revealed robust positive correlations between yawn duration and (1) brain mass, (2) total neuron number, and (3) cortical/pallial neuron number in both mammals and birds, which cannot be attributed solely to allometric scaling rules. These relationships were similar across clades, though mammals exhibited considerably longer yawns than birds of comparable brain and body mass. These findings provide further evidence suggesting that yawning is a thermoregulatory adaptation that has been conserved across amniote evolution., Massen, Hartlieb, Martin et al. study the duration of yawns across mammals and birds to test the brain cooling hypothesis. Consistent with this hypothesis, their findings indicate that brain mass and neuron numbers influence yawn duration, and that mammals yawn longer than birds with similar brain and body masses.
- Published
- 2021
50. Methods for wildlife monitoring in tropical forests: Comparing human observations, camera traps, and passive acoustic sensors
- Author
-
Zwerts, Joeri A., Stephenson, P. J., Maisels, Fiona, Rowcliffe, Marcus, Astaras, Christos, Jansen, Patrick A., Waarde, Jaap, Sterck, Liesbeth E. H. M., Verweij, Pita A., Bruce, Tom, Brittain, Stephanie, Kuijk, Marijke, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Energy System Analysis, Ecology and Biodiversity, Energy and Resources, Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Sub Animal Behaviour and Cognition, Energy System Analysis, Ecology and Biodiversity, Energy and Resources, and Animal Behaviour and Cognition
- Subjects
evidence‐based conservation ,Passive acoustic monitoring ,Ecology ,business.industry ,Environmental resource management ,automated classification ,Wildlife ,General. Including nature conservation, geographical distribution ,QH1-199.5 ,PE&RC ,wildlife monitoring methods ,passive acoustic monitoring ,camera trapping ,Evidence-based conservation ,Wildlife Ecology and Conservation ,wildlife conservation ,Automated classification ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Environmental science ,Life Science ,evidence-based conservation ,business ,QH540-549.5 ,General Environmental Science ,Wildlife conservation - Abstract
Wildlife monitoring is essential for conservation science and data‐driven decision‐making. Tropical forests pose a particularly challenging environment for monitoring wildlife due to the dense vegetation, and diverse and cryptic species with relatively low abundances. The most commonly used monitoring methods in tropical forests are observations made by humans (visual or acoustic), camera traps, or passive acoustic sensors. These methods come with trade‐offs in terms of species coverage, accuracy and precision of population metrics, available technical expertise, and costs. Yet, there are no reviews that compare the characteristics of these methods in detail. Here, we comprehensively review the advantages and limitations of the three mentioned methods, by asking four key questions that are always important in relation to wildlife monitoring: (1) What are the target species?; (2) Which population metrics are desirable and attainable?; (3) What expertise, tools, and effort are required for species identification?; and (4) Which financial and human resources are required for data collection and processing? Given the diversity of monitoring objectives and circumstances, we do not aim to conclusively prescribe particular methods for all situations. Neither do we claim that any one method is superior to others. Rather, our review aims to support scientists and conservation practitioners in understanding the options and criteria that must be considered in choosing the appropriate method, given the objectives of their wildlife monitoring efforts and resources available. We focus on tropical forests because of their high conservation priority, although the information put forward is also relevant for other biomes.
- Published
- 2021
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.