252 results on '"Psychological and Cognitive Sciences"'
Search Results
2. Correction to Supporting Information for Bogaert et al., Male homosexuality and maternal immune responsivity to the Y-linked protein NLGN4Y
- Subjects
Immunology and Inflammation ,NLGN4Y ,sexual orientation ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Sciences ,homosexuality ,Biological Sciences ,fraternal birth order ,maternal immune hypothesis - Abstract
Significance Gay men have, on average, a greater number of older brothers than do heterosexual men, a well-known finding within sexual science. This finding has been termed the fraternal birth order effect. Strong scientific interest in sexual orientation exists because it is a fundamental human characteristic, and because its origins are often the focal point of considerable social controversy. Our study is a major advance in understanding the origins of sexual orientation in men by providing support for a theorized but previously unexamined biological mechanism—a maternal immune response to a protein important in male fetal brain development—and by beginning to explain one of the most reliable correlates of male homosexuality: older brothers., We conducted a direct test of an immunological explanation of the finding that gay men have a greater number of older brothers than do heterosexual men. This explanation posits that some mothers develop antibodies against a Y-linked protein important in male brain development, and that this effect becomes increasingly likely with each male gestation, altering brain structures underlying sexual orientation in their later-born sons. Immune assays targeting two Y-linked proteins important in brain development—protocadherin 11 Y-linked (PCDH11Y) and neuroligin 4 Y-linked (NLGN4Y; isoforms 1 and 2)—were developed. Plasma from mothers of sons, about half of whom had a gay son, along with additional controls (women with no sons, men) was analyzed for male protein-specific antibodies. Results indicated women had significantly higher anti-NLGN4Y levels than men. In addition, after statistically controlling for number of pregnancies, mothers of gay sons, particularly those with older brothers, had significantly higher anti-NLGN4Y levels than did the control samples of women, including mothers of heterosexual sons. The results suggest an association between a maternal immune response to NLGN4Y and subsequent sexual orientation in male offspring.
- Published
- 2022
3. No reason to expect large and consistent effects of nudge interventions
- Author
-
Barnabas Szaszi, anthony higney, Aaron Charlton, andrew gelman, Ignazio Ziano, Balazs Aczel, Daniel G Goldstein, David Scott Yeager, and Elizabeth Tipton
- Subjects
nudge ,meta-analysis ,behavioral insights ,behavior change ,Multidisciplinary ,choice architecture ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Humans ,Social Sciences ,Choice Behavior - Abstract
Significance Changing individuals’ behavior is key to tackling some of today’s most pressing societal challenges such as the COVID-19 pandemic or climate change. Choice architecture interventions aim to nudge people toward personally and socially desirable behavior through the design of choice environments. Although increasingly popular, little is known about the overall effectiveness of choice architecture interventions and the conditions under which they facilitate behavior change. Here we quantitatively review over a decade of research, showing that choice architecture interventions successfully promote behavior change across key behavioral domains, populations, and locations. Our findings offer insights into the effects of choice architecture and provide guidelines for behaviorally informed policy making., Over the past decade, choice architecture interventions or so-called nudges have received widespread attention from both researchers and policy makers. Built on insights from the behavioral sciences, this class of behavioral interventions focuses on the design of choice environments that facilitate personally and socially desirable decisions without restricting people in their freedom of choice. Drawing on more than 200 studies reporting over 450 effect sizes (n = 2,149,683), we present a comprehensive analysis of the effectiveness of choice architecture interventions across techniques, behavioral domains, and contextual study characteristics. Our results show that choice architecture interventions overall promote behavior change with a small to medium effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.45 (95% CI [0.39, 0.52]). In addition, we find that the effectiveness of choice architecture interventions varies significantly as a function of technique and domain. Across behavioral domains, interventions that target the organization and structure of choice alternatives (decision structure) consistently outperform interventions that focus on the description of alternatives (decision information) or the reinforcement of behavioral intentions (decision assistance). Food choices are particularly responsive to choice architecture interventions, with effect sizes up to 2.5 times larger than those in other behavioral domains. Overall, choice architecture interventions affect behavior relatively independently of contextual study characteristics such as the geographical location or the target population of the intervention. Our analysis further reveals a moderate publication bias toward positive results in the literature. We end with a discussion of the implications of our findings for theory and behaviorally informed policy making.
- Published
- 2022
4. The interrelationship between the face and vocal tract configuration during audiovisual speech
- Author
-
Alan Johnston, Jeremy I. Skipper, and Chris Scholes
- Subjects
Adult ,Speech production ,Speech perception ,genetic structures ,Computer science ,speech ,Speech recognition ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Fidelity ,Vocal Cords ,Signal ,Speech Acoustics ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,otorhinolaryngologic diseases ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,audiovisual ,media_common ,PCA ,Principal Component Analysis ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Biological Sciences ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation ,Variation (linguistics) ,Face ,Face (geometry) ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Speech Perception ,Visual Perception ,Joint (audio engineering) ,psychological phenomena and processes ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Vocal tract - Abstract
Significance Speech perception is improved when we are able to see the person who is speaking, but how visual speech cues are used to improve speech perception is currently unclear. Brain imaging has revealed that regions responsible for motor control are active during the perception of speech, opening up the possibility that visual cues are mapped onto an internal representation of the vocal tract. Here, we show that there is sufficient information in the configuration of the face to recover the vocal tract configuration and that the key areas responsible for driving the correspondence vary in accordance with the articulation required to form the acoustic signal at the appropriate point in a sentence., It is well established that speech perception is improved when we are able to see the speaker talking along with hearing their voice, especially when the speech is noisy. While we have a good understanding of where speech integration occurs in the brain, it is unclear how visual and auditory cues are combined to improve speech perception. One suggestion is that integration can occur as both visual and auditory cues arise from a common generator: the vocal tract. Here, we investigate whether facial and vocal tract movements are linked during speech production by comparing videos of the face and fast magnetic resonance (MR) image sequences of the vocal tract. The joint variation in the face and vocal tract was extracted using an application of principal components analysis (PCA), and we demonstrate that MR image sequences can be reconstructed with high fidelity using only the facial video and PCA. Reconstruction fidelity was significantly higher when images from the two sequences corresponded in time, and including implicit temporal information by combining contiguous frames also led to a significant increase in fidelity. A “Bubbles” technique was used to identify which areas of the face were important for recovering information about the vocal tract, and vice versa, on a frame-by-frame basis. Our data reveal that there is sufficient information in the face to recover vocal tract shape during speech. In addition, the facial and vocal tract regions that are important for reconstruction are those that are used to generate the acoustic speech signal.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. The plasticity of well-being: A training-based framework for the cultivation of human flourishing
- Author
-
Cortland J. Dahl, Richard J. Davidson, and Christine D. Wilson-Mendenhall
- Subjects
mindfulness ,Mindfulness ,Emotions ,Psychological intervention ,Social Sciences ,Affective neuroscience ,well-being ,insight ,meta-awareness ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,purpose ,Dimension (data warehouse) ,Cognitive science ,Multidisciplinary ,Teaching ,Field (Bourdieu) ,Flourishing ,Neurosciences ,Cognition ,Awareness ,Biological Sciences ,Mental Health ,Perspective ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Well-being ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Research indicates that core dimensions of psychological well-being can be cultivated through intentional mental training. Despite growing research in this area and an increasing number of interventions designed to improve psychological well-being, the field lacks a unifying framework that clarifies the dimensions of human flourishing that can be cultivated. Here, we integrate evidence from well-being research, cognitive and affective neuroscience, and clinical psychology to highlight four core dimensions of well-being—awareness, connection, insight, and purpose. We discuss the importance of each dimension for psychological well-being, identify mechanisms that underlie their cultivation, and present evidence of their neural and psychological plasticity. This synthesis highlights key insights, as well as important gaps, in the scientific understanding of well-being and how it may be cultivated, thus highlighting future research directions.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Correction for Matsumiya, Multiple representations of the body schema for the same body part
- Subjects
eye movements ,hand movements ,health services administration ,multisensory integration ,education ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,body representation ,Social Sciences ,natural sciences ,Biological Sciences ,motor actions - Abstract
Significance Accurate motor control depends on maps of the body in the brain, called the body schema. Disorders of the body schema cause motor deficits. Although we often execute actions with different motor systems such as the eye and hand, how the body schema operates during such actions is unknown. In this study, participants simultaneously directed eye and hand movements to the same body part. These two movements were found to be guided by different body maps. This finding demonstrates multiple motor system–specific representations of the body schema, suggesting that the choice of motor system toward one’s body can determine which of the brain’s body maps is observed. This may offer a new way to visualize patients’ body schema., Purposeful motor actions depend on the brain’s representation of the body, called the body schema, and disorders of the body schema have been reported to show motor deficits. The body schema has been assumed for almost a century to be a common body representation supporting all types of motor actions, and previous studies have considered only a single motor action. Although we often execute multiple motor actions, how the body schema operates during such actions is unknown. To address this issue, I developed a technique to measure the body schema during multiple motor actions. Participants made simultaneous eye and reach movements to the same location of 10 landmarks on their hand. By analyzing the internal configuration of the locations of these points for each of the eye and reach movements, I produced maps of the mental representation of hand shape. Despite these two movements being simultaneously directed to the same bodily location, the resulting hand map (i.e., a part of the body schema) was much more distorted for reach movements than for eye movements. Furthermore, the weighting of visual and proprioceptive bodily cues to build up this part of the body schema differed for each effector. These results demonstrate that the body schema is organized as multiple effector-specific body representations. I propose that the choice of effector toward one’s body can determine which body representation in the brain is observed and that this visualization approach may offer a new way to understand patients’ body schema.
- Published
- 2022
7. Instagram photos reveal predictive markers of depression.
- Author
-
Reece, Andrew and Danforth, Christopher
- Subjects
DIAGNOSIS of mental depression ,IMAGE color analysis ,MACHINE learning ,FACE perception ,COGNITIVE science ,PREDICTION models - Abstract
Using Instagram data from 166 individuals, we applied machine learning tools to successfully identify markers of depression. Statistical features were computationally extracted from 43,950 participant Instagram photos, using color analysis, metadata components, and algorithmic face detection. Resulting models outperformed general practitioners' average unassisted diagnostic success rate for depression. These results held even when the analysis was restricted to posts made before depressed individuals were first diagnosed. Human ratings of photo attributes (happy, sad, etc.) were weaker predictors of depression, and were uncorrelated with computationally-generated features. These results suggest new avenues for early screening and detection of mental illness. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Valuation of peers’ safe choices is associated with substance-naïveté in adolescents
- Author
-
Nina Lauharatanahirun, Mark A. Orloff, Pearl H. Chiu, Brooks King-Casas, Dongil Chung, Fralin Biomedical Research Institute, and Psychology
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,Substance-Related Disorders ,education ,Social Sciences ,substance use ,Choice Behavior ,decision making ,Developmental psychology ,Risk-Taking ,Functional neuroimaging ,SAFER ,Humans ,Peer influence ,Healthy Lifestyle ,Social influence ,Valuation (finance) ,peer influence ,Multidisciplinary ,decision-making ,Biological Sciences ,Quantitative model ,Social Perception ,Adolescent Behavior ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,adolescence ,Female ,Substance use ,Psychology ,social influence - Abstract
Significance During adolescence, substance use and other health-risk behaviors emerge, particularly among those who associate with peers engaging in such behaviors and less so for adolescents with less deviant peers. Here, we provide behavioral and neural evidence for a beneficial role of safer peers, rather than a detrimental influence of risky peers, in guiding adolescents’ choices and substance use. The extent to which adolescents value peers’ safe choices predicted substance-naïveté even after controlling for other factors associated with substance use, while valuation of peers’ risky choices was unrelated to substance use. Whereas previous studies have largely examined associations between negative peers and increased health-risk behaviors, our data support a significant role of positive social peers for favorably influencing health-risk behaviors., Social influences on decision-making are particularly pronounced during adolescence and have both protective and detrimental effects. To evaluate how responsiveness to social signals may be linked to substance use in adolescents, we used functional neuroimaging and a gambling task in which adolescents who have and have not used substances (substance-exposed and substance-naïve, respectively) made choices alone and after observing peers’ decisions. Using quantitative model-based analyses, we identify behavioral and neural evidence that observing others’ safe choices increases the subjective value and selection of safe options for substance-naïve relative to substance-exposed adolescents. Moreover, the effects of observing others’ risky choices do not vary by substance exposure. These results provide neurobehavioral evidence for a role of positive peers (here, those who make safer choices) in guiding adolescent real-world risky decision-making.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Adversarial vulnerabilities of human decision-making
- Author
-
Peter Dayan, Richard Nock, and Amir Dezfouli
- Subjects
reinforcement learning ,Computer science ,Decision Making ,Behavioural sciences ,010501 environmental sciences ,Machine learning ,computer.software_genre ,01 natural sciences ,Action selection ,Choice Behavior ,03 medical and health sciences ,Adversarial system ,Reward ,Reinforcement learning ,Humans ,Learning ,recurrent neural networks ,Computer Simulation ,030304 developmental biology ,0105 earth and related environmental sciences ,0303 health sciences ,Multidisciplinary ,Artificial neural network ,business.industry ,Behavioral pattern ,decision-making ,Adversary ,Biological Sciences ,Recurrent neural network ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Artificial intelligence ,Neural Networks, Computer ,business ,computer ,Reinforcement, Psychology - Abstract
Significance “What I cannot efficiently break, I cannot understand.” Understanding the vulnerabilities of human choice processes allows us to detect and potentially avoid adversarial attacks. We develop a general framework for creating adversaries for human decision-making. The framework is based on recent developments in deep reinforcement learning models and recurrent neural networks and can in principle be applied to any decision-making task and adversarial objective. We show the performance of the framework in three tasks involving choice, response inhibition, and social decision-making. In all of the cases the framework was successful in its adversarial attack. Furthermore, we show various ways to interpret the models to provide insights into the exploitability of human choice., Adversarial examples are carefully crafted input patterns that are surprisingly poorly classified by artificial and/or natural neural networks. Here we examine adversarial vulnerabilities in the processes responsible for learning and choice in humans. Building upon recent recurrent neural network models of choice processes, we propose a general framework for generating adversarial opponents that can shape the choices of individuals in particular decision-making tasks toward the behavioral patterns desired by the adversary. We show the efficacy of the framework through three experiments involving action selection, response inhibition, and social decision-making. We further investigate the strategy used by the adversary in order to gain insights into the vulnerabilities of human choice. The framework may find applications across behavioral sciences in helping detect and avoid flawed choice.
- Published
- 2020
10. 'You' speaks to me: Effects of generic-you in creating resonance between people and ideas
- Author
-
Ariana Orvell, Susan A. Gelman, and Ethan Kross
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Structure (mathematical logic) ,Adult ,Male ,050101 languages & linguistics ,Persuasion ,Multidisciplinary ,language ,persuasion ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Social Sciences ,emotion ,Linguistics ,050105 experimental psychology ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Multilevel Analysis ,Generic you ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Female ,Sociology ,Content (Freudian dream analysis) ,media_common - Abstract
Significance Feeling resonance in response to ideas is a pervasive human experience. Previous efforts to enhance resonance have focused on changing the content of a message. Here we identify a linguistic device—the generic use of the word “you” (e.g., “You live, you learn”)—that accomplishes the same goal. Using crowd-sourced data from the Amazon Kindle application, we found that generic-you was substantially more likely to appear in passages that people highlighted (vs. did not highlight) while reading. Moreover, we present the results of four experiments (n = 1,900), indicating that generic-you causally promoted resonance. These data reveal how a subtle linguistic shift can shape a pervasive human experience, promoting connection between people and ideas., Creating resonance between people and ideas is a central goal of communication. Historically, attempts to understand the factors that promote resonance have focused on altering the content of a message. Here we identify an additional route to evoking resonance that is embedded in the structure of language: the generic use of the word “you” (e.g., “You can’t understand someone until you’ve walked a mile in their shoes”). Using crowd-sourced data from the Amazon Kindle application, we demonstrate that passages that people highlighted—collectively, over a quarter of a million times—were substantially more likely to contain generic-you compared to yoked passages that they did not highlight. We also demonstrate in four experiments (n = 1,900) that ideas expressed with generic-you increased resonance. These findings illustrate how a subtle shift in language establishes a powerful sense of connection between people and ideas.
- Published
- 2020
11. Visual motion assists in social cognition
- Author
-
Michael S. A. Graziano and Arvid Guterstam
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Social Cognition ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Theory of Mind ,Motion (physics) ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Orientation (mental) ,Social cognition ,Theory of mind ,Perception ,motion ,Humans ,030304 developmental biology ,media_common ,0303 health sciences ,Multidisciplinary ,Subthreshold conduction ,Biological Sciences ,Middle Aged ,Object (philosophy) ,attention ,Facial Expression ,Action (philosophy) ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Psychology ,Facial Recognition ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Recent evidence suggests that our brains may generate subtle, fictitious motion signals streaming from other people to the objects of their attention. However, the functional significance of this internally-generated motion signal remains unclear. Here, we tested whether subthreshold motion processing plays a causal role in judging others’ attention. Participants viewed a display including faces, objects, and a subthreshold motion hidden in the background. Judgments of the attentional state of the faces were significantly altered by the motion signal. Control experiments indicated the effect was specific to judging attention. These findings suggest that a crucial aspect of social cognition, monitoring others’ attention, draws on useful but highly physically inaccurate models of social agents in the world, rooted in low-level perceptual mechanisms., Recent evidence suggests a link between visual motion processing and social cognition. When person A watches person B, the brain of A apparently generates a fictitious, subthreshold motion signal streaming from B to the object of B’s attention. These previous studies, being correlative, were unable to establish any functional role for the false motion signals. Here, we directly tested whether subthreshold motion processing plays a role in judging the attention of others. We asked, if we contaminate people’s visual input with a subthreshold motion signal streaming from an agent to an object, can we manipulate people’s judgments about that agent’s attention? Participants viewed a display including faces, objects, and a subthreshold motion hidden in the background. Participants’ judgments of the attentional state of the faces was significantly altered by the hidden motion signal. Faces from which subthreshold motion was streaming toward an object were judged as paying more attention to the object. Control experiments showed the effect was specific to the agent-to-object motion direction and to judging attention, not action or spatial orientation. These results suggest that when the brain models other minds, it uses a subthreshold motion signal, streaming from an individual to an object, to help represent attentional state. This type of social-cognitive model, tapping perceptual mechanisms that evolved to process physical events in the real world, may help to explain the extraordinary cultural persistence of beliefs in mind processes having physical manifestation. These findings, therefore, may have larger implications for human psychology and cultural belief.
- Published
- 2020
12. Dogmatism manifests in lowered information search under uncertainty
- Author
-
Max Rollwage, Stephen M. Fleming, Raymond J. Dolan, and Lion Schulz
- Subjects
computational modeling ,education.field_of_study ,Multidisciplinary ,Information seeking ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Political Sciences ,Metacognition ,Social Sciences ,Cognition ,Biological Sciences ,Politics ,Perceptual decision ,Perception ,Phenomenon ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,dogmatism ,information search ,politics ,education ,Psychology ,metacognition ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Significance Dogmatic individuals are reluctant to seek out new information to refine their views, often skewing political, scientific, and religious discourse in the process. The cognitive drivers of this reluctance are poorly understood. Here, we isolate an influence of uncertainty on information search using a low-level perceptual decision-making task. We show that people with dogmatic views are both less likely to seek information before committing to a decision and to use fluctuations in uncertainty to guide their search. Our results highlight a cognitive mechanism that may contribute to the formation of dogmatic worldviews., When knowledge is scarce, it is adaptive to seek further information to resolve uncertainty and obtain a more accurate worldview. Biases in such information-seeking behavior can contribute to the maintenance of inaccurate views. Here, we investigate whether predispositions for uncertainty-guided information seeking relate to individual differences in dogmatism, a phenomenon linked to entrenched beliefs in political, scientific, and religious discourse. We addressed this question in a perceptual decision-making task, allowing us to rule out motivational factors and isolate the role of uncertainty. In two independent general population samples (n = 370 and n = 364), we show that more dogmatic participants are less likely to seek out new information to refine an initial perceptual decision, leading to a reduction in overall belief accuracy despite similar initial decision performance. Trial-by-trial modeling revealed that dogmatic participants placed less reliance on internal signals of uncertainty (confidence) to guide information search, rendering them less likely to seek additional information to update beliefs derived from weak or uncertain initial evidence. Together, our results highlight a cognitive mechanism that may contribute to the formation of dogmatic worldviews.
- Published
- 2020
13. Neurotypical individuals fail to understand action vitality form in children with autism spectrum disorder
- Author
-
Luca Casartelli, Alessandra Federici, Lucia Fumagalli, Giacomo Rizzolatti, Monica Nicoli, Andrea Vitale, Luca Ronconi, Massimo Molteni, Corrado Sinigaglia, Ambra Cesareo, Casartelli, L., Federici, A., Fumagalli, L., Cesareo, A., Nicoli, M., Ronconi, L., Vitale, A., Molteni, M., Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C.
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Autism Spectrum Disorder ,Autism ,Social Interaction ,autism ,Vitality ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,Motor cognition ,mental disorders ,motor cognition ,medicine ,Humans ,Child ,Multidisciplinary ,Recognition, Psychology ,Cognition ,Biological Sciences ,medicine.disease ,Healthy Volunteers ,Social relation ,Comprehension ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,vitality form ,Vitality form ,Female ,Psychology ,Neurotypical - Abstract
Significance Action vitality forms are highly pervasive aspects of daily life and have been widely assumed to be critical for basic social interactions. Previous evidence indicates that ASD children express their own vitality forms in a way that is motorically dissimilar to TD children. Here we demonstrate that this motor dissimilarity prevents neurotypical adults from recognizing vitality forms, when observing ASD children acting gently or rudely. Although ASD children differentiate these vitality forms, neurotypical adults were remarkably inaccurate in identifying them. This indicates that difficulty in social interaction for ASD individuals should not be entirely ascribed to their lack of understanding others, as standardly assumed. The failure of neurotypical individuals to understand them plays a critical role too., Any defects of sociality in individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are standardly explained in terms of those individuals’ putative impairments in a variety of cognitive functions. Recently, however, the need for a bidirectional approach to social interaction has been emphasized. Such an approach highlights differences in basic ways of acting between ASD and neurotypical individuals which would prevent them from understanding each other. Here we pursue this approach by focusing on basic action features reflecting the agent’s mood and affective states. These are action features Stern named “vitality forms,” and which are widely assumed to substantiate core social interactions [D. N. Stern, The Interpersonal World of the Infant (1985); D. N. Stern, Forms of Vitality Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, Arts, Psychotherapy, and Development (2010)]. Previously we demonstrated that, although ASD and typically developing (TD) children alike differentiate vitality forms when performing actions, ASD children express them in a way that is motorically dissimilar to TD children. To assess whether this motor dissimilarity may have consequences for vitality form recognition, we asked neurotypical participants to identify the vitality form of different types of action performed by ASD or TD children. We found that participants exhibited remarkable inaccuracy in identifying ASD children’s vitality forms. Interestingly, their performance did not benefit from information feedback. This indicates that how people act matters for understanding others and for being understood by them. Because vitality forms pervade every aspect of daily life, our findings promise to open the way to a deeper comprehension of the bidirectional difficulties for both ASD and neurotypical individuals in interacting with one another.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Longitudinally adaptive assessment and instruction increase numerical skills of preschool children
- Author
-
Stephen W. Raudenbush, Janet Eisenband Sorkin, Cristina Carrazza, Susan C. Levine, Alana Foley, Marc W. Hernandez, Debbie Leslie, and Susan Goldin-Meadow
- Subjects
Male ,preschool instruction ,Control (management) ,education ,Social Sciences ,Affect (psychology) ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,050105 experimental psychology ,Education ,Intervention (counseling) ,Mathematical skill ,randomized control trials ,Mathematics education ,Humans ,Learning ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social inequality ,Verbal comprehension ,Students ,Adaptive assessment ,adaptive assessment ,Multidisciplinary ,social inequality ,Schools ,Teaching ,05 social sciences ,Mathematical Concepts ,mathematics education ,Aptitude Tests ,Child, Preschool ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Educational Measurement ,Psychology ,Spatial skills ,Comprehension ,050104 developmental & child psychology - Abstract
Significance Socioeconomic disparities in math proficiency are observable when children enter kindergarten, and these disparities persist through the school years. Research suggests that overall proficiency at kindergarten entry depends upon specific skills that all normally developing children age 3 to 5 y can learn. We therefore designed a procedure that enables teachers to assess the skills of each child and tailor instruction to child-specific levels of skill. The procedure is iterative: Assess, teach, reassess, and teach, with three assessments per school year. We found that children in classrooms randomly assigned to this procedure gained substantially more in their numerical proficiency than did children in control classrooms. The program did not delay growth in print literacy and increased verbal proficiency., Social inequality in mathematical skill is apparent at kindergarten entry and persists during elementary school. To level the playing field, we trained teachers to assess children’s numerical and spatial skills every 10 wk. Each assessment provided teachers with information about a child’s growth trajectory on each skill, information designed to help them evaluate their students' progress, reflect on past instruction, and strategize for the next phase of instruction. A key constraint is that teachers have limited time to assess individual students. To maximize the information provided by an assessment, we adapted the difficulty of each assessment based on each child’s age and accumulated evidence about the child’s skills. Children in classrooms of 24 trained teachers scored 0.29 SD higher on numerical skills at posttest than children in 25 randomly assigned control classrooms (P = 0.005). We observed no effect on spatial skills. The intervention also positively influenced children’s verbal comprehension skills (0.28 SD higher at posttest, P < 0.001), but did not affect their print-literacy skills. We consider the potential contribution of this approach, in combination with similar regimes of assessment and instruction in elementary schools, to the reduction of social inequality in numerical skill and discuss possible explanations for the absence of an effect on spatial skills.
- Published
- 2020
15. Goal-directed and stimulus-driven selection of internal representations
- Author
-
Freek van Ede, Anna C. Nobre, Alexander G. Board, and Cognitive Psychology
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Oculomotor system ,Memory-guided behavior ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Capture ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Memory performance ,050105 experimental psychology ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,Adaptation, Psychological ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Attention ,media_common ,Visual working memory ,Multidisciplinary ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,Biological Sciences ,Gaze ,Memory, Short-Term ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Internal memory ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Goals ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Everyday behavior relies on continuously selecting relevant information from the external environment as well as from internal representations in working memory. In describing the selection of external objects, a longstanding distinction is proposed between goal-directed (voluntary) and stimulus-driven (involuntary) sources of attentional selection. We voluntarily attend to things that are relevant to our goals, but our attention may also be captured involuntarily by salient events in the external world. Yet despite decades of research on how these two sources guide our perception (external selection), to date no study has examined whether and how these two sources jointly influence the selection of internal memory representations. With innovative experimental manipulations and behavioral markers of internal selection, we fill this important gap., Adaptive behavior relies on the selection of relevant sensory information from both the external environment and internal memory representations. In understanding external selection, a classic distinction is made between voluntary (goal-directed) and involuntary (stimulus-driven) guidance of attention. We have developed a task—the anti-retrocue task—to separate and examine voluntary and involuntary guidance of attention to internal representations in visual working memory. We show that both voluntary and involuntary factors influence memory performance but do so in distinct ways. Moreover, by tracking gaze biases linked to attentional focusing in memory, we provide direct evidence for an involuntary “retro-capture” effect whereby external stimuli involuntarily trigger the selection of feature-matching internal representations. We show that stimulus-driven and goal-directed influences compete for selection in memory, and that the balance of this competition—as reflected in oculomotor signatures of internal attention—predicts the quality of ensuing memory-guided behavior. Thus, goal-directed and stimulus-driven factors together determine the fate not only of perception, but also of internal representations in working memory.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Open science, communal culture, and women’s participation in the movement to improve science
- Author
-
Jorge Mejia, Corinne A. Moss-Racusin, Aneeta Rattan, Xiaoran Yan, Kate A. Ratliff, Judith M. Harackiewicz, Amanda B. Diekman, Krishna Savani, Julie A. Garcia, Mesmin Destin, Daryl A. Wout, Alison Ledgerwood, Susanne Ressl, Sylvia P. Perry, Denise Sekaquaptewa, Sapna Cheryan, Dustin B. Thoman, Patricia L. Mabry, Elizabeth L. Haines, Lora E. Park, Diana T. Sanchez, Mary C. Murphy, Jessi L. Smith, Franco Pestilli, Valerie Jones Taylor, Stephanie A. Fryberg, Nilanjana Dasgupta, and Amanda F. Mejia
- Subjects
Open science ,Science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Research methodology ,Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Clinical Research ,open science ,ComputingMilieux_COMPUTERSANDEDUCATION ,replicability ,Humans ,Women ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,reproducibility ,Publication ,media_common ,Structure (mathematical logic) ,Multidisciplinary ,Information Dissemination ,Movement (music) ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Reproducibility of Results ,Public relations ,Authorship ,culture ,Prosocial behavior ,Open Access Publishing ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Semantic analysis (knowledge representation) ,Psychology ,business ,Diversity (politics) - Abstract
Significance Science is rapidly changing with the current movement to improve science focused largely on reproducibility/replicability and open science practices. Through network modeling and semantic analysis, this article provides an initial exploration of the structure, cultural frames of collaboration and prosociality, and representation of women in the open science and reproducibility literatures. Network analyses reveal that the open science and reproducibility literatures are emerging relatively independently with few common papers or authors. Open science has a more collaborative structure and includes more explicit language reflecting communality and prosociality than does reproducibility. Finally, women publish more frequently in high-status author positions within open science compared with reproducibility. Implications for cultivating a diverse, collaborative culture of science are discussed., Science is undergoing rapid change with the movement to improve science focused largely on reproducibility/replicability and open science practices. This moment of change—in which science turns inward to examine its methods and practices—provides an opportunity to address its historic lack of diversity and noninclusive culture. Through network modeling and semantic analysis, we provide an initial exploration of the structure, cultural frames, and women’s participation in the open science and reproducibility literatures (n = 2,926 articles and conference proceedings). Network analyses suggest that the open science and reproducibility literatures are emerging relatively independently of each other, sharing few common papers or authors. We next examine whether the literatures differentially incorporate collaborative, prosocial ideals that are known to engage members of underrepresented groups more than independent, winner-takes-all approaches. We find that open science has a more connected, collaborative structure than does reproducibility. Semantic analyses of paper abstracts reveal that these literatures have adopted different cultural frames: open science includes more explicitly communal and prosocial language than does reproducibility. Finally, consistent with literature suggesting the diversity benefits of communal and prosocial purposes, we find that women publish more frequently in high-status author positions (first or last) within open science (vs. reproducibility). Furthermore, this finding is further patterned by team size and time. Women are more represented in larger teams within reproducibility, and women’s participation is increasing in open science over time and decreasing in reproducibility. We conclude with actionable suggestions for cultivating a more prosocial and diverse culture of science.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. The neural basis of language development: Changes in lateralization over age
- Author
-
Elissa L. Newport, Catherine E. Chambers, Madison M. Berl, William D. Gaillard, Anna Seydell-Greenwald, Peter E. Turkeltaub, Olumide A Olulade, and Alexander W. Dromerick
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,brain ,Social Sciences ,Audiology ,Language Development ,Functional Laterality ,Lateralization of brain function ,Correlation ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Neuroimaging ,medicine ,Humans ,lateralization ,Child ,development ,030304 developmental biology ,Brain Mapping ,0303 health sciences ,language ,Multidisciplinary ,fMRI ,Electroencephalography ,Biological Sciences ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Broca Area ,Language development ,Electrophysiology ,Child, Preschool ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Correlation analysis ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Language network ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Significance Two types of evidence suggest different pictures of how language is represented in the brain during development. Studies of the anatomy, physiology, and fMRI activation of the two hemispheres show that language is lateralized to the left hemisphere from birth. In contrast, damage to the left versus right hemisphere in young children is equally likely to result in language impairment, suggesting that language is bilaterally represented in early development. The present study resolves this paradox by examining fMRI language activation in different ways. While group averages show LH lateralization throughout development, young children show RH language activation that declines systematically with age. Most important, this RH activation in children represents a possible mechanism for explaining language recovery following early stroke., We have long known that language is lateralized to the left hemisphere (LH) in most neurologically healthy adults. In contrast, findings on lateralization of function during development are more complex. As in adults, anatomical, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging studies in infants and children indicate LH lateralization for language. However, in very young children, lesions to either hemisphere are equally likely to result in language deficits, suggesting that language is distributed symmetrically early in life. We address this apparent contradiction by examining patterns of functional MRI (fMRI) language activation in children (ages 4 through 13) and adults (ages 18 through 29). In contrast to previous studies, we focus not on lateralization per se but rather on patterns of left-hemisphere (LH) and right-hemisphere (RH) activation across individual participants over age. Our analyses show significant activation not only in the LH language network but also in their RH homologs in all of the youngest children (ages 4 through 6). The proportion of participants showing significant RH activation decreases over age, with over 60% of adults lacking any significant RH activation. A whole-brain correlation analysis revealed an age-related decrease in language activation only in the RH homolog of Broca’s area. This correlation was independent of task difficulty. We conclude that, while language is left-lateralized throughout life, the RH contribution to language processing is also strong early in life and decreases through childhood. Importantly, this early RH language activation may represent a developmental mechanism for recovery following early LH injury.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. A map of decoy influence in human multialternative choice
- Author
-
Vickie Li, Christopher Summerfield, Tsvetomira Dumbalska, and Konstantinos Tsetsos
- Subjects
cognition ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Consumer choice ,Compromise ,Social Sciences ,Context (language use) ,Space (commercial competition) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Economic Sciences ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Similarity (psychology) ,Econometrics ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Quality (business) ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,consumer choice ,Cognition ,decision-making ,human behavior ,Biological Sciences ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Decoy ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Significance Imagine you are deciding between two goods: A is simple but inexpensive, B is luxurious but more costly. Introducing a less advantageous option (e.g., lower quality than A, same price) should not alter your choice between A and B. However, this principle is often violated; three classic biases known as “decoy effects” have been identified, each describing a stereotyped choice pattern in the presence of irrelevant information. Through behavioral testing in human participants and computer simulations, we show that these decoy effects are special cases of a wider principle, whereby stimulus value information is encoded in a relative, rather than an absolute, format. This work clarifies the origin of three behavioral phenomena that are widely studied in psychology and economics., Human decisions can be biased by irrelevant information. For example, choices between two preferred alternatives can be swayed by a third option that is inferior or unavailable. Previous work has identified three classic biases, known as the attraction, similarity, and compromise effects, which arise during choices between economic alternatives defined by two attributes. However, the reliability, interrelationship, and computational origin of these three biases have been controversial. Here, a large cohort of human participants made incentive-compatible choices among assets that varied in price and quality. Instead of focusing on the three classic effects, we sampled decoy stimuli exhaustively across bidimensional multiattribute space and constructed a full map of decoy influence on choices between two otherwise preferred target items. Our analysis reveals that the decoy influence map is highly structured even beyond the three classic biases. We identify a very simple model that can fully reproduce the decoy influence map and capture its variability in individual participants. This model reveals that the three decoy effects are not distinct phenomena but are all special cases of a more general principle, by which attribute values are repulsed away from the context provided by rival options. The model helps us understand why the biases are typically correlated across participants and allows us to validate a prediction about their interrelationship. This work helps to clarify the origin of three of the most widely studied biases in human decision-making.
- Published
- 2020
19. Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects
- Author
-
Alexander LaTourrette and Sandra R. Waxman
- Subjects
Male ,genetic structures ,naming ,Object (grammar) ,Social Sciences ,Time ,memory ,InformationSystems_MODELSANDPRINCIPLES ,Cognition ,Noun ,Humans ,Names ,Control (linguistics) ,Recognition memory ,language ,learning ,Multidisciplinary ,infants ,Representation (systemics) ,Infant ,Recognition, Psychology ,Conjunction (grammar) ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Mental Recall ,Female ,Construal level theory ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Encoding objects in memory and recalling them later is fundamental to human cognition and emerges in infancy. Here, using a new recognition memory paradigm, we show that the way an object is named, either as a unique individual or as a member of a category, is instrumental in 12-mo-old infants’ encoding of and memory for that object. When the same name is applied consistently to a set of objects, infants encode primarily their commonalities. In contrast, when a unique name is applied to each object, infants encode each object’s unique features. Thus, even as infants begin to produce their first words, a single naming event exerts powerful, nuanced effects on the fundamental cognitive processes of object representation and memory., A foundation of human cognition is the flexibility with which we can represent any object as either a unique individual (my dog Fred) or a member of an object category (dog, animal). This conceptual flexibility is supported by language; the way we name an object is instrumental to our construal of that object as an individual or a category member. Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that infants are sensitive to this principled link between naming and object representation by age 12 mo. During training, all infants (n = 77) viewed four distinct objects from the same object category, each introduced in conjunction with either the same novel noun (Consistent Name condition), a distinct novel noun for each object (Distinct Names condition), or the same sine-wave tone sequence (Consistent Tone condition). At test, infants saw each training object again, presented in silence along with a new object from the same category. Infants in the Consistent Name condition showed poor recognition memory at test, suggesting that consistently applied names focused them primarily on commonalities among the named objects at the expense of distinctions among them. Infants in the Distinct Names condition recognized three of the four objects, suggesting that applying distinct names enhanced infants’ encoding of the distinctions among the objects. Infants in the control Consistent Tone condition recognized only the object they had most recently seen. Thus, even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way in which an object is named guides infants’ encoding, representation, and memory for that object.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. The objectivity illusion and voter polarization in the 2016 presidential election
- Author
-
Lee Ross, Michael C. Schwalbe, and Geoffrey L. Cohen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Political psychology ,Presidential election ,cognitive bias ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Illusion ,Social Sciences ,Antipathy ,Humans ,Longitudinal Studies ,Social Behavior ,Objectivity (science) ,media_common ,polarization ,Multidisciplinary ,Distrust ,Politics ,Polarization (politics) ,Middle Aged ,Illusions ,Cognitive bias ,intergroup conflict ,Attitude ,Social Perception ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Psychology ,political psychology ,Goals ,Social psychology - Abstract
Significance Political polarization increasingly threatens democratic institutions. The belief that “my side” sees the world objectively while the “other side” sees it through the lens of its biases contributes to this political polarization and accompanying animus and distrust. This conviction, known as the “objectivity illusion,” was strong and persistent among Trump and Clinton supporters in the weeks before the 2016 presidential election. We show that the objectivity illusion predicts subsequent bias and polarization, including heightened partisanship over the presidential debates. A follow-up study showed that both groups impugned the objectivity of a putative blog author supporting the opposition candidate and saw supporters of that opposing candidate as evil., Two studies conducted during the 2016 presidential campaign examined the dynamics of the objectivity illusion, the belief that the views of “my side” are objective while the views of the opposing side are the product of bias. In the first, a three-stage longitudinal study spanning the presidential debates, supporters of the two candidates exhibited a large and generally symmetrical tendency to rate supporters of the candidate they personally favored as more influenced by appropriate (i.e., “normative”) considerations, and less influenced by various sources of bias than supporters of the opposing candidate. This study broke new ground by demonstrating that the degree to which partisans displayed the objectivity illusion predicted subsequent bias in their perception of debate performance and polarization in their political attitudes over time, as well as closed-mindedness and antipathy toward political adversaries. These associations, furthermore, remained significant even after controlling for baseline levels of partisanship. A second study conducted 2 d before the election showed similar perceptions of objectivity versus bias in ratings of blog authors favoring the candidate participants personally supported or opposed. These ratings were again associated with polarization and, additionally, with the willingness to characterize supporters of the opposing candidate as evil and likely to commit acts of terrorism. At a time of particular political division and distrust in America, these findings point to the exacerbating role played by the illusion of objectivity.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Stochastic sampling provides a unifying account of visual working memory limits
- Author
-
Schneegans, Sebastian, Taylor, Robert, Bays, Paul M, Schneegans, Sebastian [0000-0002-1246-2259], Taylor, Robert [0000-0002-7736-4545], Bays, Paul M [0000-0003-4684-4893], and Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository
- Subjects
Theoretical computer science ,Discretization ,Computer science ,Property (programming) ,Models, Neurological ,Sample (statistics) ,050105 experimental psychology ,visual working memory ,capacity limits ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,population coding ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Limit (mathematics) ,resource model ,Multidisciplinary ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,Probabilistic logic ,Sampling (statistics) ,Biological Sciences ,Models, Theoretical ,Memory, Short-Term ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Mental Recall ,Visual Perception ,Key (cryptography) ,Neural coding ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Significance We demonstrate that three of the most prominent accounts of visual working memory in the psychology and neuroscience literature—the slots+averaging model, the variable precision model, and the population coding model—can all be expressed in the common mathematical framework of sampling. This reformulation allows us to pinpoint the key differences between these models, and to determine which factors are critical to account for the observed patterns of recall errors across different human psychophysical experiments. Moreover, the sampling framework provides a possible neural grounding for these models in the spiking activity of neuronal populations, as well as a link to existing theories of capacity limits in visual attention., Research into human working memory limits has been shaped by the competition between different formal models, with a central point of contention being whether internal representations are continuous or discrete. Here we describe a sampling approach derived from principles of neural coding as a framework to understand working memory limits. Reconceptualizing existing models in these terms reveals strong commonalities between seemingly opposing accounts, but also allows us to identify specific points of difference. We show that the discrete versus continuous nature of sampling is not critical to model fits, but that, instead, random variability in sample counts is the key to reproducing human performance in both single- and whole-report tasks. A probabilistic limit on the number of items successfully retrieved is an emergent property of stochastic sampling, requiring no explicit mechanism to enforce it. These findings resolve discrepancies between previous accounts and establish a unified computational framework for working memory that is compatible with neural principles.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Heuristics and optimal solutions to the breadth–depth dilemma
- Author
-
Benjamin Y. Hayden, Jorge Ramírez-Ruiz, Rubén Moreno-Bote, and Jan Drugowitsch
- Subjects
Rationalization ,Mathematical optimization ,Multidisciplinary ,Computer science ,Bounded rationality ,Breadth–depth dilemma ,Foraging ,Social Sciences ,Sample (statistics) ,Models, Theoretical ,Biological Sciences ,Choice Behavior ,Dilemma ,Risky choice ,Bounded function ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Heuristics ,Humans ,Fraction (mathematics) ,Relevance (information retrieval) ,Metareasoning ,Decision making ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Significance From choosing among the many courses offered in graduate school to dividing budget into research programs, the breadth–depth is a commonplace dilemma that arises when finite resources (e.g., time, money, cognitive capabilities) need to be allocated among a large range of alternatives. For such problems, decision makers need to trade off breadth—allocating little capacity to each of many alternatives—and depth—focusing capacity on a few options. We found that little available capacity (less than 10 samples for search) promotes allocating resources broadly, and thus breadth search is favored. Increased capacity results in an abrupt transition toward favoring a balance between breadth and depth. We finally describe a rich casuistic and heuristics for metareasoning with finite resources., In multialternative risky choice, we are often faced with the opportunity to allocate our limited information-gathering capacity between several options before receiving feedback. In such cases, we face a natural trade-off between breadth—spreading our capacity across many options—and depth—gaining more information about a smaller number of options. Despite its broad relevance to daily life, including in many naturalistic foraging situations, the optimal strategy in the breadth–depth trade-off has not been delineated. Here, we formalize the breadth–depth dilemma through a finite-sample capacity model. We find that, if capacity is small (∼10 samples), it is optimal to draw one sample per alternative, favoring breadth. However, for larger capacities, a sharp transition is observed, and it becomes best to deeply sample a very small fraction of alternatives, which roughly decreases with the square root of capacity. Thus, ignoring most options, even when capacity is large enough to shallowly sample all of them, is a signature of optimal behavior. Our results also provide a rich casuistic for metareasoning in multialternative decisions with bounded capacity using close-to-optimal heuristics.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Protecting memory from misinformation: Warnings modulate cortical reinstatement during memory retrieval
- Author
-
Nathaniel Rabb, Elizabeth Race, Jessica M. Karanian, McKinzey G Torrance, Ayanna K. Thomas, and Alia N. Wulff
- Subjects
Memory distortion ,Adult ,Male ,Time Factors ,Social Sciences ,050105 experimental psychology ,cortical reinstatement ,03 medical and health sciences ,Neural activity ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Memory ,Encoding (memory) ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Misinformation ,Memory test ,misinformation ,Multidisciplinary ,Narration ,Memory errors ,Communication ,05 social sciences ,fMRI ,Reproducibility of Results ,Retention, Psychology ,eyewitness memory ,16. Peace & justice ,Reduced susceptibility ,Eyewitness memory ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Mental Recall ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Exposure to misleading information can distort memory for past events (misinformation effect). Here, we show that providing individuals with a simple warning about the threat of misinformation significantly reduces the misinformation effect, regardless of whether warnings are provided proactively (before exposure to misinformation) or retroactively (after exposure to misinformation). In the brain, this protective effect of warning is associated with increased reactivation of sensory regions associated with the original event and decreased reactivation of sensory regions associated with the misleading information. These findings reveal that warnings can protect memory from misinformation by modulating reconstructive processes at the time of memory retrieval and have important practical implications for improving the accuracy of eyewitness testimony as well as everyday memory reports., Exposure to even subtle forms of misleading information can significantly alter memory for past events. Memory distortion due to misinformation has been linked to faulty reconstructive processes during memory retrieval and the reactivation of brain regions involved in the initial encoding of misleading details (cortical reinstatement). The current study investigated whether warning participants about the threat of misinformation can modulate cortical reinstatement during memory retrieval and reduce misinformation errors. Participants watched a silent video depicting a crime (original event) and were given an initial test of memory for the crime details. Then, participants listened to an auditory narrative describing the crime in which some original details were altered (misinformation). Importantly, participants who received a warning about the reliability of the auditory narrative either before or after exposure to misinformation demonstrated less susceptibility to misinformation on a final test of memory compared to unwarned participants. Warned and unwarned participants also demonstrated striking differences in neural activity during the final memory test. Compared to participants who did not receive a warning, participants who received a warning (regardless of its timing) demonstrated increased activity in visual regions associated with the original source of information as well as decreased activity in auditory regions associated with the misleading source of information. Stronger visual reactivation was associated with reduced susceptibility to misinformation, whereas stronger auditory reactivation was associated with increased susceptibility to misinformation. Together, these results suggest that a simple warning can modulate reconstructive processes during memory retrieval and reduce memory errors due to misinformation.
- Published
- 2020
24. Historically rice-farming societies have tighter social norms in China and worldwide
- Author
-
Thomas Talhelm and Alexander S. English
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Irrigation ,China ,Social Sciences ,Norm of reciprocity ,subsistence style ,Young Adult ,Urbanization ,Development economics ,Social Norms ,Humans ,East Asia ,Rice farming ,Rice farmers ,Triticum ,Farmers ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Agriculture ,Oryza ,tightness–looseness ,Middle Aged ,culture ,Geography ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Norm (social) ,Rural area ,business ,norms ,rice theory ,Regional differences - Abstract
Significance Rice is a highly interdependent crop. Rice required far more labor than dryland crops like wheat, and rice’s irrigation networks forced farmers to coordinate water use. To deal with these demands, rice villages developed strong norms for labor exchange. Using China as a natural test case, we compare nearby provinces that differ in rice and wheat, but share the same ethnicity, religion, and national government. In survey data from over 11,000 Chinese citizens, rice-farming provinces report tighter norms than traditionally wheat-growing provinces. Rice also predicts tight norms around the world. These data suggest that China’s agricultural past still shapes cultural differences in the modern day—and perhaps explain why East Asia has tighter social norms than the wheat-growing West., Data recently published in PNAS mapped out regional differences in the tightness of social norms across China [R. Y. J. Chua, K. G. Huang, M. Jin, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 6720–6725 (2019)]. Norms were tighter in developed, urbanized areas and weaker in rural areas. We tested whether historical paddy rice farming has left a legacy on social norms in modern China. Premodern rice farming could plausibly create strong social norms because paddy rice relied on irrigation networks. Rice farmers coordinated their water use and kept track of each person’s labor contributions. Rice villages also established strong norms of reciprocity to cope with labor demands that were twice as high as dryland crops like wheat. In line with this theory, China’s historically rice-farming areas had tighter social norms than wheat-farming areas, even beyond differences in development and urbanization. Rice–wheat differences were just as large among people in 10 neighboring provinces (n = 3,835) along the rice–wheat border. These neighboring provinces differ sharply in rice and wheat, but little in latitude, temperature, and other potential confounding variables. Outside of China, rice farming predicted norm tightness in 32 countries around the world. Finally, people in rice-farming areas scored lower on innovative thinking, which tends to be lower in societies with tight norms. This natural test case within China might explain why East Asia—historically reliant on rice farming—has tighter social norms than the wheat-farming West.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Adherence to suicide reporting guidelines by news shared on a social networking platform
- Author
-
Farshad Kooti, Steven A. Sumner, and Moira Burke
- Subjects
Suicide Prevention ,social networks ,Facebook ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Social Sciences ,Guidelines as Topic ,Social Networking ,Perception ,news reporting ,Odds Ratio ,Humans ,Social media ,Empirical evidence ,News media ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,Guideline adherence ,Odds ratio ,Biological Sciences ,United States ,Suicide ,contagion ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Guideline Adherence ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Social Media - Abstract
Significance Rates of suicide in the United States have increased over recent decades. One modifiable risk factor for suicide is exposure to sensationalized media content about suicide. Health authorities have published guidelines for news media reporting on suicide; however, uptake of recommendations remains limited. We conducted a study to analyze adherence to the guidelines on news shared on Facebook and assessed how adherence affects reader engagement. Certain harmful elements were widely prevalent in news media while protective elements were limited; however, contrary to popular perception, articles which more closely adhered to safe-reporting practices were shared more often. Variability in scores exists across nations and publisher types with implications for journalists, public health organizations, social media companies, researchers, and lay readers., Rates of suicide in the United States are at a more than 20-y high. Suicide contagion, or spread of suicide-related thoughts and behaviors through exposure to sensationalized and harmful content is a well-recognized phenomenon. Health authorities have published guidelines for news media reporting on suicide to help prevent contagion; however, uptake of recommendations remains limited. A key barrier to widespread voluntary uptake of suicide-reporting guidelines is that more sensational content is perceived to be more engaging to readers and thus enhances publisher visibility and engagement; however, no empirical information exists on the actual influence of adherence to safe-reporting practices on reader engagement. Hence, we conducted a study to analyze adherence to suicide-reporting guidelines on news shared on social media and to assess how adherence affects reader engagement. Our analysis of Facebook data revealed that harmful elements were prevalent in news articles about suicide shared on social media while the presence of protective elements was generally rare. Contrary to popular perception, closer adherence to safe-reporting practices was associated with a greater likelihood of an article being reshared (adjusted odds ratio [AOR] = 1.19, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.10 to 1.27) and receiving positive engagement (“love” reactions) (AOR = 1.20, 95% CI = 1.13 to 1.26). Mean safe-reporting scores were lower in the US than other English-speaking nations and variation existed by publisher characteristics. Our results provide empirical evidence that improved adherence to suicide-reporting guidelines may benefit not only the health of individuals, but also support publisher goals of reach and engagement.
- Published
- 2020
26. The limits of color awareness during active, real-world vision
- Author
-
Caroline E. Robertson, Thomas L. Botch, and Michael A. Cohen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,vision ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,ComputingMethodologies_IMAGEPROCESSINGANDCOMPUTERVISION ,scenes ,Social Sciences ,Color ,Virtual reality ,050105 experimental psychology ,Visual processing ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Visual experience ,Color detection ,Vision, Ocular ,ComputingMethodologies_COMPUTERGRAPHICS ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Virtual Reality ,Observer (special relativity) ,Biological Sciences ,Awareness ,Gaze ,attention ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Eye tracking ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Color is a foundational aspect of visual experience that aids in segmenting objects, identifying food sources, and signaling emotions. Intuitively, it feels that we are immersed in a colorful world that extends to the farthest limits of our periphery. How accurate is our intuition? Here, we used gaze-contingent rendering in immersive VR to reveal the limits of color awareness during naturalistic viewing. Observers explored 360° real-world environments, which we altered so that only the regions where observers looked were in color, while their periphery was black-and-white. Overall, we found that observers routinely failed to notice when color vanished from the majority of their visual world. These results show that our intuitive sense of a rich, colorful world is largely incorrect., Color ignites visual experience, imbuing the world with meaning, emotion, and richness. As soon as an observer opens their eyes, they have the immediate impression of a rich, colorful experience that encompasses their entire visual world. Here, we show that this impression is surprisingly inaccurate. We used head-mounted virtual reality (VR) to place observers in immersive, dynamic real-world environments, which they naturally explored via saccades and head turns. Meanwhile, we monitored their gaze with in-headset eye tracking and then systematically altered the visual environments such that only the parts of the scene they were looking at were presented in color and the rest of the scene (i.e., the visual periphery) was entirely desaturated. We found that observers were often completely unaware of these drastic alterations to their visual world. In the most extreme case, almost a third of observers failed to notice when less than 5% of the visual display was presented in color. This limitation on perceptual awareness could not be explained by retinal neuroanatomy or previous studies of peripheral visual processing using more traditional psychophysical approaches. In a second study, we measured color detection thresholds using a staircase procedure while a set of observers intentionally attended to the periphery. Still, we found that observers were unaware when a large portion of their field of view was desaturated. Together, these results show that during active, naturalistic viewing conditions, our intuitive sense of a rich, colorful visual world is largely incorrect.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Individual differences in trust evaluations are shaped mostly by environments, not genes
- Author
-
Jemma R. Collova, Clare A. M. Sutherland, Gillian Rhodes, Nichola Burton, Romina Palermo, Laura Germine, Jeremy Wilmer, Gabriëlla A.M. Blokland, Psychiatrie & Neuropsychologie, and RS: MHeNs - R2 - Mental Health
- Subjects
Attractiveness ,media_common.quotation_subject ,INFERENCES ,Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,UNIQUE ,first impressions ,Facial recognition system ,050105 experimental psychology ,FACE RECOGNITION ,JUDGMENTS ,Social cognition ,Perception ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,SOCIAL ATTRIBUTIONS ,behavioral genetics ,face evaluation ,Behavioural genetics ,media_common ,classical twin design ,Multidisciplinary ,FACIAL 1ST IMPRESSIONS ,05 social sciences ,Perspective (graphical) ,trust ,Variation (linguistics) ,Dominance (ethology) ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Rapid impressions of trustworthiness can have extreme consequences, impacting financial lending, partner selection, and death-penalty sentencing decisions. But to what extent do people disagree about who looks trustworthy, and why? Here, we demonstrate that individual differences in trustworthiness and other impressions are substantial and stable, agreeing with the classic idea that social perception can be influenced in part by the “eye of the beholder.” Moreover, by examining twins, we show that individual differences in impressions of trustworthiness are shaped mostly by personal experiences, instead of genes or familial experiences. Our study highlights individual social learning as a key mechanism by which we individually come to trust others, with potentially profound consequences for everyday trust decisions., People evaluate a stranger’s trustworthiness from their facial features in a fraction of a second, despite common advice “not to judge a book by its cover.” Evaluations of trustworthiness have critical and widespread social impact, predicting financial lending, mate selection, and even criminal justice outcomes. Consequently, understanding how people perceive trustworthiness from faces has been a major focus of scientific inquiry, and detailed models explain how consensus impressions of trustworthiness are driven by facial attributes. However, facial impression models do not consider variation between observers. Here, we develop a sensitive test of trustworthiness evaluation and use it to document substantial, stable individual differences in trustworthiness impressions. Via a twin study, we show that these individual differences are largely shaped by variation in personal experience, rather than genes or shared environments. Finally, using multivariate twin modeling, we show that variation in trustworthiness evaluation is specific, dissociating from other key facial evaluations of dominance and attractiveness. Our finding that variation in facial trustworthiness evaluation is driven mostly by personal experience represents a rare example of a core social perceptual capacity being predominantly shaped by a person’s unique environment. Notably, it stands in sharp contrast to variation in facial recognition ability, which is driven mostly by genes. Our study provides insights into the development of the social brain, offers a different perspective on disagreement in trust in wider society, and motivates new research into the origins and potential malleability of face evaluation, a critical aspect of human social cognition.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Decomposing loss aversion from gaze allocation and pupil dilation
- Author
-
Arjun Ramakrishnan, Wenjia Joyce Zhao, Feng Sheng, Michael L. Platt, Samuel Thelaus, Darsol Seok, and Puti Cen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Computer science ,Decision Making ,Social Sciences ,Fixation, Ocular ,Models, Psychological ,pupil dilation ,050105 experimental psychology ,Odds ,loss aversion ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,Risk-Taking ,0302 clinical medicine ,gaze allocation ,Loss aversion ,Econometrics ,Pupillary response ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,10. No inequality ,Valuation (finance) ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Pupil ,Cognition ,Biological Sciences ,Middle Aged ,Response bias ,Gaze ,Nontherapeutic Human Experimentation ,drift-diffusion model ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Pupillometry ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Significance We revisit the concept of loss aversion by synthesizing distinct views into an integrative framework and by probing physiological biomarkers associated with the behavior. The framework decomposes loss aversion into a valuation bias, which weighs losses over gains, and a response bias, which avoids loss-related choices altogether. Further, we reveal a double dissociation in physiology underlying the decision process. Valuation bias was associated with preferential gaze allocation to losses whereas response bias was associated with pupillary dilation. Our framework exposes biological heterogeneity underlying loss aversion and distinguishes different loss-averse decision makers who are otherwise indistinguishable using conventional approaches. Our integrative approach provides a deeper analysis of the mechanisms underlying loss aversion and incorporates distinct views within a unified biological framework., Loss-averse decisions, in which one avoids losses at the expense of gains, are highly prevalent. However, the underlying mechanisms remain controversial. The prevailing account highlights a valuation bias that overweighs losses relative to gains, but an alternative view stresses a response bias to avoid choices involving potential losses. Here we couple a computational process model with eye-tracking and pupillometry to develop a physiologically grounded framework for the decision process leading to accepting or rejecting gambles with equal odds of winning and losing money. Overall, loss-averse decisions were accompanied by preferential gaze toward losses and increased pupil dilation for accepting gambles. Using our model, we found gaze allocation selectively indexed valuation bias, and pupil dilation selectively indexed response bias. Finally, we demonstrate that our computational model and physiological biomarkers can identify distinct types of loss-averse decision makers who would otherwise be indistinguishable using conventional approaches. Our study provides an integrative framework for the cognitive processes that drive loss-averse decisions and highlights the biological heterogeneity of loss aversion across individuals.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Other people’s gaze encoded as implied motion in the human brain
- Author
-
Andrew I. Wilterson, Davis Wachtell, Arvid Guterstam, and Michael S. A. Graziano
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Brain activity and meditation ,Fixation, Ocular ,social cognition ,gaze ,Young Adult ,Social cognition ,Parietal Lobe ,Theory of mind ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Letters ,Motion perception ,Social Behavior ,theory of mind ,Multidisciplinary ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Middle Aged ,Biological Sciences ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Gaze ,motion perception ,Healthy Volunteers ,Temporal Lobe ,Sketch ,visual attention ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging ,Psychology ,Classifier (UML) ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance This study used fMRI scans of people to show that the brain processes the gaze of others, and visual flow, in a similar manner. Visual motion brain areas and social cognition areas were involved. It is as if the brain draws a quick visual sketch with moving arrows to help keep track of who is attending to what. We propose that this implicit, fluid-flow model of other people’s gaze may help explain culturally universal myths about the mind as an energy-like, flowing essence., Keeping track of other people’s gaze is an essential task in social cognition and key for successfully reading other people’s intentions and beliefs (theory of mind). Recent behavioral evidence suggests that we construct an implicit model of other people’s gaze, which may incorporate physically incoherent attributes such as a construct of force-carrying beams that emanate from the eyes. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging and multivoxel pattern analysis to test the prediction that the brain encodes gaze as implied motion streaming from an agent toward a gazed-upon object. We found that a classifier, trained to discriminate the direction of visual motion, significantly decoded the gaze direction in static images depicting a sighted face, but not a blindfolded one, from brain activity patterns in the human motion-sensitive middle temporal complex (MT+) and temporo-parietal junction (TPJ). Our results demonstrate a link between the visual motion system and social brain mechanisms, in which the TPJ, a key node in theory of mind, works in concert with MT+ to encode gaze as implied motion. This model may be a fundamental aspect of social cognition that allows us to efficiently connect agents with the objects of their attention. It is as if the brain draws a quick visual sketch with moving arrows to help keep track of who is attending to what. This implicit, fluid-flow model of other people’s gaze may help explain culturally universal myths about the mind as an energy-like, flowing essence.
- Published
- 2020
30. Estimating geographic subjective well-being from Twitter: A comparison of dictionary and data-driven language methods
- Author
-
Lyle H. Ungar, Margaret L. Kern, Kokil Jaidka, H. Andrew Schwartz, Salvatore Giorgi, and Johannes C. Eichstaedt
- Subjects
020205 medical informatics ,Computer science ,Twitter ,Word count ,Population ,Big data ,Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Sample (statistics) ,02 engineering and technology ,big data ,Phone ,Language assessment ,0202 electrical engineering, electronic engineering, information engineering ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social media ,education ,Estimation ,education.field_of_study ,Multidisciplinary ,Computer Sciences ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Data science ,machine learning ,subjective well-being ,language analysis ,Physical Sciences ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,business - Abstract
Significance Spatial aggregation of Twitter language may make it possible to monitor the subjective well-being of populations on a large scale. Text analysis methods need to yield robust estimates to be dependable. On the one hand, we find that data-driven machine learning-based methods offer accurate and robust measurements of regional well-being across the United States when evaluated against gold-standard Gallup survey measures. On the other hand, we find that standard English word-level methods (such as Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count 2015’s Positive emotion dictionary and Language Assessment by Mechanical Turk) can yield estimates of county well-being inversely correlated with survey estimates, due to regional cultural and socioeconomic differences in language use. Some of the most frequent misleading words can be removed to improve the accuracy of these word-level methods., Researchers and policy makers worldwide are interested in measuring the subjective well-being of populations. When users post on social media, they leave behind digital traces that reflect their thoughts and feelings. Aggregation of such digital traces may make it possible to monitor well-being at large scale. However, social media-based methods need to be robust to regional effects if they are to produce reliable estimates. Using a sample of 1.53 billion geotagged English tweets, we provide a systematic evaluation of word-level and data-driven methods for text analysis for generating well-being estimates for 1,208 US counties. We compared Twitter-based county-level estimates with well-being measurements provided by the Gallup-Sharecare Well-Being Index survey through 1.73 million phone surveys. We find that word-level methods (e.g., Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count [LIWC] 2015 and Language Assessment by Mechanical Turk [LabMT]) yielded inconsistent county-level well-being measurements due to regional, cultural, and socioeconomic differences in language use. However, removing as few as three of the most frequent words led to notable improvements in well-being prediction. Data-driven methods provided robust estimates, approximating the Gallup data at up to r = 0.64. We show that the findings generalized to county socioeconomic and health outcomes and were robust when poststratifying the samples to be more representative of the general US population. Regional well-being estimation from social media data seems to be robust when supervised data-driven methods are used.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. The effects of communicating uncertainty on public trust in facts and numbers
- Author
-
Anne Marthe van der Bles, Sander van der Linden, David Spiegelhalter, Alexandra L. J. Freeman, van der Bles, Anne Marthe [0000-0002-7953-9425], van der Linden, Sander [0000-0002-0269-1744], Freeman, Alexandra LJ [0000-0002-4115-161X], Spiegelhalter, David J [0000-0001-9350-6745], Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, Social Psychology, and Freeman, Alexandra L J [0000-0002-4115-161X]
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Immigration ,Internet privacy ,Social Sciences ,Sample (statistics) ,010501 environmental sciences ,DECISION-MAKING ,01 natural sciences ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Meta-Analysis as Topic ,Global health ,Humans ,030212 general & internal medicine ,Uncertainty quantification ,uncertainty ,0105 earth and related environmental sciences ,media_common ,posttruth ,Internet ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,communication ,trust ,Human knowledge ,RISKS ,Trustworthiness ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Public trust ,contested ,business ,Psychology - Abstract
Significance Does openly communicating uncertainty around facts and numbers necessarily undermine audiences’ trust in the facts, or the communicators? Despite concerns among scientists, experts, and journalists, this has not been studied extensively. In four experiments and one field experiment on the BBC News website, words and numerical ranges were used to communicate uncertainty in news article-like texts. The texts included contested topics such as climate change and immigration statistics. While people’s prior beliefs about topics influenced their trust in the facts, they did not influence how people responded to the uncertainty being communicated. Communicating uncertainty numerically only exerted a minor effect on trust. Knowing this should allow academics and science communicators to be more transparent about the limits of human knowledge., Uncertainty is inherent to our knowledge about the state of the world yet often not communicated alongside scientific facts and numbers. In the “posttruth” era where facts are increasingly contested, a common assumption is that communicating uncertainty will reduce public trust. However, a lack of systematic research makes it difficult to evaluate such claims. We conducted five experiments—including one preregistered replication with a national sample and one field experiment on the BBC News website (total n = 5,780)—to examine whether communicating epistemic uncertainty about facts across different topics (e.g., global warming, immigration), formats (verbal vs. numeric), and magnitudes (high vs. low) influences public trust. Results show that whereas people do perceive greater uncertainty when it is communicated, we observed only a small decrease in trust in numbers and trustworthiness of the source, and mostly for verbal uncertainty communication. These results could help reassure all communicators of facts and science that they can be more open and transparent about the limits of human knowledge.
- Published
- 2020
32. Inconsistent allocations of harms versus benefits may exacerbate environmental inequality
- Author
-
Tamar Makov, Gal Zauberman, and George E. Newman
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,inequality ,Inequality ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Social Sciences ,fairness ,050109 social psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Stakeholder Participation ,Air Pollution ,Water Quality ,Economics ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Policy Making ,environmental justice ,media_common ,Environmental justice ,Environmental inequality ,Multidisciplinary ,Public economics ,05 social sciences ,Middle Aged ,Environmental Policy ,harms vs. benefits ,allocation decisions ,Framing (social sciences) ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female - Abstract
Significance Local environmental conditions, such as air and water quality, are shaped, in part, by how societies allocate environmental harms and benefits. Since environmental conditions have long-lasting impacts on people’s lives, understanding the psychology behind such allocation decisions is critical. Across studies, we demonstrate that people are less likely to support decisions that increase environmental equality when considering the allocation of environmental harms (vs. benefits). Our findings suggest that careful attention to the way that the allocation of environmental harms is presented to the public could change the support for decisions that address environmental inequality., We report five studies that examine preferences for the allocation of environmental harms and benefits. In all studies, participants were presented with scenarios in which an existing environmental inequality between two otherwise similar communities could either be decreased or increased through various allocation decisions. Our results demonstrate that despite well-established preferences toward equal outcomes, people express weaker preferences for options that increase equality when considering the allocation of environmental harms (e.g., building new polluting facilities) than when considering the allocation of environmental benefits (e.g., applying pollution-reducing technologies). We argue that this effect emerges from fairness considerations rooted in a psychological incompatibility between the allocation of harms, which is seen as an inherently unfair action, and equality, which is a basic fairness principle. Since the allocation of harms is an inevitable part of operations of both governments and businesses, our results suggest that where possible, parties interested in increasing environmental equality may benefit from framing such proposals as bestowing relative benefits instead of imposing relative harms.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Humans incorporate trial-to-trial working memory uncertainty into rewarded decisions
- Author
-
Wei Ji Ma, Maija Honig, and Daryl Fougnie
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Decision Making ,metamemory ,Social Sciences ,Stimulus (physiology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,visual working memory ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Prior probability ,Metamemory ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,reward ,priors ,Multidisciplinary ,Memory errors ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,Uncertainty ,Memory, Short-Term ,Action planning ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance Information stored in working memory (WM) is incorporated into many daily decisions and actions, and many complex decisions involve WM; however, there has been little work on investigating what WM information is used in memory decisions. Here we try to draw connections between WM and decision making by manipulating prior beliefs in a standard WM task with rewards. We use this paradigm to show that WM contains a representation of the trial-by-trial uncertainty of visual stimuli. This uncertainty is incorporated into rewarded decisions along with other information, such as expectations about the environment. By studying WM in parallel with decision making, we can gain new insight into how these systems work together., Working memory (WM) plays an important role in action planning and decision making; however, both the informational content of memory and how that information is used in decisions remain poorly understood. To investigate this, we used a color WM task in which subjects viewed colored stimuli and reported both an estimate of a stimulus color and a measure of memory uncertainty, obtained through a rewarded decision. Reported memory uncertainty is correlated with memory error, showing that people incorporate their trial-to-trial memory quality into rewarded decisions. Moreover, memory uncertainty can be combined with other sources of information; after inducing expectations (prior beliefs) about stimuli probabilities, we found that estimates became shifted toward expected colors, with the shift increasing with reported uncertainty. The data are best fit by models in which people incorporate their trial-to-trial memory uncertainty with potential rewards and prior beliefs. Our results suggest that WM represents uncertainty information, and that this can be combined with prior beliefs. This highlights the potential complexity of WM representations and shows that rewarded decision can be a powerful tool for examining WM and informing and constraining theoretical, computational, and neurobiological models of memory.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Signing at the beginning versus at the end does not decrease dishonesty
- Author
-
Max H. Bazerman, Francesca Gino, Lisa L. Shu, Ashley V. Whillans, Nina Mazar, Dan Ariely, and Ariella Kristal
- Subjects
nudge ,replication ,Deception ,Policy making ,Statement (logic) ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Internet privacy ,Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Contracts ,050105 experimental psychology ,Honesty ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Function (engineering) ,media_common ,Government ,Multidisciplinary ,Dishonesty ,policy-making ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Sign (semiotics) ,morality ,Morality ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychology ,business - Abstract
Significance In 2012, five of the current authors published a paper in PNAS showing that people are more honest when they are asked to sign a veracity statement at the beginning instead of at the end of a tax or insurance audit form. In a recent investigation, across five related experiments we failed to find an effect of signing at the beginning on dishonesty. Following up on these studies, we conducted one preregistered, high-powered direct replication of experiment 1 of the PNAS paper, in which we failed to replicate the original result. The current paper updates the scientific record by showing that signing at the beginning is unlikely to be a simple solution for increasing honest reporting., Honest reporting is essential for society to function well. However, people frequently lie when asked to provide information, such as misrepresenting their income to save money on taxes. A landmark finding published in PNAS [L. L. Shu, N. Mazar, F. Gino, D. Ariely, M. H. Bazerman, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15197–15200 (2012)] provided evidence for a simple way of encouraging honest reporting: asking people to sign a veracity statement at the beginning instead of at the end of a self-report form. Since this finding was published, various government agencies have adopted this practice. However, in this project, we failed to replicate this result. Across five conceptual replications (n = 4,559) and one highly powered, preregistered, direct replication (n = 1,235) conducted with the authors of the original paper, we observed no effect of signing first on honest reporting. Given the policy applications of this result, it is important to update the scientific record regarding the veracity of these results.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. A little history goes a long way toward understanding why we study consciousness the way we do today
- Author
-
Joseph E. LeDoux, Matthias Michel, and Hakwan Lau
- Subjects
Unconscious mind ,Consciousness ,media_common.quotation_subject ,unconscious ,Social Sciences ,Identity (social science) ,Blindsight ,Stimulus (physiology) ,split brain ,amnesia ,Animals ,Humans ,Psychology ,blindsight ,Function (engineering) ,media_common ,Cognitive science ,Behavior ,Multidisciplinary ,Psychological research ,Neurosciences ,Brain ,History, 19th Century ,History, 20th Century ,Biological Sciences ,Perspective ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Thriving ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Consciousness is currently a thriving area of research in psychology and neuroscience. While this is often attributed to events that took place in the early 1990s, consciousness studies today are a continuation of research that started in the late 19th century and that continued throughout the 20th century. From the beginning, the effort built on studies of animals to reveal basic principles of brain organization and function, and of human patients to gain clues about consciousness itself. Particularly important and our focus here is research in the 1950s, 1960s, and 1970s involving three groups of patients—amnesia, split brain, and blindsight. Across all three groups, a similar pattern of results was found—the patients could respond appropriately to stimuli that they denied seeing (or in the case of amnesiacs, having seen before). These studies paved the way for the current wave of research on consciousness. The field is, in fact, still grappling with the implications of the findings showing that the ability to consciously know and report the identity of a visual stimulus can be dissociated in the brain from the mechanisms that underlie the ability to behave in a meaningful way to the same stimulus.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Two systems for thinking about others’ thoughts in the developing brain
- Author
-
Nikolaus Steinbeis, Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann, Angela D. Friederici, and Tania Singer
- Subjects
Male ,Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,False belief ,Temporoparietal junction ,Theory of Mind ,Precuneus ,brain development ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,050105 experimental psychology ,Cortical thickness ,Thinking ,false belief ,03 medical and health sciences ,Nonverbal communication ,0302 clinical medicine ,Supramarginal gyrus ,Theory of mind ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,medicine ,Humans ,Encoding (semiotics) ,Interpersonal Relations ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Gray matter ,Problem Solving ,Brain Mapping ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,gray matter ,Biological Sciences ,cortical thickness ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Brain development ,Social relation ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Child, Preschool ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance The ability to reason about other people’s thoughts and beliefs characterizes the complex social interaction among humans. This ability, called Theory of Mind (ToM), has long been argued to develop around 4 y when children start explicitly reasoning about others' beliefs. However, when tested nonverbally, infants already show action expectations congruent with others’ beliefs before the age of 2 y. Do these behaviors reflect different systems for understanding others’ minds—an early and a later developing one—or when does ToM develop? We show that these abilities are supported by the maturation of independent brain networks, suggesting different systems for explicit verbal ToM and early nonverbal action expectations., Human social interaction crucially relies on the ability to infer what other people think. Referred to as Theory of Mind (ToM), this ability has long been argued to emerge around 4 y of age when children start passing traditional verbal ToM tasks. This developmental dogma has recently been questioned by nonverbal ToM tasks passed by infants younger than 2 y of age. How do young children solve these tests, and what is their relation to the later-developing verbal ToM reasoning? Are there two different systems for nonverbal and verbal ToM, and when is the developmental onset of mature adult ToM? To address these questions, we related markers of cortical brain structure (i.e., cortical thickness and surface area) of 3- and 4-y-old children to their performance in novel nonverbal and traditional verbal TM tasks. We showed that verbal ToM reasoning was supported by cortical surface area and thickness of the precuneus and temporoparietal junction, classically involved in ToM in adults. Nonverbal ToM reasoning, in contrast, was supported by the cortical structure of a distinct and independent neural network including the supramarginal gyrus also involved in emotional and visual perspective taking, action observation, and social attention or encoding biases. This neural dissociation suggests two systems for reasoning about others’ minds—mature verbal ToM that emerges around 4 y of age, whereas nonverbal ToM tasks rely on different earlier-developing possibly social-cognitive processes.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Brain activity forecasts video engagement in an internet attention market
- Author
-
Brian Knutson, Lester Chun-pong Tong, M. Yavuz Acikalin, Alexander Genevsky, Baba Shiv, Department of Marketing Management, and Neuroeconomics
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Brain activity and meditation ,education ,Time allocation ,ComputingMethodologies_IMAGEPROCESSINGANDCOMPUTERVISION ,Video Recording ,Prefrontal Cortex ,forecasting ,Nucleus accumbens ,video ,Affect (psychology) ,insula ,Choice Behavior ,Nucleus Accumbens ,Humans ,Attention ,Nervous System Physiological Phenomena ,Prefrontal cortex ,Cerebral Cortex ,Internet ,Anterior insula ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Brain ,Biological Sciences ,accumbens ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,FMRI ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,The Internet ,Psychology ,business ,Social Media ,Insula ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance People currently spend over a billion of hours a day watching internet video content. To understand why, we combined neuroimaging with a behavioral video viewing task that simulated an internet attention market (i.e., youtube.com). While brain activity at video onset (increased nucleus accumbens [NAcc] and medial prefrontal cortex but decreased anterior insula [AIns]) predicted individuals’ choices to start and stop viewing, only activity in a subset of these regions implicated in anticipatory affect (increased NAcc and decreased AIns) at video onset forecasts aggregate video view frequency and duration on the internet. These findings suggest that brain activity can reveal “hidden” information capable of forecasting video engagement in attention markets., The growth of the internet has spawned new “attention markets,” in which people devote increasing amounts of time to consuming online content, but the neurobehavioral mechanisms that drive engagement in these markets have yet to be elucidated. We used functional MRI (FMRI) to examine whether individuals’ neural responses to videos could predict their choices to start and stop watching videos as well as whether group brain activity could forecast aggregate video view frequency and duration out of sample on the internet (i.e., on youtube.com). Brain activity during video onset predicted individual choice in several regions (i.e., increased activity in the nucleus accumbens [NAcc] and medial prefrontal cortex [MPFC] as well as decreased activity in the anterior insula [AIns]). Group activity during video onset in only a subset of these regions, however, forecasted both aggregate view frequency and duration (i.e., increased NAcc and decreased AIns)—and did so above and beyond conventional measures. These findings extend neuroforecasting theory and tools by revealing that activity in brain regions implicated in anticipatory affect at the onset of video viewing (but not initial choice) can forecast time allocation out of sample in an internet attention market.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Parent coaching increases conversational turns and advances infant language development
- Author
-
Naja Ferjan Ramirez, Sarah Roseberry Lytle, and Patricia K. Kuhl
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,parent coaching ,Psychological intervention ,Social Sciences ,Coaching ,law.invention ,Developmental psychology ,language intervention ,Child Development ,Randomized controlled trial ,law ,Intervention (counseling) ,Humans ,Speech ,Parent-Child Relations ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Intonation (linguistics) ,conversational turns ,Infant ,social interaction ,Language acquisition ,Social relation ,Language development ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,business ,Psychology ,parentese speech ,Child Language - Abstract
Significance How parents talk to infants is strongly associated with children’s language development, but many parents are not aware of this. We assigned families of 6-mo-old infants to a parent coaching intervention group or a no-coaching control group to determine whether specific parental language variables (“parentese”) can be enhanced through intervention. Naturalistic audio recordings were made in both groups at 6, 10, 14, and 18 mo of age. Parent coaching reviewed parents’ use of parentese from recordings and discussed the social engagement that promotes children’s language skills. Intervention increased parental use of parentese and parent-child turn-taking; both were correlated with children’s language growth and outcomes. Parental interventions targeting social components of language input have the potential to improve children’s language outcomes., Parental language input is one of the best predictors of children’s language achievement. Parentese, a near-universal speaking style distinguished by higher pitch, slower tempo, and exaggerated intonation, has been documented in speech directed toward young children in many countries. Previous research shows that the use of parentese and parent–child turn-taking are both associated with advances in children’s language learning. We conducted a randomized controlled trial to determine whether a parent coaching intervention delivered when the infants are 6, 10, and 14 mo of age can enhance parental language input and whether this, in turn, changes the trajectory of child language development between 6 and 18 mo of age. Families of typically developing 6-mo-old infants (n = 71) were randomly assigned to intervention and control groups. Naturalistic first-person audio recordings of the infants’ home language environment and vocalizations were recorded when the infants were 6, 10, 14, and 18 mo of age. After the 6-, 10-, and 14-mo recordings, intervention, but not control parents attended individual coaching appointments to receive linguistic feedback, listen to language input in their own recordings, and discuss age-appropriate activities that promote language growth. Intervention significantly enhanced parental use of parentese and parent–child turn-taking between 6 and 18 mo. Increases in both variables were significantly correlated with children’s language growth during the same period, and children’s language outcomes at 18 mo. Using parentese, a socially and linguistically enhanced speaking style, improves children’s social language turn-taking and language skills. Research-based interventions targeting social aspects of parent–child interactions can enhance language outcomes.
- Published
- 2020
39. Transformation of speech sequences in human sensorimotor circuits
- Author
-
Kathrin Müsch, Kean Ming Tan, Christopher J. Honey, Kevin D. Himberger, and Taufik A. Valiante
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Social Sciences ,Representation (arts) ,Semantics ,Premotor cortex ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,subvocal rehearsal ,medicine ,Humans ,Natural (music) ,030304 developmental biology ,media_common ,Cerebral Cortex ,0303 health sciences ,Multidisciplinary ,sentence repetition ,Contrast (music) ,Middle Aged ,Biological Sciences ,ECoG ,verbal short-term memory ,Memory, Short-Term ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Cerebral cortex ,Dynamics (music) ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Speech Perception ,Female ,Electrocorticography ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Sentence ,Neuroscience ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance When we hear a series of words, it is not only the auditory pathways of our brains that respond, but also the pathways that organize speech and action. What is the purpose of “motor” activity during language perception? We recorded electrical signals from the surfaces of the brain as people heard, mentally rehearsed, and spoke natural sentences. We found that human motor pathways generated precise, time-locked representations of sentences during listening. When listening was complete, the motor activity transformed to a distinct representation while people repeated the sentence in their heads and as they spoke the sentence aloud. Thus, motor circuits generate representations that can bridge from perception to short-term memory of spoken language., After we listen to a series of words, we can silently replay them in our mind. Does this mental replay involve a reactivation of our original perceptual dynamics? We recorded electrocorticographic (ECoG) activity across the lateral cerebral cortex as people heard and then mentally rehearsed spoken sentences. For each region, we tested whether silent rehearsal of sentences involved reactivation of sentence-specific representations established during perception or transformation to a distinct representation. In sensorimotor and premotor cortex, we observed reliable and temporally precise responses to speech; these patterns transformed to distinct sentence-specific representations during mental rehearsal. In contrast, we observed less reliable and less temporally precise responses in prefrontal and temporoparietal cortex; these higher-order representations, which were sensitive to sentence semantics, were shared across perception and rehearsal of the same sentence. The mental rehearsal of natural speech involves the transformation of stimulus-locked speech representations in sensorimotor and premotor cortex, combined with diffuse reactivation of higher-order semantic representations.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Universals of word order reflect optimization of grammars for efficient communication
- Author
-
Michael Hahn, Richard Futrell, and Dan Jurafsky
- Subjects
Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Social Sciences ,computer.software_genre ,Language Development ,Generalization, Psychological ,050105 experimental psychology ,computational linguistics ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cognition ,0302 clinical medicine ,Rule-based machine translation ,Clinical Research ,language universals ,Subject (grammar) ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Language ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Communication ,language processing ,05 social sciences ,Linguistics ,Ambiguity ,Problem of universals ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Neural Networks, Computer ,Artificial intelligence ,Computational linguistics ,business ,computer ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Linguistic universal ,Word (computer architecture) ,Natural language processing ,Word order - Abstract
Significance Human languages share many grammatical properties. We show that some of these properties can be explained by the need for languages to offer efficient communication between humans given our cognitive constraints. Grammars of languages seem to find a balance between two communicative pressures: to be simple enough to allow the speaker to easily produce sentences, but complex enough to be unambiguous to the hearer, and this balance explains well-known word-order generalizations across our sample of 51 varied languages. Our results offer quantitative and computational evidence that language structure is dynamically shaped by communicative and cognitive pressures., The universal properties of human languages have been the subject of intense study across the language sciences. We report computational and corpus evidence for the hypothesis that a prominent subset of these universal properties—those related to word order—result from a process of optimization for efficient communication among humans, trading off the need to reduce complexity with the need to reduce ambiguity. We formalize these two pressures with information-theoretic and neural-network models of complexity and ambiguity and simulate grammars with optimized word-order parameters on large-scale data from 51 languages. Evolution of grammars toward efficiency results in word-order patterns that predict a large subset of the major word-order correlations across languages.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Larger images are better remembered during naturalistic encoding
- Author
-
Shaimaa Masarwa, Olga Kreichman, and Sharon Gilaie-Dotan
- Subjects
vision ,Memory, Long-Term ,Multidisciplinary ,Brain ,Biological Sciences ,naturalistic ,size ,memory ,images ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Visual Perception ,Humans ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Significance It is unclear what makes some of the numerous visual scenes we encounter every day memorable (while others are not) when we make no intentional effort to memorize them. Here, we reasoned that although visual perception is somewhat size invariant (e.g., we can recognize a person from multiple distances), visual memory would depend on image size. Across experiments, where participants freely viewed images without any memory- or nonmemory-related task (similar to naturalistic visual behavior), larger images were remembered better than smaller ones (about 1.5 times better), and this effect was proportional to image size. Our study indicates that physical stimulus dimensions (as the size of an image) influence memory, and this may have significant implications to learning, aging, development, etc., We are constantly exposed to multiple visual scenes, and while freely viewing them without an intentional effort to memorize or encode them, only some are remembered. It has been suggested that image memory is influenced by multiple factors, such as depth of processing, familiarity, and visual category. However, this is typically investigated when people are instructed to perform a task (e.g., remember or make some judgment about the images), which may modulate processing at multiple levels and thus, may not generalize to naturalistic visual behavior. Visual memory is assumed to rely on high-level visual perception that shows a level of size invariance and therefore is not assumed to be highly dependent on image size. Here, we reasoned that during naturalistic vision, free of task-related modulations, bigger images stimulate more visual system processing resources (from retina to cortex) and would, therefore, be better remembered. In an extensive set of seven experiments, naïve participants (n = 182) were asked to freely view presented images (sized 3° to 24°) without any instructed encoding task. Afterward, they were given a surprise recognition test (midsized images, 50% already seen). Larger images were remembered better than smaller ones across all experiments (∼20% higher accuracy or ∼1.5 times better). Memory was proportional to image size, faces were better remembered, and outdoors the least. Results were robust even when controlling for image set, presentation order, screen resolution, image scaling at test, or the amount of information. While multiple factors affect image memory, our results suggest that low- to high-level processes may all contribute to image memory.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Fast response times signal social connection in conversation
- Author
-
Emma M. Templeton, Luke J. Chang, Elizabeth A. Reynolds, Marie D. Cone LeBeaumont, and Thalia Wheatley
- Subjects
Male ,Multidisciplinary ,Verbal Behavior ,Communication ,Emotions ,Social Interaction ,Social Sciences ,social connection ,Friends ,Young Adult ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,New Hampshire ,Female ,Interpersonal Relations ,response time ,turn taking ,conversation - Abstract
Significance Social connection is critical for our mental and physical health yet assessing and measuring connection has been challenging. Here, we demonstrate that a feature intrinsic to conversation itself—the speed with which people respond to each other—is a simple, robust, and sufficient metric of social connection. Strangers and friends feel more connected when their conversation partners respond quickly. Because extremely short response times (, Clicking is one of the most robust metaphors for social connection. But how do we know when two people "click"? We asked pairs of friends and strangers to talk with each other and rate their felt connection. For both friends and strangers, speed in response was a robust predictor of feeling connected. Conversations with faster response times felt more connected than conversations with slower response times, and within conversations, connected moments had faster response times than less-connected moments. This effect was determined primarily by partner responsivity: People felt more connected to the degree that their partner responded quickly to them rather than by how quickly they responded to their partner. The temporal scale of these effects (
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. No robust relation between larger cities and depression
- Author
-
Karoline B. S. Huth, Adam Finnemann, Maarten W. J. van den Ende, Peter M. A. Sloot, Psychology Other Research (FMG), Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Computational Science Lab (IVI, FNWI), Graduate School, and Public and occupational health
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,Ecology ,Socioeconomic Factors ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Sciences ,Letters ,Biological Sciences ,Cities - Abstract
“Larger cities provide a buffer against depression”—this astounding statement is from a PNAS article by Stier et al. (1) on how depression rates scale with the population of metropolitan statistical areas (MSAs). It is astounding as it runs contrary to a wealth of psychological and epidemiological research showing the complex nature of depression and the detrimental influence of cities (2⇓–4). This conflict with prior research makes it paramount to study the quality of the evidence. In this letter we carry out this task by considering the robustness of the finding. Our analysis suggests that their data do not support their conclusion.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Reply to Huth et al.: Cities are defined by their spatially aggregated socioeconomic networks
- Author
-
Andrew J. Stier, Kathryn E. Schertz, Nak Won Rim, Carlos Cardenas-Iniguez, Benjamin B. Lahey, Luís M. A. Bettencourt, and Marc G. Berman
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,Ecology ,Urban Population ,Depression ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Sciences ,Humans ,Letters ,Biological Sciences ,Cities - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Children across societies enforce conventional norms but in culturally variable ways
- Author
-
Patricia Kanngiesser, Marie Schäfer, Esther Herrmann, Henriette Zeidler, Daniel Haun, and Michael Tomasello
- Subjects
Male ,Multidisciplinary ,coordination ,Social Identification ,sanctions ,Social Sciences ,conventions ,Biological Sciences ,Child Development ,cross-cultural ,Child, Preschool ,Anthropology ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Norms ,Humans ,Female ,cross cultural ,Child ,100 Philosophie und Psychologie::150 Psychologie::150 Psychologie ,norms - Abstract
Significance Humans, as compared with other animals, create and follow conventional norms that determine how we greet each other, dress, or play certain games. Conventional norms are universal in all human societies, but it is an open question whether individuals in all societies also actively enforce conventional norms when others in their group break them. We show that 5- to 8-y-old children from eight highly diverse societies enforced conventional norms (i.e., game rules) when they observed a peer who apparently broke them. Magnitude and style of enforcement varied across societies. Third-party enforcement of conventional norms appears to be a human universal that is expressed in culturally variable ways., Individuals in all societies conform to their cultural group’s conventional norms, from how to dress on certain occasions to how to play certain games. It is an open question, however, whether individuals in all societies actively enforce the group’s conventional norms when others break them. We investigated third-party enforcement of conventional norms in 5- to 8-y-old children (n = 376) from eight diverse small-scale and large-scale societies. Children learned the rules for playing a new sorting game and then, observed a peer who was apparently breaking them. Across societies, observer children intervened frequently to correct their misguided peer (i.e., more frequently than when the peer was following the rules). However, both the magnitude and the style of interventions varied across societies. Detailed analyses of children’s interactions revealed societal differences in children’s verbal protest styles as well as in their use of actions, gestures, and nonverbal expressions to intervene. Observers’ interventions predicted whether their peer adopted the observer’s sorting rule. Enforcement of conventional norms appears to be an early emerging human universal that comes to be expressed in culturally variable ways.
- Published
- 2022
46. Unlocking adults’ implicit statistical learning by cognitive depletion
- Author
-
Eleonore H. M. Smalle, Tatsuya Daikoku, Arnaud Szmalec, Wouter Duyck, Riikka Möttönen, Mind and Matter, Department of Digital Humanities, Developmental Psychology, and UCL - SSH/IPSY - Psychological Sciences Research Institute
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,6162 Cognitive science ,TRANSCRANIAL MAGNETIC STIMULATION ,CORTEX ,Cognitive load ,Implicit learning ,Adolescent ,515 Psychology ,auditory statistical learning ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Social Sciences ,LANGUAGE ,Language Development ,MECHANISMS ,Young Adult ,Cognition ,Memory ,Learning ,Humans ,6121 Languages ,EEG ,Short-Term/physiology ,magnetic stimulation ,Cognition/physiology ,Auditory statistical learning ,Multidisciplinary ,cognitive load ,Memory, Short-Term/physiology ,Learning/physiology ,Linguistics ,Electroencephalography ,Biological Sciences ,Memory, Short-Term ,Prefrontal Cortex/growth & development ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Mental Recall ,Female ,MEMORY-SYSTEMS ,implicit learning ,transcranial ,electroencephalography - Abstract
Significance Statistical learning mechanisms enable extraction of patterns in the environment from infancy to adulthood. For example, they enable segmentation of continuous speech streams into novel words. Adults typically become aware of the hidden words even when passively listening to speech streams. It remains poorly understood how cognitive development and brain maturation affect implicit statistical learning (i.e., infant-like learning without awareness). Here, we show that the depletion of the cognitive control system by noninvasive brain stimulation or by demanding cognitive tasks boosts adults’ implicit but not explicit word-segmentation abilities. These findings suggest that the adult cognitive architecture constrains statistical learning mechanisms that are likely to contribute to early language acquisition and opens avenues to enhance language-learning abilities in adults., Human learning is supported by multiple neural mechanisms that maturate at different rates and interact in mostly cooperative but also sometimes competitive ways. We tested the hypothesis that mature cognitive mechanisms constrain implicit statistical learning mechanisms that contribute to early language acquisition. Specifically, we tested the prediction that depleting cognitive control mechanisms in adults enhances their implicit, auditory word-segmentation abilities. Young adults were exposed to continuous streams of syllables that repeated into hidden novel words while watching a silent film. Afterward, learning was measured in a forced-choice test that contrasted hidden words with nonwords. The participants also had to indicate whether they explicitly recalled the word or not in order to dissociate explicit versus implicit knowledge. We additionally measured electroencephalography during exposure to measure neural entrainment to the repeating words. Engagement of the cognitive mechanisms was manipulated by using two methods. In experiment 1 (n = 36), inhibitory theta-burst stimulation (TBS) was applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or to a control region. In experiment 2 (n = 60), participants performed a dual working-memory task that induced high or low levels of cognitive fatigue. In both experiments, cognitive depletion enhanced word recognition, especially when participants reported low confidence in remembering the words (i.e., when their knowledge was implicit). TBS additionally modulated neural entrainment to the words and syllables. These findings suggest that cognitive depletion improves the acquisition of linguistic knowledge in adults by unlocking implicit statistical learning mechanisms and support the hypothesis that adult language learning is antagonized by higher cognitive mechanisms.
- Published
- 2022
47. REPLY TO KOMATSU ET AL.: From local social mindfulness to global sustainability efforts?
- Author
-
Doesum, N., Murphy, R., Gallucci, M., Aharonov-Majar, E., Athenstaedt, U., Au, W., Bai, L., Böhm, R., Bovina, I., Buchan, N., Chen, X., Dumont, K., Engelmann, J., Eriksson, K., Euh, H., Fiedler, S., Friesen, J., Gächter, S., Garcia, C., González, R., Graf, S., Growiec, K., Guimond, S., Hřebíčková, M., Immer-Bernold, E., Joireman, J., Karagonlar, G., Kawakami, K., Kiyonari, T., Kou, Y., Kuhlman, D., Kyrtsis, A., Lay, S., Leonardelli, G., Li, N., Li, Y., Maciejovsky, B., Manesi, Z., Mashuri, A., Mok, A., Moser, K., Moták, L., Netedu, A., Pammi, C., Platow, M., Raczka-Winkler, K., Folmer, C., Reyna, C., Romano, A., Shalvi, S., Simão, C., Stivers, A., Strimling, P., Tsirbas, Y., Utz, S., van der Meij, L., Waldzus, S., Wang, Y., Weber, B., Weisel, O., Wildschut, T., Winter, F., Wu, J., Yong, J., Lange, P., Human Performance Management, Microeconomics (ASE, FEB), Faculteit Economie en Bedrijfskunde, and PSC (FdR)
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Sciences ,Letters ,Mindfulness - Abstract
Komatsu et al. (1) argue that Van Doesum et al. (2) may have overlooked the role of GDP in reporting a positive association between social mindfulness (SoMi) and the Environmental Performance Index (EPI) at country level. Although the relationship between EPI and SoMi is relatively weaker for countries with higher GDP, that does not imply that the overall observed relationship is a statistical artifact. Rather, it implies that GDP may be a moderator of the relationship between EPI and SoMi. The observed correlation is a valid result on average across countries, and the actual effect size would, at least to some degree, depend on GDP.
- Published
- 2022
48. Narratives imagined in response to instrumental music reveal culture-bounded intersubjectivity
- Author
-
Elizabeth H. Margulis, Patrick C. M. Wong, Cara Turnbull, Benjamin M. Kubit, and J. Devin McAuley
- Subjects
narrative ,Multidisciplinary ,Narration ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Culture ,Auditory Perception ,Imagination ,Humans ,Social Sciences ,music ,Semantics - Abstract
Significance Are we all imagining the same thing when we listen to music, or are our experiences hopelessly subjective? This research analyzes the similarity of responses from 622 participants in three locations on a highly unconstrained task: free-response descriptions of the stories they imagined while listening to instrumental music. Strikingly, participants in two separate locations that share an overarching culture imagine highly similar narratives to individual excerpts. But these similarity patterns do not extend to narratives imagined by participants in a third location with a distinct culture. This work shows that music—often considered an “abstract stimulus”—can trigger shared stories in listeners’ minds but that this intersubjectivity depends on a shared underlying culture., The scientific literature sometimes considers music an abstract stimulus, devoid of explicit meaning, and at other times considers it a universal language. Here, individuals in three geographically distinct locations spanning two cultures performed a highly unconstrained task: they provided free-response descriptions of stories they imagined while listening to instrumental music. Tools from natural language processing revealed that listeners provide highly similar stories to the same musical excerpts when they share an underlying culture, but when they do not, the generated stories show limited overlap. These results paint a more complex picture of music’s power: music can generate remarkably similar stories in listeners’ minds, but the degree to which these imagined narratives are shared depends on the degree to which culture is shared across listeners. Thus, music is neither an abstract stimulus nor a universal language but has semantic affordances shaped by culture, requiring more sustained attention from psychology.
- Published
- 2022
49. Using big data to track major shifts in human cognition
- Author
-
Simon DeDeo
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Commentary ,Social Sciences - Published
- 2022
50. Politicians polarize and experts depolarize public support for COVID-19 management policies across countries
- Author
-
Alexandra Flores, Jennifer C. Cole, Stephan Dickert, Kimin Eom, Gabriela M. Jiga-Boy, Tehila Kogut, Riley Loria, Marcus Mayorga, Eric J. Pedersen, Beatriz Pereira, Enrico Rubaltelli, David K. Sherman, Paul Slovic, Daniel Västfjäll, and Leaf Van Boven
- Subjects
Male ,polarization ,political polarization ,Multidisciplinary ,COVID-19 ,cross-country comparisons ,affective ,expertise ,Female ,Humans ,Health Policy ,Patient Acceptance of Health Care ,Political Activism ,SARS-CoV-2 ,Social Sciences ,Tvärvetenskapliga studier inom samhällsvetenskap ,affective polarization ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Social Sciences Interdisciplinary - Abstract
Significance Political polarization impeded public support for policies to address the spread of COVID-19, much as polarization hinders responses to other societal challenges. The present cross-country study demonstrates how the cues from political elites and affective polarization are analogous across countries addressing COVID-19. Far from being an outlier, the United States faces polarization challenges similar to those of other countries. Importantly, the results demonstrate that policies to combat public health crises are more supported when proposed by nonpartisan experts and bipartisan coalitions of political leaders. These results provide clear guidance on depolarizing communication strategies to improve global responses to health crises., Political polarization impeded public support for policies to reduce the spread of COVID-19, much as polarization hinders responses to other contemporary challenges. Unlike previous theory and research that focused on the United States, the present research examined the effects of political elite cues and affective polarization on support for policies to manage the COVID-19 pandemic in seven countries (n = 12,955): Brazil, Israel, Italy, South Korea, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States. Across countries, cues from political elites polarized public attitudes toward COVID-19 policies. Liberal and conservative respondents supported policies proposed by ingroup politicians and parties more than the same policies from outgroup politicians and parties. Respondents disliked, distrusted, and felt cold toward outgroup political elites, whereas they liked, trusted, and felt warm toward both ingroup political elites and nonpartisan experts. This affective polarization was correlated with policy support. These findings imply that policies from bipartisan coalitions and nonpartisan experts would be less polarizing, enjoying broader public support. Indeed, across countries, policies from bipartisan coalitions and experts were more widely supported. A follow-up experiment replicated these findings among US respondents considering international vaccine distribution policies. The polarizing effects of partisan elites and affective polarization emerged across nations that vary in cultures, ideologies, and political systems. Contrary to some propositions, the United States was not exceptionally polarized. Rather, these results suggest that polarizing processes emerged simply from categorizing people into political ingroups and outgroups. Political elites drive polarization globally, but nonpartisan experts can help resolve the conflicts that arise from it.
- Published
- 2022
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.